ToK Today

ToK Essay #5 May 23: Visual Representations

The video guide to ToK Essay #5 May 23: Visual Representations has been posted on YouTube today (and is linked below). This essay was a real challenge for me to summarise in a short video. When I wrote the Essay Guide Notes (linked) I found it to be a very wide ranging subject, and the notes ran to over 6,300 words !

visual representation in tok

Visual Representations.

The focus on visual representations seems fairly straightforward, they're tangible things such as maps and graphs. However, the knowledge links are a little more obscure in this essay than in the others. The essay question specifies that we look at visual representation in terms of the communication of knowledge. This lends itself to the purpose, and maybe, context of knowledge. I also played around with ideas such as the simplification of knowledge (harking back to a PT a few years ago), but this seemed to be a potential diversion.

There's such a wide range of visual representations that students can look at, in some ways it's a possible diversionary danger. It's important that students stay focussed on whether those representations are 'helpful' in communication. I have tried to think of 'helpful' in terms of the purpose and context of knowledge.

The Knower & Knowledge.

The focus on visual representations in the communication of knowledge seems to link well with the core theme of The Knower & Knowledge, and that's mainly where I ended up. It gives us good links to the ToK Concepts of Interpretation, Explanation and Evidence. The (re)introduction of The Knower into the ToK course gives students a lot more scope in their ToK Essays to go beyond the production of knowledge that they were largely constrained to in the last syllabus.

You can find the video guide for  Essay #1 linked here .

You can find the video guide for  Essay #2 linked here.

You can find the video guide for  Essay #3 linked here.

If you want more detailed notes on the Knowledge Questions and arguments raised in the video then please check out the  detailed notes available here.

If you want help with your ToK Essay or Exhibition then please check out the  Student Support page linked here .

You can always contact me at [email protected] , stay Tok-tastic!, Daniel

ToK Essay #4 M23: Astonishing !

Tok essay #3 bubbles video guide posted.

  • TOK Talk Podcast
  • TOK concepts
  • TOK Exhibition Prompts
  • TOK Essay Posters

TOK RESOURCE.ORG - 2024

 areas of knowledge: the arts.

Kibbutz Contemporary Dance Company from Israel, performing: If At All.

Kibbutz Contemporary Dance Company from Israel, performing: If At All.

The Arts include a range of disciplines including visual arts, theatre, dance, music, film and literature. The ways in which meaning is encoded, evoked and played out in these various forms of representation do not always overlap and may have no exact counterpart elsewhere. The arts, from the sublime to the mediocre (and even the banal) can provide insight and provoke questions about what it means to be human. Art can be made for its own sake, or created with a functionality in mind like catalyzing social change, conveying overt political or religious messages, and even self-referentially pushing the boundaries of art itself.

Takashi Murakami (2012) The 500 Arhats (detail). Acrylic on canvas mounted on board.  Mori Art Museum, Tokyo. The entire painting is 100 meters long! Takashi Murakami is represented by the Kaikai Kiki Gallery.

Takashi Murakami (2012) The 500 Arhats (detail). Acrylic on canvas mounted on board. Mori Art Museum, Tokyo. The entire painting is 100 meters long!

Takashi Murakami is represented by the Kaikai Kiki Gallery.

CLASS ACTIVITIES

Non-linguistic forms of representation: Shostakovich 8th string quartet Experiencing the music Evoked emotions in the visual arts Gallery of powerful emotions and appetites Picasso’s lie and Coleridge's willing suspension of disbelief Picasso's Tête de taureau Willing suspension of disbelief Imagination (with constraints) and living in the subjunctive Artistic imagination Imagination in a tight straitjacket Are imagining and remembering almost the same? Do we live in the subjunctive? Rembrandt self portraits A recognized central case of greatness in traditional  art Duchamp's Fountain and Cage's 4'33'' Provocative artworks stack The value of art Artworks key and auction value Astonishing high art brain imaging Glass Brain and Self Reflected

KNOWLEDGE QUESTIONS

The new Theory of Knowledge Guide (2020) provides 385 Knowledge Questions for student exploration. Here are my personal favorites from the Arts section.

Is artistic knowledge something that cannot be expressed in any other way?

Does art enlarge what it is possible for us to think and know?

PERSPECTIVE

Can a work of art have meaning of which the artist themselves is unaware?

How does knowing more about the social, cultural or historical context of a work of art have an impact on our knowledge of the work itself?

Should your judgments about art be given the same weight as those of an expert?

METHODS AND TOOLS

Can some knowledge in the arts only be gained through experience? How does the medium used change the way that knowledge is produced, shared or understood?

How important is the study of literature in our individual ethical development?

Is the production and enjoyment of art subject to ethical constraints?

Can we separate the moral character of the artist from the value of the Artwork?

CONNECTING TO THE CORE THEME

What moral responsibilities do we have regarding art that has been created or published by other people?

