What is Gestalt Psychology? Theory, Principles, & Examples

Nathalia Bustamante

Harvard Graduate School of Education

Nathalia Bustamante is a Brazilian journalist at Harvard’s Graduate School of Education.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Saul McLeod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul McLeod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

On This Page:

Key Takeaways

  • Gestalt psychology is a school of thought that seeks to understand how the human brain perceives experiences. It suggests that structures, perceived as a whole, have specific properties that are different from the sum of their individual parts.
  • For instance, when reading a text, a person perceives each word and sentence as a whole with meaning, rather than seeing individual letters; and while each letterform is an independent individual unit, the greater meaning of the text depends on the arrangement of the letters into a specific configuration.
  • Gestalt grew from the field of psychology in the beginning of the 19th Century. Austrian and German psychologists started researching the human mind’s tendency to try to make sense of the world around us through automatic grouping and association.
  • The Gestalt Principles, or Laws of Perception, explain how this behavior of “pattern seeking” operates. They offer a powerful framework to understand human perception, and yet are simple to assimilate and implement.
  • For that reason, the Gestalt Laws are appealing not only to psychologists but also to visual artists, educators and communicators.

What Does Gestalt Mean?

In a loose translation, the German word ‘Gestalt’ (pronounced “ge-shtalt”) means ‘configuration’, or ‘structure’. It makes a reference to the way individual components are structured by our perception as a psychical whole (Wulf, 1996). That structure provides a scientific explanation for why changes in spacing, organization and timing can radically transform how information is received and assimilated.

How the Gestalt Approach Formed?

Two of the main philosophical influences of Gestalt are Kantian epistemology and Husserl’s phenomenological method .

Both Kant and Husserls sought to understand human consciousness and perceptions of the world, arguing that those mental processes are not entirely mediated by rational thought (Jorge, 2010).

Similarly, the Gestalt researchers Wertheimer, Koffka and Kohler observed that the human brain tends to automatically organize and interpret visual data through grouping.

They theorized that, because of those “mental shortcuts”, the perception of the whole is different from the sum of individual elements.

This idea that the whole is different from the sum of its parts – the central tenet of Gestalt psychology – challenged the then-prevailing theory of Structuralism .

This school of thought defended that mental processes should be broken down into their basic components, to focus on them individually.

Structuralists believed that complex perceptions could be understood by identifying the primitive sensations it caused – such as the points that make a square or particular pitches in a melody.

Gestalt, on the other hand, suggests the opposite path. It argues that the whole is grasped even before the brain perceives the individual parts – like when, looking at a photograph, we see the image of a face rather than a nose, two eyes, and the shape of a chin.

Therefore, to understand the subjective nature of human perception, we should transcend the specific parts to focus on the whole.

Gestalt Psychologists

Max wertheimer.

The inaugural article of Gestalt Psychology was Max Wertheimer’s Experimental Studies of the Perception of Movement , published in 1912.

Wertheimer, then at the Institute of Psychology in Frankfurt am Main, described a visual illusion called apparent motion in this article.

Apparent motion is the perception of movement that results from viewing a rapid sequence of static images, as happens in the movies or in flip books.

Wertheimer realized that the perception of the whole (the group of figures in a sequence) was radically different from the perception of its components (each static image).

Wolfgang Köhler

Wolfgang Köhler was particularly interested in physics and natural sciences. He introduced the concept of psychophysical isomorphism – arguing that how a stimulus is received is influenced by the brain’s general state while perceiving it (Shelvock, 2016).

He believed that organic processes tend to evolve to a state of equilibrium – like soap bubbles, that start in various shapes but always change into perfect spheres because that is their minimum energy state.

In the same way, the human brain would “converge” towards a minimum energy state through a process of simplifying perception – a mechanism that he called Pragnanz (Rock & Palmer, 1990).

Kurt Koffka

Koffka contributed to expanding Gestalt applications beyond visual perception. In his major article, Principles of Gestalt psychology (1935) he detailed the application of the Gestalt Laws to topics such as motor action, learning and memory, personality and society.

He also played a key role in taking the Gestalt Theory to the United States, to where he emigrated after the rise of Nazism in Germany.

Gestalt principles

Gestalt’s principles, or Laws of Perception, were formalized by Wertheimer in a treaty published in 1923, and further elaborated by Köhler, Koffka, and Metzger.

The principles are grounded on the human natural tendency of finding order in disorder – a process that happens in the brain, not in the sensory organs such as the eye. According to Wertheimer, the mind “makes sense” of stimulus captured by the eyes following a predictable set of principles.

The brain applies these principles to enable individuals to perceive uniform forms rather than simply collections of unconnected images.

Although these principles operate in a predictable way, they are actually mental shortcuts to interpreting information. As shortcuts, they sometimes make mistakes – and that is why they can lead to incorrect perceptions.

Gestalt’s principles

Prägnanz (law of simplicity)

  • The law of Prägnanz is also called “law of simplicity” or “law of good figure”. It states that when faced with a set of ambiguous or complex objects, the human brain seeks to make them as simple as possible.
  • The “good figure” is an object or image that can easily be perceived as a whole.
  • A good example of this process is our perception of the Olympic logo. We tend to see overlapping circles (the simpler version) rather than a series of curved, connected lines (Dresp-Langley, 2015).
  • This law suggests that we tend to group shapes, objects or design elements that share some similarity in terms of color, shape, orientation, texture or size.
  • The law of proximity states that shapes, objects or design elements located near each other tend to be perceived as a group.
  • Conversely, randomly located items tend to be perceived as isolated.
  • This principle can be applied to direct attention to key elements within a design: the closer visual elements are to each other, the more likely they will be perceived as related to each other, and too much negative space between elements serve to isolate them from one another.

Common Region

  • This law proposes that elements that are located within the same closed region – such as inside a circle or a shape – tend to be perceived as belonging to the same group.
  • Those clearly defined boundaries between the inside and the outside of a shape create a stronger connection between elements, and can even overpower the law of Proximity or of Similarity.
  • This law argues that shapes, objects or design elements that are positioned in a way that suggests lines, curves or planes will be perceived as such, and not as individual elements.
  • We perceptually group the elements together to form a continuous image.
  • This law suggests that the human brain has a natural tendency to visually close gaps in forms, particularly when identifying familiar images.
  • When information is missing, our focus goes to what is present and automatically “fills” the missing parts with familiar lines, colors or patterns.
  • Once a form has been identified, even if additional gaps are introduced, we still tend to visually complete the form, in order to make them stable.
  • IBM’s iconic logo is one example of applied closure – blue horizontal lines are arranged in three stacks that we “close” to form the letterforms (Graham 2008).