“No one who has not seen the Sistine Chapel can have a clear idea of what a human being can achieve… The master's inner security and strength, his greatness is beyond all description…” Johann Wolfgang von Goethe (1786) In Goldscheider, L (1975) Michelangelo: Paintings, Sculptures, Architecture: Complete Edition . Phaidon

amazing-script

Theory of Knowledge 's Sample TOK Essay

Are visual representations always helpful in the communication of knowledge discuss with reference to the human sciences & mathematics.

B

Table of content

Works cited.

The purpose of knowledge is for it to be communicated and shared among communities and individuals as it is a crucial tool for human progress as it enables humans to make informed decisions and solve complex problems. This suggests that sharing knowledge is essential for societies to advance. First, we should explore the true meaning of knowledge and visual representations. Knowledge is facts, information, and skills gained by someone through experience or education. (Murray) Often visual representations, which are demonstrations of concepts and ideas accompanied by images or texts, are used to communicate knowledge as they can be considered as knowledge themselves and are evaluated based on their accuracy, effectiveness, and clarity. Communicating a message or intention with words alone can be difficult but if visual representations are incorporated, it allows the message to be received easier. However, this depends on if they are used and communicated successfully, if not they may not always be helpful. Visual representations are present in everyday life, numbers and words classify as visual representations because they define things for our understanding. The use of visual representations has been present since the earliest cave paintings to make sense of the world around us and to transfer our understanding to others. These visual representations were used to tell stories and record experiences before the evolution of written language.

Visual representations are often essential in the field of mathematics since they are used to convey complex concepts and skills to a variety of audiences, including students, teachers, researchers, and the public. However, they serve different purposes depending on how they are interpreted. In addition, visual representations are helpful in the field of human sciences, particularly economics. By using graphs, charts, and other visual representations, economists can communicate trends, relationships, and patterns that may not instantly be apparent from observing data. However, although visual aids can indeed be effective in many circumstances, there are some situations in which they might not be as helpful as other methods of communication might be. In some cases, visual aids might be misleading creating incorrect conclusions to be drawn from the data. This can occur when the visual representation is presented incorrectly or overly simplified. Although visual representations may be helpful tools in the communication of knowledge, their effectiveness is dependent on various factors such as the audience, the complexity of the topic, the reliability of the visual representation and the perception of the communication, as demonstrated in Economics and Mathematics.

Visual representations are not only useful but also necessary in the communication of knowledge. One of the major advantages of visual representations in mathematics is that they can provide a clear and concise method of understanding complex concepts that form part of mathematics. This is seen through the visual perception of third-dimensional objects. Three-dimensional objects are difficult to imagine when depicted on two-dimensional objects; however, one can get a glimpse of the third dimension through an optical illusion called the Necker curve. (Rowe) The Necker cube can be used to demonstrate the different perspectives of a visual representation because the cube can be seen as facing one of two possible directions. This visual representation aids in creating an intuition of considering multiple different perspectives when attempting to solve mathematical equations. In doing this, new insights and conclusions can be drawn for a problem. Although we do not consciously process the knowledge that is being communicated to us through the Necker cube, our brain processes this information due to the differing perspectives and perceptions of the two-dimensional image. While the cube helps knowledge to be communicated to people individually, it also highlights the subjective nature of perception meaning that one individual may perceive it differently from another. The Necker cube is useful for the understanding of mathematics since it aids in the understanding of three-dimensional space which is needed in many mathematical concepts such as vectors.

However, visual representations may not always be helpful due to the targeted audience. For example, visual representations in mathematics may not be highly accessible for visually impaired individuals because it is a language which consists of symbols and equations. The Nemeth Code is a special type of braille used for mathematical and scientific notation, which is always helpful in the communication of knowledge. It was developed by Dr. Abraham Nemeth who was blind as part of his doctoral studies in mathematics. (“What is Nemeth”) Mathematical concepts are often abstract and difficult to visualize which deems them difficult to comprehend. For visually impaired individuals, this comprehension barrier is due to the lack of visual cues. However, the Nemeth Code allows them to touch mathematical symbols and graphs, providing physical representation which aids in conceptualizing and explanation, this code allows equations and symbols to be conveyed with a similar level of precision and accuracy as visual representations, allowing for equal access to knowledge for everyone. However, not all concepts can be easily represented through braille since the complexity of some diagrams may be difficult to translate into a tactile representation and not all information is suitable to be presented visually due to its complexity. The Nemeth Code does not classify as a visual representation since it is a tactile code, and it does not rely on visual perception of information. Therefore, visual representations are not always helpful in the communication of knowledge.