The classic gestalt principles have been extended in various directions. The ones above are some of the most commonly cited, but there are others, such as the symmetry principle (symmetrical components will tend to be grouped together) and the common faith principle (elements tend to be perceived as grouped together if they move together).

Applications of Gestalt

Gestalt Psychology and the Laws of Perception influenced research from a multitude of disciplines – including linguistic, design, architecture and visual communication.

Gestalt Therapy

Gestalt therapy was founded by Frederick (Fritz) and Laura Perls in the 1940s. It focuses on the phenomenological method of awareness that distinguishes perceptions, feelings and actions from their interpretations.

It believes that explanations and interpretations are less reliable than the concrete – what is directly perceived and felt. It is a therapy rooted in dialogue, in which patients and therapists discuss differences in perspectives (Yontef, G, 1993).

Design Professor and specialist Gregg Berryman pointed out, in his book Notes on Graphic Design and Visual Communication (1979), that ‘Gestalt perceptual factors build a visual frame of reference which can provide the designer with a reliable psychological basis for the spatial organization of graphic information’.

In essence, Gestalt provided a framework of understanding upon which designers can make decisions.

What made gestalt theory appealing to visual artists and designers is its attempt to explain “pattern seeking” in human behavior.

The Gestalt Laws provided scientific validation of compositional structure, and were used by designers in the mid-twentieth century to explain and improve visual work.

They are particularly useful in the creation of posters, magazines, logos and billboards in a meaningful and organized way. More recently, they have also been applied to the design of websites, user interfaces and digital experiences (Graham 2008).

Product Development

The product’s form and other perceptual attributes such as color and texture are crucial in influencing customer’s buying decisions.

Product development has adopted Gestalt Laws in approaches that consider how the target customer will perceive the final product.

By considering these perceptions, the product developer is better able to understand potential risks, ambiguities and meanings of the product he or she is working on (Cziulik & Santos 2012).

Education and Learning

In Education, Gestalt Theory was applied as a reaction to behaviorism, which reduced experiences to simple stimulus-response reflections.

Gestalt suggested that students should perceive the whole of the learning goal, and then discover the relations between parts and the whole. That meant that teachers should provide the basic framework of the lesson as an organized and meaningful structure, and then go into details.

That would help students to understand the relation between contents and the overall goal of the lesson.

Problem-based learning methodologies also arose based on Gestalt principles.

When students are exposed to the whole of a problem, they can “make sense” of it before engaging in introspective thinking to analyze the connection between elements and craft independent solutions (Çeliköz et al. 2019).

The Gestalt Principles are applied to the design of advertisement, packaging and even physical stores.

Researchers that investigated how consumers form overall impressions of consumption objects found that they usually integrate visual information with their own evaluation of specific features (Zimmer & Golden, 1988).

More recent applications also analyze how consumer perceptions apply to online shopping environments. The fundamental Gestalt Laws are thus applied to site architecture and visual impact (Demangeot, 2010).

Gestalt Legacy

Most psychologists consider that the Gestalt School, as a theoretical field of study, died with its founding fathers in the 1940s. Two main reasons may have contributed to that decline.

The first reason are institutional and personal constraints: after they left Germany, Wetheimer, Koffka and Köhler obtained positions in which they could conduct research, but could not train PhDs.

At the same time, most of the students and researchers that had remained in Germany broadened the scope of their research beyond Gestalt topics.

The second reason for the decline of Gestalt Psychology were empirical findings dismantling Köhler’s electrical field theory that sought to explain the brain’s functioning.

Neuroscience and cognitive science emerged in the 1960s as stronger frameworks for explaining the functioning of the brain.

Still, nearly all psychology students can expect to find at least one chapter dedicated to Gestalt Psychology in their textbooks.

Similarly, fundamental questions about the subjective nature of perception and awareness are still addressed in contemporary scientific research – with the perks of counting on advanced methods that were not available for the Gestaltists in the first half of the XX Century (Wagemans et al, 2012).

Berryman, G. (1979). Notes on Graphic Design and Visual Communication. Los Altos. William Kaufmann. Inc., t979.

Cziulik, C., & dos Santos, F. L. (2011). An approach to define formal requirements into product development according to Gestalt principles. Product: Management and Development, 9(2), 89-100.

Çeliköz, N., Erisen, Y., & Sahin, M. (2019). Cognitive Learning Theories with Emphasis on Latent Learning, Gestalt and Information Processing Theories. Online Submission, 9(3), 18-33.

Demangeot, C., & Broderick, A. J. (2010). Consumer perceptions of online shopping environments: A gestalt approach. Psychology & Marketing, 27(2), 117-140.

Dresp-Langley, B. (2015). Principles of perceptual grouping: Implications for image-guided surgery. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1565.

Graham, L. (2008). Gestalt theory in interactive media design. Journal of Humanities & Social Sciences, 2(1).

Jorge, MLM. (2010) Implicaciones epistemológicas de la noción de forma en la psicología de la Gestalt. Revista de Historia de la Psicología. vol. 31, núm. 4 (diciembre)

O”Connor, Z. (2015). Colour, contrast and gestalt theories of perception: The impact in contemporary visual communications design. Color Research & Application, 40(1), 85-92.

Rock, I., & Palmer, S. (1990). The legacy of Gestalt psychology . Scientific American, 263(6), 84-91.

Shelvock, M. T. (2016). Gestalt theory and mixing audio. Innovation in Music II, 1-14.

Wagemans, J., Elder, J. H., Kubovy, M., Palmer, S. E., Peterson, M. A., Singh, M., & von der Heydt, R. (2012). A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception : I. Perceptual grouping and figure–ground organization. Psychological bulletin, 138(6), 1172.

Yontef, G., & Simkin, J. (1993). Gestalt therapy: An introduction. Gestalt Journal Press.

Zimmer, M. R., & Golden, L. L. (1988). Impressions of retail stores: A content analysis of consume. Journal of retailing, 64(3), 265.

Further Information

Wagemans, J., Elder, J. H., Kubovy, M., Palmer, S. E., Peterson, M. A., Singh, M., & von der Heydt, R. (2012). A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: I. Perceptual grouping and figure–ground organization. Psychological bulletin, 138(6), 1172.

Raffagnino, R. (2019). Gestalt Therapy Effectiveness: A Systematic Review of Empirical Evidence. Open Journal of Social Sciences, 7(6), 66-83.

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

Information

  • Author Services

Initiatives

You are accessing a machine-readable page. In order to be human-readable, please install an RSS reader.

All articles published by MDPI are made immediately available worldwide under an open access license. No special permission is required to reuse all or part of the article published by MDPI, including figures and tables. For articles published under an open access Creative Common CC BY license, any part of the article may be reused without permission provided that the original article is clearly cited. For more information, please refer to https://www.mdpi.com/openaccess .

Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or approaches, provides an outlook for future research directions and describes possible research applications.

Feature papers are submitted upon individual invitation or recommendation by the scientific editors and must receive positive feedback from the reviewers.

Editor’s Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area. The aim is to provide a snapshot of some of the most exciting work published in the various research areas of the journal.

Original Submission Date Received: .

  • Active Journals
  • Find a Journal
  • Journal Proposal
  • Proceedings Series
  • For Authors
  • For Reviewers
  • For Editors
  • For Librarians
  • For Publishers
  • For Societies
  • For Conference Organizers
  • Open Access Policy
  • Institutional Open Access Program
  • Special Issues Guidelines
  • Editorial Process
  • Research and Publication Ethics
  • Article Processing Charges
  • Testimonials
  • Preprints.org
  • SciProfiles
  • Encyclopedia

jintelligence-logo

Article Menu

  • Subscribe SciFeed
  • Recommended Articles
  • PubMed/Medline
  • Google Scholar
  • on Google Scholar
  • Table of Contents

Find support for a specific problem in the support section of our website.

Please let us know what you think of our products and services.

Visit our dedicated information section to learn more about MDPI.

JSmol Viewer

Gestalt’s perspective on insight: a recap based on recent behavioral and neuroscientific evidence.

gestalt psychologists consider problem solving

1. Introduction

2. the role of perceptual experience in problem-solving cognition: was the parallelism between bistable figures and insight problem-solving warranted, 3. the holistic approach: what has recent research discovered about the idea that solutions to problems sometimes come to mind in an off-on manner.

Using Koffka ’s ( 1935, p. 176 ) words: “The whole is something else than the sum of its parts, because summing is a meaningless procedure, whereas the whole-part relationship is meaningful”. Similarly, insight problem-solving is processed in a discrete off–on manner, and when solutions to problems emerge, they do so as a “whole”, and the solver cannot retroactively report the reasoning process that led him or her to the solution.

4. The Gestalt Psychologists Assume That the Solution to Problems Comes “With Sudden Clarity.” Can We See in This Statement a Proto-Assumption That Insightful Solutions Might Be Characterized by a Perception of Higher Accuracy?

5. conclusions and future directions, author contributions, institutional review board statement, informed consent statement, data availability statement, conflicts of interest.