Regarding the human sciences, economics makes use of visual diagrams since they are often essential for effectively explaining and proving relationships between factors. Arthur Laffer introduced the ‘Laffer Curve’ which is a graphical representation that shows the relationship between tax rates and government revenue. (Hayes) The Laffer Curve is used to simplify a complex theory that is based on the principle that as tax rates increase, government revenue will also increase, but only up until a certain point but beyond that point, higher tax rates will lead to a decrease in government revenue, as people may make the choice to evade taxes since they are too high, or they will relocate to another country with lower tax rates. The Laffer Curve helps to portray this theory in a simple way, but it is not intuitive. Furthermore, this curve objectifies conventional ways of thinking and preconceived notions about taxation. For individuals who do not study economics, this curve might be slightly confusing because the curve solely presents the abnormal relationship between taxes and revenue but does not provide an explanation or the consequences. This is validated because critics argue that the Laffer curve is too simplistic. (Hayes)

Nonetheless, visual representations are not always helpful in the communication of knowledge in relation to the human sciences due to their validity. In psychology, visual representations are often used to aid in the process of assessing and diagnosing a patient. For example, the Rorschach Inkblot Test (RIT) is a projective psychological test in which subjects’ perception of inkblots are recorded and then analyzed to examine a person’s personality characteristics and emotional functioning. (“Rorschach Test”) However, there are doubts regarding its validity. The RIT involves problems with the standardization of the test. The test involves the patient describing what they see in ambiguous inkblot images (Cherry) , however, these images may be presented to the patient in contrasting ways by the practitioner such as varying the orientation, size, and order that they are presented in. This could lead to inconsistent results which means that the test might not always provide accurate information, which hinders its effectiveness for communicating knowledge since the patient could be falsely diagnosed. Furthermore, the RIT can be exposed to misinterpretation and biases since the specialist may have preconceived perceptions about the outcome of the test which would influence their interpretation of the patient’s response. This later leads to inaccurate conclusions being made, which counteracts the claim that visual representations are always helpful in the communication of knowledge. In certain situations, such as this one, a verbal or written explanation may be more effective at communicating knowledge than through the use of visual representations. In addition, the patient’s responses may be influenced by various factors such as their mood at the time of taking the test which would influence their results based on the timing, this could reveal an inaccurate truth about that person.

In conclusion, visual representations can be helpful in the communication of knowledge, as seen with the Necker cube and the Laffer Curve, however, they are not without limitations. Thus, although visual representations can be useful in the communication of knowledge, this is not always the case. This is because visual representations allow scope for misinterpretation due to various factors such as the audience, the complexity of the topic, the reliability of the visual representation and the perception, as seen with the Nemeth Code and the Rorschach Inkblot Test.

Cherry, Kendra. "What Is the Rorschach Inkblot Test?" Verywellmind, 6 Nov. 2022 , www.verywellmind.com/what-is-the-rorschach-inkblot-test-2795806. Accessed 15 Feb. 2023.

Hayes, Adam. "Laffer Curve: History and Critique." Investopedia , 26 Jan. 2023 , www.investopedia.com/terms/l/laffercurve.asp. Accessed 15 Feb. 2023 .

Murray, Dom. "The Difference between Knowledge and Skills." Go 1 , 22 May 2017 , www.go1.com/blog/post-difference-knowledge-skills. Accessed 14 Feb. 2023 .

"Rorschach Test." Wikipedia , 11 Jan. 2023 , en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Rorschach_test. Accessed 15 Feb. 2023 .

Rowe, Chip. "A Travel Guide for the Fourth Dimension." Nautilus , 15 Oct. 2015 , nautil.us/a-travel-guide-for-the-fourth-dimension- 235660 /. Accessed 16 Feb. 2023 .

"What Is Nemeth Code?" The Alliance for Access to Computing Careers , www.washington.edu/accesscomputing/what-nemeth-code. Accessed 14 Feb. 2023 .

  • Open access
  • Published: 19 July 2015

The role of visual representations in scientific practices: from conceptual understanding and knowledge generation to ‘seeing’ how science works

  • Maria Evagorou 1 ,
  • Sibel Erduran 2 &
  • Terhi Mäntylä 3  

International Journal of STEM Education volume  2 , Article number:  11 ( 2015 ) Cite this article

76k Accesses

78 Citations

13 Altmetric

Metrics details

The use of visual representations (i.e., photographs, diagrams, models) has been part of science, and their use makes it possible for scientists to interact with and represent complex phenomena, not observable in other ways. Despite a wealth of research in science education on visual representations, the emphasis of such research has mainly been on the conceptual understanding when using visual representations and less on visual representations as epistemic objects. In this paper, we argue that by positioning visual representations as epistemic objects of scientific practices, science education can bring a renewed focus on how visualization contributes to knowledge formation in science from the learners’ perspective.