  • Becker, Maxi, Simone Kühn, and Tobias Sommer. 2021. Verbal insight revisited—Dissociable neurocognitive processes underlying solutions accompanied by an AHA! experience with and without prior restructuring. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 33: 659–84. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Beversdorf, D. Q., J. D. Hughes, B. A. Steinberg, L. D. Lewis, and K. M. Heilman. 1999. Noradrenergic modulation of cognitive flexibility in problem-solving. Neuroreport 10: 2763–67. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Bianchi, Ivana, Erika Branchini, Roberto Burro, Elena Capitani, and Ugo Savardi. 2020. Overtly prompting people to “think in opposites” supports insight problem-solving. Thinking & Reasoning 26: 31–67. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bijleveld, Erik, Ruud Custers, and Henk A. G. Aarts. 2009. The unconscious eye opener: Pupil dilation reveals strategic recruitment of resources upon presentation of subliminal reward cues. Psychological Science 20: 1313–15. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Bowden, Edward M., Mark Jung-Beeman, Jessica Fleck, and John Kounios. 2005. New approaches to demystifying insight. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 322–28. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Campbell, Heather L., Madalina E. Tivarus, Ashleigh Hillier, and David Q. Beversdorf. 2008. Increased task difficulty results in greater impact of noradrenergic modulation of cognitive flexibility. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior 88: 222–29. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Chapman, C. Richard, Shunichi Oka, David H. Bradshaw, Robert C. Jacobson, and Gary W. Donaldson. 1999. Phasic pupil dilation response to noxious stimulation in normal volunteers: Relationship to brain evoked potentials and pain report. Psychophysiology 36: 44–52. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Corbetta, Maurizio, Gaurav Patel, and Gordon L. Shulman. 2008. The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58: 306–24. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Coull, Jennifer T., Christian Büchel, Karl J. Friston, and Chris D. Frith. 1999. Noradrenergically mediated plasticity in a human attentional neuronal network. NeuroImage 10: 705–15. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Danek, Amory H. 2018. Magic tricks, sudden restructuring, and the Aha! experience: A new model of nonmonotonic problem-solving. In Insight . London: Routledge. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Danek, Amory H., and Carola Salvi. 2018. Moment of Truth: Why Aha! Experiences are Correct. The Journal of Creative Behavior 54: 484–86. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Danek, Amory H., and Jasmin M. Kizilirmak. 2021. The whole is more than the sum of its parts—Addressing insight problem solving concurrently from a cognitive and an affective perspective. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 33: 609–15. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Danek, Amory H., and Jennifer Wiley. 2017. What about False Insights? Deconstructing the Aha! Experience along Its Multiple Dimensions for Correct and Incorrect Solutions Separately. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 2077. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Danek, Amory H., Thomas Fraps, Albrecht von Müller, Benedikt Grothe, and Michael Öllinger. 2014. Working Wonders? Investigating insight with magic tricks. Cognition 130: 174–85. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • de Rooij, Alwin, Ruben D. Vromans, and Matthijs Dekker. 2018. Noradrenergic modulation of creativity: Evidence from pupillometry. Creativity Research Journal 30: 339–51. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Duncan, Seth, and Lisa Feldman Barrett. 2007. The role of the amygdala in visual awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11: 190–92. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Einhäuser, Wolfgang, James Stout, Christof Koch, and Olivia Carter. 2008. Pupil dilation reflects perceptual selection and predicts subsequent stability in perceptual rivalry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105: 1704–09. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Ellen, Paul. 1982. Direction, experience, and hints in creative problem solving: Reply to Weisberg and Alba. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 111: 316–25. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Feldman, Harriet, and Karl Friston. 2010. Attention, Uncertainty, and Free Energy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 4: 215. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Friston, Karl. 2009. The free-energy principle: A rough guide to the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13: 293–301. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Friston, Karl, Thomas FitzGerald, Francesco Rigoli, Philipp Schwartenbeck, and Giovanni Pezzulo. 2017. Active Inference: A Process Theory. Neural Computation 29: 1–49. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Friston, Karl, Thomas FitzGerald, Francesco Rigoli, Philipp Schwartenbeck, John O’Doherty, and Giovanni Pezzulo. 2016a. Active inference and learning. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 68: 862–79. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Friston, Karl, Vladimir Litvak, Ashwini Oswal, Adeel Razi, Klaas E. Stephan, Bernadette C. M. van Wijk, Gabriel Ziegler, and Peter Zeidman. 2016b. Bayesian model reduction and empirical Bayes for group (DCM) studies. Neuroimage 128: 413–31. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Haarsma, J., P. C. Fletcher, J. D. Griffin, H. J. Taverne, H. Ziauddeen, T. J. Spencer, C. Miller, T. Katthagen, I. Goodyer, K. M. J. Diederen, and et al. 2021. Precision weighting of cortical unsigned prediction error signals benefits learning, is mediated by dopamine, and is impaired in psychosis. Molecular Psychiatry 26: 9. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hedne, Mikael R., Elisabeth Norman, and Janet Metcalfe. 2016. Intuitive Feelings of Warmth and Confidence in Insight and Noninsight Problem Solving of Magic Tricks. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1314. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Jung-Beeman, Mark, Edward M. Bowden, Jason Haberman, Jennifer L. Frymiare, Stella Arambel-Liu, Richard Greenblatt, Paul J. Reber, and John Kounios. 2004. Neural activity when people solve verbal problems with insight. PLoS Biology 2: 500–10. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kietzmann, Tim C., Stephan Geuter, and Peter König. 2011. Overt Visual Attention as a Causal Factor of Perceptual Awareness. PLoS ONE 6: e22614. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Koch, Christof, and Naotsugu Tsuchiya. 2007. Attention and consciousness: Two distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11: 16–22. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Koffka, Kurt. 1935. Principles of Gestalt Psychology . Brace: Harcourt. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Köhler, Wolfgang. 1925. The Mentality of Apes . Brace: Harcourt, p. viii+, 342. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Korovkin, Sergei, Anna Savinova, Julia Padalka, and Anastasia Zhelezova. 2021. Beautiful mind: Grouping of actions into mental schemes leads to a full insight Aha! experience. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 33: 620–30. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kounios, John, and Mark Beeman. 2009. The Aha! Moment: The Cognitive Neuroscience of Insight. Current Directions in Psychological Science 18: 210–16. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kounios, John, and Mark Beeman. 2014. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Insight. Annual Review of Psychology 65: 71–93. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kounios, John, Jennifer L. Frymiare, Edward M. Bowden, Jessica I. Fleck, Karuna Subramaniam, Todd B. Parrish, and Mark Jung-Beeman. 2006. The prepared mind: Neural activity prior to problem presentation predicts subsequent solution by sudden insight. Psychological Science 17: 882–90. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Kounios, John, Jessica I. Fleck, Deborah L. Green, Lisa Payne, Jennifer L. Stevenson, Edward M. Bowden, and Mark Jung-Beeman. 2008. The Origins of Insight in Resting-State Brain Activity. Neuropsychologia 46: 281–91. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Laeng, Bruno, and Dinu-Stefan Teodorescu. 2002. Eye scanpath during visual imagery reenact those of perception of the same visual scene. Cognitive Science 26: 207–31. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Laukkonen, Ruben E., and Jason M. Tangen. 2017. Can observing a Necker cube make you more insightful? Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal 48: 198–211. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Laukkonen, Ruben E., Benjamin T. Kaveladze, Jason M. Tangen, and Jonathan W. Schooler. 2020. The dark side of Eureka: Artificially induced Aha moments make facts feel true. Cognition 196: 104122. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Laukkonen, Ruben E., Daniel J. Ingledew, Hilary J. Grimmer, Jonathan W. Schooler, and Jason M. Tangen. 2021. Getting a grip on insight: Real-time and embodied Aha experiences predict correct solutions. Cognition and Emotion 35: 918–35. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Laukkonen, Ruben E., Margaret Webb, Carola Salvi, Jason M. Tangen, Heleen A. Slagter, and Jonathan W. Schooler. 2023. Insight and the selection of ideas. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 153: 105363. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Maier, N. R. F. 1940. The behavior mechanisms concerned with problem solving. Psychological Review 47: 43–58. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Metcalfe, Janet. 1986. Feeling of knowing in memory and problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 12: 288–94. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Mungan, Esra. 2023. Gestalt theory: A revolution put on pause? Prospects for a paradigm shift in the psychological sciences. New Ideas in Psychology 71: 101036. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Oh, Yongtaek, Christine Chesebrough, Brian Erickson, Fengqing Zhang, and John Kounios. 2020. An insight-related neural reward signal. NeuroImage 214: 116757. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Salvi, Carola. 2023. Markers of Insight. Available online: https://osf.io/preprints/psyarxiv/73y5k (accessed on 15 September 2023).
  • Salvi, Carola, and Edward M. Bowden. 2016. Looking for creativity: Where do we look when we look for new ideas? Frontiers in Psychology 7: 161. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Salvi, Carola, Claudio Simoncini, Jordan Grafman, and Mark Beeman. 2020. Oculometric signature of switch into awareness? Pupil size predicts sudden insight whereas microsaccades predict problem-solving via analysis. NeuroImage 217: 116933. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Salvi, Carola, Emanuela Bricolo, John Kounios, Edward Bowden, and Mark Beeman. 2016. Insight solutions are correct more often than analytic solutions. Thinking & Reasoning 22: 443–60. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Salvi, Carola, Emanuela Bricolo, Steven L. Franconeri, John Kounios, and Mark Beeman. 2015. Sudden insight is associated with shutting out visual inputs. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 22: 1814–19. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Salvi, Carola, Emily K. Leiker, Beatrix Baricca, Maria A. Molinari, Roberto Eleopra, Paolo F. Nichelli, Jordan Grafman, and Joseph E. Dunsmoor. 2021. The Effect of Dopaminergic Replacement Therapy on Creative Thinking and Insight Problem-Solving in Parkinson’s Disease Patients. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 646448. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Sara, Susan J. 2009. The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 10: 211–23. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Sara, Susan J., and Sebastien Bouret. 2012. Orienting and reorienting: The locus coeruleus mediates cognition through arousal. Neuron 76: 130–41. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Smith, Roderick W., and John Kounios. 1996. Sudden insight: All-or-none processing revealed by speed-accuracy decomposition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22: 1443–62. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Thorndike, Edward L. 1911. Animal Intelligence: Experimental Studies . New York: Macmillan. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tik, Martin, Ronald Sladky, Caroline Di Bernardi Luft, David Willinger, André Hoffmann, Michael J. Banissy, Joydeep Bhattacharya, and Christian Windischberger. 2018. Ultra-high-field fMRI insights on insight: Neural correlates of the Aha!-moment. Human Brain Mapping 39: 3241–52. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Webb, Margaret E., Daniel R. Little, and Simon J. Cropper. 2016. Insight Is Not in the Problem: Investigating Insight in Problem Solving across Task Types. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1424. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Weisberg, Robert. 1986. Creativity: Genius and Other Myths . New York: W H Freeman/Times Books/Henry Holt & Co., p. 169. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weisberg, Robert W., and Joseph W. Alba. 1981. An examination of the alleged role of “fixation” in the solution of several “insight” problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 110: 169–92. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Weisberg. 2018. Problem solving. In The Routledge International Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning . Edited by L. J. Ball and V. A. Thompson. Routledge: Taylor & Francis Group, pp. 607–23. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wertheimer, Max. 1923. Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt. II. Psychologische Forschung 4: 301–50. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Wertheimer, Max. 1959. Productive Thinking . New York: Harper, p. xvi, 302. [ Google Scholar ]
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

Vitello, M.; Salvi, C. Gestalt’s Perspective on Insight: A Recap Based on Recent Behavioral and Neuroscientific Evidence. J. Intell. 2023 , 11 , 224. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11120224

Vitello M, Salvi C. Gestalt’s Perspective on Insight: A Recap Based on Recent Behavioral and Neuroscientific Evidence. Journal of Intelligence . 2023; 11(12):224. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11120224

Vitello, Mary, and Carola Salvi. 2023. "Gestalt’s Perspective on Insight: A Recap Based on Recent Behavioral and Neuroscientific Evidence" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 12: 224. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11120224

Article Metrics

Article access statistics, further information, mdpi initiatives, follow mdpi.

MDPI

Subscribe to receive issue release notifications and newsletters from MDPI journals

  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Sweepstakes
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2024 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

Gestalt Psychology

Seeing the Mind and Behavior as a Whole

Gestalt psychology is a  school of thought  that looks at the human mind and behavior as a whole. Gestalt psychology suggests that humans don't focus on separate components but instead tend to perceive objects as elements of more complex systems.

Emily Roberts / Verywell

A core belief in Gestalt psychology is holism —that the whole is greater than the sum of its parts. The approach has played a major role in the study of human sensation and perception .

Gestalt Meaning

Gestalt is a German word that roughly means "configuration" or the way things are put together to form a whole.

The History of Gestalt Psychology

Originating in the work of Max Wertheimer , Gestalt psychology formed in part as a response to the structuralism of  Wilhelm Wundt .

Whereas followers of structuralism were interested in breaking psychological matters down into their smallest possible parts, Gestalt psychologists wanted to look at the totality of the mind and behavior. Guided by the principle of holism, Wertheimer and his followers identified instances where perception was based on seeing things as a complete whole, not as separate components.

A number of thinkers influenced the development of Gestalt psychology, including Immanuel Kant, Ernst Mach, and Johann Wolfgang von Goethe.

Wertheimer developed Gestalt psychology after observing what he called the phi phenomenon while watching alternating lights on a railway signal. The phi phenomenon is an optical illusion where two stationary objects seem to move if they are shown appearing and disappearing in rapid succession. In other words, we perceive movement where there is none.

Based on his observations of the phi phenomenon, Wertheimer concluded that we perceive things by seeing the whole perception, not by understanding individual parts. In the example of blinking lights at a train station, the whole we perceive is that one light appears to move quickly between two points. The reality is that two separate lights are blinking rapidly without moving at all.

Influential Gestalt Psychologists

Wertheimer's observations of the phi phenomenon are widely credited as the beginning of Gestalt psychology and he went on to publicize the core principles of the field. Other psychologists also had an influence on this school of psychology.

Wolfgang Köhler : Köhler connected Gestalt psychology to the natural sciences, arguing that organic phenomena are examples of holism at work. He also studied hearing and looked at problem-solving abilities in chimpanzees.

Kurt Koffka : Together with Wertheimer and Köhler, Koffka is considered a founder of the field. He applied the concept of Gestalt to child psychology , arguing that infants first understand things holistically before learning to differentiate them into parts. Koffka played a key role in bringing Gestalt principles to the United States.

Principles of Gestalt Psychology

Gestalt psychology helped introduce the idea that human perception is not just about seeing what is actually present in the world around us. It is also heavily influenced by our motivations and expectations .

Wertheimer created principles to explain how Gestalt perception functions. Some of the most important principles of Gestalt theory are:

  • Prägnanz : This foundational principle states that we naturally perceive things in their simplest form or organization.
  • Similarity : This Gestalt principle suggests that we naturally group similar items together based on elements like color, size, and orientation. An example would be grouping dogs based on whether they are small or large, or if they are big or small.
  • Proximity : The principle of proximity states that objects near each other tend to be viewed as a group.
  • Continuity : According to this Gestalt principle, we perceive elements arranged on a line or curve as related to each other, while elements that are not on the line or curve are seen as separate.
  • Closure : This suggests that elements that form a closed object will be perceived as a group. We will even fill in missing information to create closure and make sense of an object. An example of this Gestalt psychology principle is using negative space to give the illusion that a particular shape exists when it doesn't.
  • Common region : This Gestalt psychology principle states that we tend to group objects together if they're located in the same bounded area. (For example, objects inside a box tend to be considered a group.)

Uses of Gestalt Psychology

Gestalt psychology is useful in many areas, including therapy, design, product development, and learning.

Gestalt Therapy

Gestalt therapy is based on the idea that overall perception depends on the interaction between many factors. Among these factors are our past experiences, current environment, thoughts, feelings, and needs. Gestalt therapy involves key concepts such as awareness , unfinished business, and personal responsibility.

The main goal of Gestalt therapy is to help us focus on the present . While past context is important for viewing yourself as a whole, a Gestalt therapist will encourage you to keep your focus on your present experience.

Research suggests that Gestalt therapy is effective at treating symptoms of depression and anxiety , and it may help people gain confidence and increase feelings of self-efficacy and self-kindness. It is often a helpful way to structure group therapy .

The therapeutic process is reliant on the relationship between the client and therapist . As a client, you must feel comfortable enough to develop a close partnership with your therapist, and they must be able to create an unbiased environment where you can discuss your thoughts and experiences.

Beginning in the 1920s, designers began incorporating Gestalt principles in their work. Gestalt psychology led these designers to believe that we all share certain characteristics in the way we perceive visual objects and that we all have a natural ability to see "good" design.