This is a theoretical paper, and in order to argue about the role of visualization, we first present a case study, that of the discovery of the structure of DNA that highlights the epistemic components of visual information in science. The second case study focuses on Faraday’s use of the lines of magnetic force. Faraday is known of his exploratory, creative, and yet systemic way of experimenting, and the visual reasoning leading to theoretical development was an inherent part of the experimentation. Third, we trace a contemporary account from science focusing on the experimental practices and how reproducibility of experimental procedures can be reinforced through video data.

Conclusions

Our conclusions suggest that in teaching science, the emphasis in visualization should shift from cognitive understanding—using the products of science to understand the content—to engaging in the processes of visualization. Furthermore, we suggest that is it essential to design curriculum materials and learning environments that create a social and epistemic context and invite students to engage in the practice of visualization as evidence, reasoning, experimental procedure, or a means of communication and reflect on these practices. Implications for teacher education include the need for teacher professional development programs to problematize the use of visual representations as epistemic objects that are part of scientific practices.

During the last decades, research and reform documents in science education across the world have been calling for an emphasis not only on the content but also on the processes of science (Bybee 2014 ; Eurydice 2012 ; Duschl and Bybee 2014 ; Osborne 2014 ; Schwartz et al. 2012 ), in order to make science accessible to the students and enable them to understand the epistemic foundation of science. Scientific practices, part of the process of science, are the cognitive and discursive activities that are targeted in science education to develop epistemic understanding and appreciation of the nature of science (Duschl et al. 2008 ) and have been the emphasis of recent reform documents in science education across the world (Achieve 2013 ; Eurydice 2012 ). With the term scientific practices, we refer to the processes that take place during scientific discoveries and include among others: asking questions, developing and using models, engaging in arguments, and constructing and communicating explanations (National Research Council 2012 ). The emphasis on scientific practices aims to move the teaching of science from knowledge to the understanding of the processes and the epistemic aspects of science. Additionally, by placing an emphasis on engaging students in scientific practices, we aim to help students acquire scientific knowledge in meaningful contexts that resemble the reality of scientific discoveries.

Despite a wealth of research in science education on visual representations, the emphasis of such research has mainly been on the conceptual understanding when using visual representations and less on visual representations as epistemic objects. In this paper, we argue that by positioning visual representations as epistemic objects, science education can bring a renewed focus on how visualization contributes to knowledge formation in science from the learners’ perspective. Specifically, the use of visual representations (i.e., photographs, diagrams, tables, charts) has been part of science and over the years has evolved with the new technologies (i.e., from drawings to advanced digital images and three dimensional models). Visualization makes it possible for scientists to interact with complex phenomena (Richards 2003 ), and they might convey important evidence not observable in other ways. Visual representations as a tool to support cognitive understanding in science have been studied extensively (i.e., Gilbert 2010 ; Wu and Shah 2004 ). Studies in science education have explored the use of images in science textbooks (i.e., Dimopoulos et al. 2003 ; Bungum 2008 ), students’ representations or models when doing science (i.e., Gilbert et al. 2008 ; Dori et al. 2003 ; Lehrer and Schauble 2012 ; Schwarz et al. 2009 ), and students’ images of science and scientists (i.e., Chambers 1983 ). Therefore, studies in the field of science education have been using the term visualization as “the formation of an internal representation from an external representation” (Gilbert et al. 2008 , p. 4) or as a tool for conceptual understanding for students.

In this paper, we do not refer to visualization as mental image, model, or presentation only (Gilbert et al. 2008 ; Philips et al. 2010 ) but instead focus on visual representations or visualization as epistemic objects. Specifically, we refer to visualization as a process for knowledge production and growth in science. In this respect, modeling is an aspect of visualization, but what we are focusing on with visualization is not on the use of model as a tool for cognitive understanding (Gilbert 2010 ; Wu and Shah 2004 ) but the on the process of modeling as a scientific practice which includes the construction and use of models, the use of other representations, the communication in the groups with the use of the visual representation, and the appreciation of the difficulties that the science phase in this process. Therefore, the purpose of this paper is to present through the history of science how visualization can be considered not only as a cognitive tool in science education but also as an epistemic object that can potentially support students to understand aspects of the nature of science.

Scientific practices and science education

According to the New Generation Science Standards (Achieve 2013 ), scientific practices refer to: asking questions and defining problems; developing and using models; planning and carrying out investigations; analyzing and interpreting data; using mathematical and computational thinking; constructing explanations and designing solutions; engaging in argument from evidence; and obtaining, evaluating, and communicating information. A significant aspect of scientific practices is that science learning is more than just about learning facts, concepts, theories, and laws. A fuller appreciation of science necessitates the understanding of the science relative to its epistemological grounding and the process that are involved in the production of knowledge (Hogan and Maglienti 2001 ; Wickman 2004 ).