Designers embraced Gestalt concepts, using our perception of contrast, color, symmetry, repetition, and proportion to create their work. Gestalt psychology influenced other design concepts, such as:

  • Figure-ground relationship : This describes the contrast between a focal object (like a word, phrase, or image) and the negative space around it. Designers often use this to create impact.
  • Visual hierarchy : Designers use the way we perceive and group visual objects to establish a visual hierarchy, ensuring that the most important word or image attracts our attention first.
  • Associativity : This concept involves the principle of proximity. Designers often use this to determine where to place important objects, including text elements such as headlines, captions, and lists.

Product Development

Product designers use Gestalt psychology to inform their decisions during the development process. Consumers tend to like products that follow Gestalt principles.

This influence can be seen in the appearance of the products themselves and in their packaging and advertising. We can also see Gestalt principles at work in apps and digital products. Concepts like proximity, similarity, and continuity have become standards of our expected user experience.

Learning and Education

The Gestalt Theory of Learning relies on the law of simplicity. In simple terms, it states that each learning stimulus is perceived in its simplest form.

The psychology behind this learning theory states that we use our senses and previous experiences to gain knowledge about the world around us. It also suggests that we learn from the methods by which we are taught, in addition to being impacted by classroom environments and the academic culture.

Impact of Gestalt Psychology

Gestalt psychology has largely been subsumed by other types of psychology, but it had an enormous influence on the field. Researchers like Kurt Lewin and Kurt Goldstein were influenced by Gestalt concepts before going on to make important contributions to psychology.

Gestalt theory is also important in that the idea of the whole being different than its parts has influenced our understanding of the brain and social behavior. Gestalt theory still impacts how we understand vision and the ways that context, visual illusions, and information processing impact our perception.

The Takeaway

Gestalt therapy continues as an effective tool for psychologists today. Its emphasis on a holistic approach plays an important role in cognitive psychology , perception, and social psychology , among other fields.

Frequently Asked Questions

Gestalt psychology was founded by Max Wertheimer, a Czechoslovakian psychologist who also developed a lie detection device to objectively study courtroom testimony. ]Wolfgang Köhler and Kurt Koffka are also considered co-founders of the Gestalt theory.

Most of the foundational principles of Gestalt psychology explain how we group things, such as by similarity, proximity, continuity, closure, and common reason. Prägnanz is another Gestalt principle and says that we tend to perceive complex things in their most simple form. Prägnanz is sometimes referred to as the law of simplicity, a concept that was first presented in 1914.

Gestalt psychology has influenced how we study perception and sensation. It also increases our understanding of how our cognitive processes influence the way we behave socially.

Some therapists use Gestalt psychology to help patients focus on the present over the past. Designers and product developers also use Gestalt theory to make their creations more appealing or to draw focus to certain elements over others. Educators may also use Gestalt principles to help their students learn.

Jäkel F, Singh M, Wichmann FA, Herzog MH. An overview of quantitative approaches in Gestalt perception . Vis Res . 2016;126:3-8. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2016.06.004

Wagemans J, Elder JH, Kubovy M, et al. A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: I. Perceptual grouping and figure-ground organization . Psychol Bull . 2012;138(6):1172-1217. doi:10.1037/a0029333

Bonacchi S, Czerniak S. Introduction: Gestalt as structure principles in science, art and language . Dialogue Universal. 2015;25(4):7-9. doi:10.5840/du201525480

Gundlach H. Max Wertheimer, Habilitation candidate at the Frankfurt Psychological Institute . History Psychol . 2014;17(2):134-48. doi:10.1037/a0036159

Wagemans J. Historical and conceptual background: Gestalt theory . In: Wagemans J, ed., The Oxford Handbook of Perceptual Organization . Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199686858.013.026

Wagemans J, Feldman J, Gepshtein S, et al. A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception II. Conceptual and theoretical foundations . Psychol Bull . 2012;138(6):1218-1252. doi:10.1037/a0029334

O'Leary E. Key concepts of Gestalt therapy and processing . Gestalt Therapy Around the World . doi:10.1002/9781118323410.ch2

Raffagnino R. Gestalt therapy effectiveness: A systematic review of empirical evidence .  Open J Soc Sci . 2019;7(6):66-83. doi:10.4236/jss.2019.76005

Moszkowicz J. Gestalt and graphic design: An exploration of the humanistic and therapeutic effects of visual organization .  Des Issues . 2011;27(4):56-67. doi:10.1162/DESI_a_00105

O’Connor Z. Colour, contrast and Gestalt theories of perception: The impact in contemporary visual communications design .  Color Res Appl . 2015;40(1):85-92. doi:10.1002/col.21858

Valencia-Romero A, Lugo J. An immersive virtual discrete choice experiment for elicitation of product aesthetics using Gestalt principles . Design Sci . 2017;3:E11. doi:10.1017/dsj.2017.12

Liang Y. Application of Gestalt psychology in product human-machine interface design . IOP Conf Series: Materials Sci Engineer . 2018;392:062054. doi:10.1088/1757-899X/392/6/062054

Lee C. Instructional design models and theories: The Gestalt psychology of learning . Arizona State University.

Britannica. Max Wertheimer: Czech psychologist .

Koenderink J, van Doorn A, Pinna B. Measures of Prägnanz? Gestalt Theory . 2018;40(1):7-28. doi:10.2478/gth-2018-0002

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

A Gestalt Perspective on the Psychology of Thinking

  • First Online: 11 November 2009

Cite this chapter

gestalt psychologists consider problem solving

  • Michael Wertheimer 4  

Part of the book series: On Thinking ((ONTHINKING))

2767 Accesses

3 Citations

Gestalt theory, one of the major “schools” of psychology during the first half of the twentieth century, recently returned to prominence because of the enormous relevance to current research in cognitive science and other areas. Core concepts in Gestalt theory are dynamic self-distribution, structure, relational determination, organization, Prägnanz, reorganization, insight, and understanding. The most basic principle of Gestalt theory is that most wholes in nature are not merely the sums of their constituent elements, nor just more than the sums of their parts, but qualitatively entirely different from some additive product. Gestalten are dynamic structures the qualities and nature of which determine the place, role, and function of their constituent parts. Several examples illustrate how productive human thinking involves transforming a confused, opaque, incomprehensible problem situation into a clear, clean Gestalt or organization which makes sense, is coherent, and generates insight about the genuine nature of the problem structure and its solution.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

gestalt psychologists consider problem solving

Gestalt/Gestalt Theory

gestalt psychologists consider problem solving

Meta-Intelligence: Understanding, Control, and Coordination of Higher Cognitive Processes

gestalt psychologists consider problem solving

Gestalt, Equivalency, and Functional Dependency: Kurt Grelling’s Formal Ontology

Arnheim R (1954) Art and visual perception. University of California Press, Berkeley, CA

Google Scholar  

Asch SE (1952) Social psychology. Prentice-Hall, New York

Book   Google Scholar  

Ash MG (1995) Gestalt psychology in German culture, 1890-1967: holism and the quest for objectivity. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England

Duncker K (1945) On problem solving. In Psychological Monographs 58, Whole No. 270 (Original German edition 191)

Katona G (1940) Organizing and memorizing. Columbia University Press, New York

King DB, Wertheimer M (2005) Max Wertheimer and Gestalt theory. Transaction Publishers, New Brunswick, NJ

Koffka K (1922) Perception: an introduction to the Gestalt-Theorie. Psychological Bulletin 19:531-585

Article   Google Scholar  

Koffka K (1935) Principles of Gestalt psychology. Harcourt, Brace, New York

Köhler W (1920) Die physischen Gestalten in Ruhe und im stationären Zustand. Vieweg, Braunschweig, Germany

Köhler W (1929) Gestalt psychology. Liveright, New York

Köhler W (1969) The task of Gestalt psychology. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ

Kölher W (1925) The mentality of apes. Harcourt, New York, Brace Original German edition, 1917

Lewin K (1935) A dynamic theory of personality. McGraw-Hill, New York

Lewin K (1948) Resolving social conflicts. Harper, New York

Luchins AS (1942) Mechanization in problem solving: the effect of Einstellung. In: Psychological Monographs 54: Whole No. 248

Spillmann L (1999) Gehirn und Gestalt. Psychologische Beiträge 41:458-493

Spillmann L (2001) Gehirn und Gestalt: II Neuronale Mechanismen. Kognitionswissenschaft 9:122-143

Von Ehrenfels C (1890) Über Gestaltqualiäten. Vierteljahresschrift für wissenschaftliche Philosophie 14:249-292

Wertheimer M (1910) Musik der Wedda. Sammelbände der internationalen Musikgesellschaft 11:300-309

Wertheimer M (1912a) Über das Denken der Naturvölker: I Zahlen und Zahlgebilde. Zeitschrift für Psychologie 60:321-378

Wertheimer M (1912b) Experimentelle Untersuchungen über das Sehen von Bewegung. Zeitschrift für Psychologie 61:161-265

Wertheimer M (1920) Über Schlussprozesse im produktiven Denken. De Gruyter, Berlin, Germany

Wertheimer M (1922) Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt: I Prinzipielle Bemerkungen. Psychologische Forschung 1:47-58

Wertheimer M (1923) Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt: II. Psychologische Forschung 4:301-350

Wertheimer M (1934) On truth. Soc Res 1:135-146

Wertheimer M (1935) Some problems in the theory of ethics. Soc Res 2:353-367

Wertheimer M (1937) On the concept of democracy. In: Ascoli M, Lehmann F (eds) Political and economic democracy. Norton, New York, pp 271-283

Wertheimer M (1940) A story of three days. In: Anshen RN (ed) Freedom: its meaning. Harcourt, Brace, New York, pp 555-569

Wertheimer M (1945) Productive thinking. Harper, New York Multiple translations, and enlarged edition 1959, 1982

Wertheimer M (1980) Gestalt theory of learning. In: Gazda GM, Corsini RJ (eds) Theories of learning: a comparative approach. Peacock, Itasca, IL, pp 208-251

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Psychology, University of Colorado, Muenzinger Psychology Building, 345, Boulder, 80309, CO, USA

Michael Wertheimer

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael Wertheimer .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

Parmenides Foundation, Kardinal Faulhaber Str. 14 a, Munich, 80333, Germany

Britt Glatzeder

Dept. Psychology, York University, Keele Street 4700, Toronto, M3J 1P3, Canada

Albrecht Müller

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2010 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Wertheimer, M. (2010). A Gestalt Perspective on the Psychology of Thinking. In: Glatzeder, B., Goel, V., Müller, A. (eds) Towards a Theory of Thinking. On Thinking. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-03129-8_4

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-03129-8_4

Published : 11 November 2009

Publisher Name : Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

Print ISBN : 978-3-642-03128-1

Online ISBN : 978-3-642-03129-8

eBook Packages : Biomedical and Life Sciences Biomedical and Life Sciences (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

Loading to Gestalt Psychologists Consider Problem Solving As A Process Involving(3)....

You are now being redirected to goldpdf.site....

  • Mathematics Education
  • Problem Solving

Gestalt Research on Problem-Solving and Today's Gestalt

  • August 2021

Esra Mungan at Bogazici University

  • Bogazici University

Köhler, 1921; arrow and metric information added by author)

Discover the world's research

  • 25+ million members
  • 160+ million publication pages
  • 2.3+ billion citations

Esra Mungan

  • PATTERN ANAL APPL

Markus Hörhan

  • Katherine R. Storrs

Erich H. Witte

  • Max Wertheimer

John E. Richters

  • Angelos Amanatiadis

Vassilis Kaburlasos

  • E. B. Kosmatopoulos
  • Wolfgang Köhler
  • Recruit researchers
  • Join for free
  • Login Email Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google Welcome back! Please log in. Email · Hint Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google No account? Sign up

Loading to Gestalt Psychologists Consider Problem Solving As A Process Involving....

Gestalt Psychologists Consider Problem Solving as a Process Involving

gestalt psychologists consider problem solving

Gestalt psychologists consider problem solving as a process involving

A) reorganization or restructuring. B) multiple goal states. C) sensory operators. D) continuity and form.

Correct Answer:

Unlock this answer now Get Access to more Verified Answers free of charge

Q1: Newell and Simon were early pioneers in

Q2: The typical purpose of subgoals is to A)

Q3: The elements of the problem space include

Q4: In the two-string problem, tying the pliers

Q5: Holly was in her mother-in-law's kitchen preparing

Q7: Which of the following provides the best

Q8: Which of the following is not part

Q9: Illustrative of functional fixedness, people are more

Q10: The circle problem, in which the task

Q11: Janet is alone in a room that

Unlock this Answer For Free Now!

View this answer and more for free by performing one of the following actions

qr-code

Scan the QR code to install the App and get 2 free unlocks

upload documents

Unlock quizzes for free by uploading documents

COMMENTS

  1. What is Gestalt Psychology: Theory & Principles

    Key Takeaways. Gestalt psychology is a school of thought that seeks to understand how the human brain perceives experiences. It suggests that structures, perceived as a whole, have specific properties that are different from the sum of their individual parts. For instance, when reading a text, a person perceives each word and sentence as a ...