The New Generation Science Standards is, among other changes, shifting away from science inquiry and towards the inclusion of scientific practices (Duschl and Bybee 2014 ; Osborne 2014 ). By comparing the abilities to do scientific inquiry (National Research Council 2000 ) with the set of scientific practices, it is evident that the latter is about engaging in the processes of doing science and experiencing in that way science in a more authentic way. Engaging in scientific practices according to Osborne ( 2014 ) “presents a more authentic picture of the endeavor that is science” (p.183) and also helps the students to develop a deeper understanding of the epistemic aspects of science. Furthermore, as Bybee ( 2014 ) argues, by engaging students in scientific practices, we involve them in an understanding of the nature of science and an understanding on the nature of scientific knowledge.

Science as a practice and scientific practices as a term emerged by the philosopher of science, Kuhn (Osborne 2014 ), refers to the processes in which the scientists engage during knowledge production and communication. The work that is followed by historians, philosophers, and sociologists of science (Latour 2011 ; Longino 2002 ; Nersessian 2008 ) revealed the scientific practices in which the scientists engage in and include among others theory development and specific ways of talking, modeling, and communicating the outcomes of science.

Visualization as an epistemic object

Schematic, pictorial symbols in the design of scientific instruments and analysis of the perceptual and functional information that is being stored in those images have been areas of investigation in philosophy of scientific experimentation (Gooding et al. 1993 ). The nature of visual perception, the relationship between thought and vision, and the role of reproducibility as a norm for experimental research form a central aspect of this domain of research in philosophy of science. For instance, Rothbart ( 1997 ) has argued that visualizations are commonplace in the theoretical sciences even if every scientific theory may not be defined by visualized models.

Visual representations (i.e., photographs, diagrams, tables, charts, models) have been used in science over the years to enable scientists to interact with complex phenomena (Richards 2003 ) and might convey important evidence not observable in other ways (Barber et al. 2006 ). Some authors (e.g., Ruivenkamp and Rip 2010 ) have argued that visualization is as a core activity of some scientific communities of practice (e.g., nanotechnology) while others (e.g., Lynch and Edgerton 1988 ) have differentiated the role of particular visualization techniques (e.g., of digital image processing in astronomy). Visualization in science includes the complex process through which scientists develop or produce imagery, schemes, and graphical representation, and therefore, what is of importance in this process is not only the result but also the methodology employed by the scientists, namely, how this result was produced. Visual representations in science may refer to objects that are believed to have some kind of material or physical existence but equally might refer to purely mental, conceptual, and abstract constructs (Pauwels 2006 ). More specifically, visual representations can be found for: (a) phenomena that are not observable with the eye (i.e., microscopic or macroscopic); (b) phenomena that do not exist as visual representations but can be translated as such (i.e., sound); and (c) in experimental settings to provide visual data representations (i.e., graphs presenting velocity of moving objects). Additionally, since science is not only about replicating reality but also about making it more understandable to people (either to the public or other scientists), visual representations are not only about reproducing the nature but also about: (a) functioning in helping solving a problem, (b) filling gaps in our knowledge, and (c) facilitating knowledge building or transfer (Lynch 2006 ).

Using or developing visual representations in the scientific practice can range from a straightforward to a complicated situation. More specifically, scientists can observe a phenomenon (i.e., mitosis) and represent it visually using a picture or diagram, which is quite straightforward. But they can also use a variety of complicated techniques (i.e., crystallography in the case of DNA studies) that are either available or need to be developed or refined in order to acquire the visual information that can be used in the process of theory development (i.e., Latour and Woolgar 1979 ). Furthermore, some visual representations need decoding, and the scientists need to learn how to read these images (i.e., radiologists); therefore, using visual representations in the process of science requires learning a new language that is specific to the medium/methods that is used (i.e., understanding an X-ray picture is different from understanding an MRI scan) and then communicating that language to other scientists and the public.

There are much intent and purposes of visual representations in scientific practices, as for example to make a diagnosis, compare, describe, and preserve for future study, verify and explore new territory, generate new data (Pauwels 2006 ), or present new methodologies. According to Latour and Woolgar ( 1979 ) and Knorr Cetina ( 1999 ), visual representations can be used either as primary data (i.e., image from a microscope). or can be used to help in concept development (i.e., models of DNA used by Watson and Crick), to uncover relationships and to make the abstract more concrete (graphs of sound waves). Therefore, visual representations and visual practices, in all forms, are an important aspect of the scientific practices in developing, clarifying, and transmitting scientific knowledge (Pauwels 2006 ).

Methods and Results: Merging Visualization and scientific practices in science

In this paper, we present three case studies that embody the working practices of scientists in an effort to present visualization as a scientific practice and present our argument about how visualization is a complex process that could include among others modeling and use of representation but is not only limited to that. The first case study explores the role of visualization in the construction of knowledge about the structure of DNA, using visuals as evidence. The second case study focuses on Faraday’s use of the lines of magnetic force and the visual reasoning leading to the theoretical development that was an inherent part of the experimentation. The third case study focuses on the current practices of scientists in the context of a peer-reviewed journal called the Journal of Visualized Experiments where the methodology is communicated through videotaped procedures. The three case studies represent the research interests of the three authors of this paper and were chosen to present how visualization as a practice can be involved in all stages of doing science, from hypothesizing and evaluating evidence (case study 1) to experimenting and reasoning (case study 2) to communicating the findings and methodology with the research community (case study 3), and represent in this way the three functions of visualization as presented by Lynch ( 2006 ). Furthermore, the last case study showcases how the development of visualization technologies has contributed to the communication of findings and methodologies in science and present in that way an aspect of current scientific practices. In all three cases, our approach is guided by the observation that the visual information is an integral part of scientific practices at the least and furthermore that they are particularly central in the scientific practices of science.

Case study 1: use visual representations as evidence in the discovery of DNA

The focus of the first case study is the discovery of the structure of DNA. The DNA was first isolated in 1869 by Friedrich Miescher, and by the late 1940s, it was known that it contained phosphate, sugar, and four nitrogen-containing chemical bases. However, no one had figured the structure of the DNA until Watson and Crick presented their model of DNA in 1953. Other than the social aspects of the discovery of the DNA, another important aspect was the role of visual evidence that led to knowledge development in the area. More specifically, by studying the personal accounts of Watson ( 1968 ) and Crick ( 1988 ) about the discovery of the structure of the DNA, the following main ideas regarding the role of visual representations in the production of knowledge can be identified: (a) The use of visual representations was an important part of knowledge growth and was often dependent upon the discovery of new technologies (i.e., better microscopes or better techniques in crystallography that would provide better visual representations as evidence of the helical structure of the DNA); and (b) Models (three-dimensional) were used as a way to represent the visual images (X-ray images) and connect them to the evidence provided by other sources to see whether the theory can be supported. Therefore, the model of DNA was built based on the combination of visual evidence and experimental data.

An example showcasing the importance of visual representations in the process of knowledge production in this case is provided by Watson, in his book The Double Helix (1968):

…since the middle of the summer Rosy [Rosalind Franklin] had had evidence for a new three-dimensional form of DNA. It occurred when the DNA 2molecules were surrounded by a large amount of water. When I asked what the pattern was like, Maurice went into the adjacent room to pick up a print of the new form they called the “B” structure. The instant I saw the picture, my mouth fell open and my pulse began to race. The pattern was unbelievably simpler than those previously obtained (A form). Moreover, the black cross of reflections which dominated the picture could arise only from a helical structure. With the A form the argument for the helix was never straightforward, and considerable ambiguity existed as to exactly which type of helical symmetry was present. With the B form however, mere inspection of its X-ray picture gave several of the vital helical parameters. (p. 167-169)

As suggested by Watson’s personal account of the discovery of the DNA, the photo taken by Rosalind Franklin (Fig.  1 ) convinced him that the DNA molecule must consist of two chains arranged in a paired helix, which resembles a spiral staircase or ladder, and on March 7, 1953, Watson and Crick finished and presented their model of the structure of DNA (Watson and Berry 2004 ; Watson 1968 ) which was based on the visual information provided by the X-ray image and their knowledge of chemistry.

X-ray chrystallography of DNA

In analyzing the visualization practice in this case study, we observe the following instances that highlight how the visual information played a role:

Asking questions and defining problems: The real world in the model of science can at some points only be observed through visual representations or representations, i.e., if we are using DNA as an example, the structure of DNA was only observable through the crystallography images produced by Rosalind Franklin in the laboratory. There was no other way to observe the structure of DNA, therefore the real world.

Analyzing and interpreting data: The images that resulted from crystallography as well as their interpretations served as the data for the scientists studying the structure of DNA.

Experimenting: The data in the form of visual information were used to predict the possible structure of the DNA.

Modeling: Based on the prediction, an actual three-dimensional model was prepared by Watson and Crick. The first model did not fit with the real world (refuted by Rosalind Franklin and her research group from King’s College) and Watson and Crick had to go through the same process again to find better visual evidence (better crystallography images) and create an improved visual model.

Example excerpts from Watson’s biography provide further evidence for how visualization practices were applied in the context of the discovery of DNA (Table  1 ).

In summary, by examining the history of the discovery of DNA, we showcased how visual data is used as scientific evidence in science, identifying in that way an aspect of the nature of science that is still unexplored in the history of science and an aspect that has been ignored in the teaching of science. Visual representations are used in many ways: as images, as models, as evidence to support or rebut a model, and as interpretations of reality.

Case study 2: applying visual reasoning in knowledge production, the example of the lines of magnetic force

The focus of this case study is on Faraday’s use of the lines of magnetic force. Faraday is known of his exploratory, creative, and yet systemic way of experimenting, and the visual reasoning leading to theoretical development was an inherent part of this experimentation (Gooding 2006 ). Faraday’s articles or notebooks do not include mathematical formulations; instead, they include images and illustrations from experimental devices and setups to the recapping of his theoretical ideas (Nersessian 2008 ). According to Gooding ( 2006 ), “Faraday’s visual method was designed not to copy apparent features of the world, but to analyse and replicate them” (2006, p. 46).