  2. Gestalt’s Perspective on Insight: A Recap Based on Recent ...

    The Gestalt psychologists' theory of insight problem-solving was based on a direct parallelism between perceptual experience and higher-order forms of cognition (e.g., problem-solving). Similarly, albeit not exclusively, to the sudden recognition of bistable figures, these psychologists contended that problem-solving involves a restructuring of one's initial representation of the problem ...

  3. Gestalt Psychology

    Gestalt psychology is a school of thought that looks at the human mind and behavior as a whole. Gestalt psychology suggests that humans don't focus on separate components but instead tend to perceive objects as elements of more complex systems. A core belief in Gestalt psychology is holism —that the whole is greater than the sum of its parts.

  4. (PDF) Gestalt's Perspective on Insight: A Recap Based on Recent

    The Gestalt psychologists' theory of insight problem-solving was based on a direct parallelism between perceptual experience and higher-order forms of cognition (e.g., problem-solving).

  5. Gestalt psychology

    Gestalt psychology, gestaltism, or configurationism is a school of psychology and a theory of perception that emphasises the processing of entire patterns and configurations, and not merely individual components. It emerged in the early twentieth century in Austria and Germany as a rejection of basic principles of Wilhelm Wundt's and Edward Titchener's elementalist and structuralist psychology.

  6. A Gestalt Perspective on the Psychology of Thinking

    Gestalt theory, one of several "schools" of psychology that flourished during the first half of the twentieth century, recently became prominent again because of its relevance to current research issues in fields as diverse as cognitive neuroscience, perception, visual neuroscience, the psychology of art, social psychology, the study of personality - and problem solving and thinking.

  7. Gestalt Psychologists Consider Problem Solving As A Process Involving

    Gestalt Psychologists Consider Problem Solving As A Process Involving Thinking in Perspective Andrew Burton 2022-12-15 Originally published in 1978, the main task of this book was to consider the psychology of thinking in relation to the various perspectives from which thought processes were studied at the time. It provided an up-to-date and ...

  8. Gestalt Psychologists Consider Problem Solving As A Process Involving

    Gestalt Psychologists Consider Problem Solving As A Process Involving Creative Cognition Ronald A. Finke 1996-01-05 Creative Cognition combines original experiments with existing work in cognitive psychology to provide the first explicit account of the cognitive processes and structures that contribute to creative thinking and discovery.

  9. Gestalt psychology

    The word Gestalt is used in modern German to mean the way a thing has been "placed," or "put together.". There is no exact equivalent in English. "Form" and "shape" are the usual translations; in psychology the word is often interpreted as "pattern" or "configuration.". Gestalt theory originated in Austria and Germany as ...

  10. PDF Gestalt Psychologists Consider Problem Solving As A Process Involving

    through problem solving will also make it of relevance to educationalists and business psychologists. On Problem-solving Karl Duncker,1972-04-24 A scholarly study of the elements which make up the process of problem solving. The Task of Gestalt Psychology Wolfgang Kohler,2015-03-08 Contents: Wölfgang Kohler (1887-1967), by Carroll C. Pratt. I ...

  11. Chapter 12 Flashcards

    Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like Gestalt psychologists consider problem solving as a process involving a. sensory operators. b. multiple goal states. c. reorganization or restructuring. d. continuity and form., Gentner and Goldinmeadow (2003) illustrated that analogical encoding causes problem solvers to pay attention to _____ features that _____ their ability ...

  12. PDF Gestalt Psychologists Consider Problem Solving As A Process Involving (PDF)

    through problem solving will also make it of relevance to educationalists and business psychologists. On Problem-solving Karl Duncker,1972-04-24 A scholarly study of the elements which make up the process of problem solving. The Task of Gestalt Psychology Wolfgang Kohler,2015-03-08 Contents: Wölfgang Kohler (1887-1967), by Carroll C. Pratt. I ...

  13. Problem Solving

    In this theory, people solve problems by searching in a problem space. The problem space consists of the initial (current) state, the goal state, and all possible states in between. The actions that people take in order to move from one state to another are known as operators. Consider the eight puzzle. The problem space for the eight puzzle ...

  14. Gestalt Research on Problem-Solving and Today's Gestalt

    Abstract. This article will focus on two pioneering scientific works in problem-solving, or per Gestalt theory, "productive thinking". One of them is Köhler's research on goal-directed tool ...

  15. UCF EXP 3604c CH 12 & 13 Flashcards

    Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like Gestalt psychologists consider problem solving as a process involving, Which of the following provides the best example of functional fixedness?, The water-jug problem demonstrates that one consequence of having a procedure that does provide a solution to a problem is that, if well-learned, it may prevent us from and more.

  16. PDF Gestalt Psychologists Consider Problem Solving As A Process Involving

    Gestalt Psychologists Consider Problem Solving As A Process Involving Problem Solving S. Ian Robertson,2003-09-02 Problem solving is an integral part of everyday life yet few books are dedicated to this important aspect of human cognition In each case the problem such as solving a crossword or writing an essay has a goal In this

  17. Cognitive Psych Ch. 12 (Problem Solving) Book Questions

    Demonstrate a difference between how people solve insight and non-insight problems. Warmth judgments on nearness to a solution ______ prior to the solution of an insight problem and ______ prior to the solution of a non-insight problem. Rise suddenly just; gradually rise. Functional fixedness would be LOWEST for a (n)...

  18. Chapter 12 Questions Flashcards

    A single correct answer. Gestalt psychologists consider problem solving as a process involving. a. restructuring. b. multiple goal states. c. sensory operators. d. continuity and form. Restructuring. The circle problem, in which the task is to determine the length of a line inside a circle, was proposed to illustrate.

  19. Gestalt psychologists consider problem solving as a process ...

    Gestalt psychologists consider problem solving as a process involving A) reorganization or restructuring. B) multiple goal states. C) sensory operators. D) continuity and form.

  20. exam 4 practice quiz questions Flashcards

    Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like Gestalt psychologists consider problem solving as a process involving A) reorganization or restructuring. B) multiple goal states. C) sensory operators. D) continuity and form, The analogical paradox refers to problem-solving differences between A) experts and novices. B) laboratory and real-world settings. C) experimental groups ...

  21. Psych 258 Chapter 12 Flashcards

    c. the initial state is not clearly defined. d. the goal state is not clearly defined., Ill-defined problems are so named because it is difficult to specify _____ for the problems. a. analogies b. initial states c. a single correct answer d. schemas, Gestalt psychologists consider problem solving as a process involving a. restructuring. b.