The lines of force played a central role in Faraday’s research on electricity and magnetism and in the development of his “field theory” (Faraday 1852a ; Nersessian 1984 ). Before Faraday, the experiments with iron filings around magnets were known and the term “magnetic curves” was used for the iron filing patterns and also for the geometrical constructs derived from the mathematical theory of magnetism (Gooding et al. 1993 ). However, Faraday used the lines of force for explaining his experimental observations and in constructing the theory of forces in magnetism and electricity. Examples of Faraday’s different illustrations of lines of magnetic force are given in Fig.  2 . Faraday gave the following experiment-based definition for the lines of magnetic forces:

a Iron filing pattern in case of bar magnet drawn by Faraday (Faraday 1852b , Plate IX, p. 158, Fig. 1), b Faraday’s drawing of lines of magnetic force in case of cylinder magnet, where the experimental procedure, knife blade showing the direction of lines, is combined into drawing (Faraday, 1855, vol. 1, plate 1)

A line of magnetic force may be defined as that line which is described by a very small magnetic needle, when it is so moved in either direction correspondent to its length, that the needle is constantly a tangent to the line of motion; or it is that line along which, if a transverse wire be moved in either direction, there is no tendency to the formation of any current in the wire, whilst if moved in any other direction there is such a tendency; or it is that line which coincides with the direction of the magnecrystallic axis of a crystal of bismuth, which is carried in either direction along it. The direction of these lines about and amongst magnets and electric currents, is easily represented and understood, in a general manner, by the ordinary use of iron filings. (Faraday 1852a , p. 25 (3071))

The definition describes the connection between the experiments and the visual representation of the results. Initially, the lines of force were just geometric representations, but later, Faraday treated them as physical objects (Nersessian 1984 ; Pocovi and Finlay 2002 ):

I have sometimes used the term lines of force so vaguely, as to leave the reader doubtful whether I intended it as a merely representative idea of the forces, or as the description of the path along which the power was continuously exerted. … wherever the expression line of force is taken simply to represent the disposition of forces, it shall have the fullness of that meaning; but that wherever it may seem to represent the idea of the physical mode of transmission of the force, it expresses in that respect the opinion to which I incline at present. The opinion may be erroneous, and yet all that relates or refers to the disposition of the force will remain the same. (Faraday, 1852a , p. 55-56 (3075))

He also felt that the lines of force had greater explanatory power than the dominant theory of action-at-a-distance:

Now it appears to me that these lines may be employed with great advantage to represent nature, condition, direction and comparative amount of the magnetic forces; and that in many cases they have, to the physical reasoned at least, a superiority over that method which represents the forces as concentrated in centres of action… (Faraday, 1852a , p. 26 (3074))

For giving some insight to Faraday’s visual reasoning as an epistemic practice, the following examples of Faraday’s studies of the lines of magnetic force (Faraday 1852a , 1852b ) are presented:

(a) Asking questions and defining problems: The iron filing patterns formed the empirical basis for the visual model: 2D visualization of lines of magnetic force as presented in Fig.  2 . According to Faraday, these iron filing patterns were suitable for illustrating the direction and form of the magnetic lines of force (emphasis added):

It must be well understood that these forms give no indication by their appearance of the relative strength of the magnetic force at different places, inasmuch as the appearance of the lines depends greatly upon the quantity of filings and the amount of tapping; but the direction and forms of these lines are well given, and these indicate, in a considerable degree, the direction in which the forces increase and diminish . (Faraday 1852b , p.158 (3237))

Despite being static and two dimensional on paper, the lines of magnetic force were dynamical (Nersessian 1992 , 2008 ) and three dimensional for Faraday (see Fig.  2 b). For instance, Faraday described the lines of force “expanding”, “bending,” and “being cut” (Nersessian 1992 ). In Fig.  2 b, Faraday has summarized his experiment (bar magnet and knife blade) and its results (lines of force) in one picture.

(b) Analyzing and interpreting data: The model was so powerful for Faraday that he ended up thinking them as physical objects (e.g., Nersessian 1984 ), i.e., making interpretations of the way forces act. Of course, he made a lot of experiments for showing the physical existence of the lines of force, but he did not succeed in it (Nersessian 1984 ). The following quote illuminates Faraday’s use of the lines of force in different situations:

The study of these lines has, at different times, been greatly influential in leading me to various results, which I think prove their utility as well as fertility. Thus, the law of magneto-electric induction; the earth’s inductive action; the relation of magnetism and light; diamagnetic action and its law, and magnetocrystallic action, are the cases of this kind… (Faraday 1852a , p. 55 (3174))

(c) Experimenting: In Faraday's case, he used a lot of exploratory experiments; in case of lines of magnetic force, he used, e.g., iron filings, magnetic needles, or current carrying wires (see the quote above). The magnetic field is not directly observable and the representation of lines of force was a visual model, which includes the direction, form, and magnitude of field.

(d) Modeling: There is no denying that the lines of magnetic force are visual by nature. Faraday’s views of lines of force developed gradually during the years, and he applied and developed them in different contexts such as electromagnetic, electrostatic, and magnetic induction (Nersessian 1984 ). An example of Faraday’s explanation of the effect of the wire b’s position to experiment is given in Fig.  3 . In Fig.  3 , few magnetic lines of force are drawn, and in the quote below, Faraday is explaining the effect using these magnetic lines of force (emphasis added):

Picture of an experiment with different arrangements of wires ( a , b’ , b” ), magnet, and galvanometer. Note the lines of force drawn around the magnet. (Faraday 1852a , p. 34)

It will be evident by inspection of Fig. 3 , that, however the wires are carried away, the general result will, according to the assumed principles of action, be the same; for if a be the axial wire, and b’, b”, b”’ the equatorial wire, represented in three different positions, whatever magnetic lines of force pass across the latter wire in one position, will also pass it in the other, or in any other position which can be given to it. The distance of the wire at the place of intersection with the lines of force, has been shown, by the experiments (3093.), to be unimportant. (Faraday 1852a , p. 34 (3099))

In summary, by examining the history of Faraday’s use of lines of force, we showed how visual imagery and reasoning played an important part in Faraday’s construction and representation of his “field theory”. As Gooding has stated, “many of Faraday’s sketches are far more that depictions of observation, they are tools for reasoning with and about phenomena” (2006, p. 59).

Case study 3: visualizing scientific methods, the case of a journal

The focus of the third case study is the Journal of Visualized Experiments (JoVE) , a peer-reviewed publication indexed in PubMed. The journal devoted to the publication of biological, medical, chemical, and physical research in a video format. The journal describes its history as follows:

JoVE was established as a new tool in life science publication and communication, with participation of scientists from leading research institutions. JoVE takes advantage of video technology to capture and transmit the multiple facets and intricacies of life science research. Visualization greatly facilitates the understanding and efficient reproduction of both basic and complex experimental techniques, thereby addressing two of the biggest challenges faced by today's life science research community: i) low transparency and poor reproducibility of biological experiments and ii) time and labor-intensive nature of learning new experimental techniques. ( http://www.jove.com/ )

By examining the journal content, we generate a set of categories that can be considered as indicators of relevance and significance in terms of epistemic practices of science that have relevance for science education. For example, the quote above illustrates how scientists view some norms of scientific practice including the norms of “transparency” and “reproducibility” of experimental methods and results, and how the visual format of the journal facilitates the implementation of these norms. “Reproducibility” can be considered as an epistemic criterion that sits at the heart of what counts as an experimental procedure in science:

Investigating what should be reproducible and by whom leads to different types of experimental reproducibility, which can be observed to play different roles in experimental practice. A successful application of the strategy of reproducing an experiment is an achievement that may depend on certain isiosyncratic aspects of a local situation. Yet a purely local experiment that cannot be carried out by other experimenters and in other experimental contexts will, in the end be unproductive in science. (Sarkar and Pfeifer 2006 , p.270)

We now turn to an article on “Elevated Plus Maze for Mice” that is available for free on the journal website ( http://www.jove.com/video/1088/elevated-plus-maze-for-mice ). The purpose of this experiment was to investigate anxiety levels in mice through behavioral analysis. The journal article consists of a 9-min video accompanied by text. The video illustrates the handling of the mice in soundproof location with less light, worksheets with characteristics of mice, computer software, apparatus, resources, setting up the computer software, and the video recording of mouse behavior on the computer. The authors describe the apparatus that is used in the experiment and state how procedural differences exist between research groups that lead to difficulties in the interpretation of results:

The apparatus consists of open arms and closed arms, crossed in the middle perpendicularly to each other, and a center area. Mice are given access to all of the arms and are allowed to move freely between them. The number of entries into the open arms and the time spent in the open arms are used as indices of open space-induced anxiety in mice. Unfortunately, the procedural differences that exist between laboratories make it difficult to duplicate and compare results among laboratories.

The authors’ emphasis on the particularity of procedural context echoes in the observations of some philosophers of science:

It is not just the knowledge of experimental objects and phenomena but also their actual existence and occurrence that prove to be dependent on specific, productive interventions by the experimenters” (Sarkar and Pfeifer 2006 , pp. 270-271)

The inclusion of a video of the experimental procedure specifies what the apparatus looks like (Fig.  4 ) and how the behavior of the mice is captured through video recording that feeds into a computer (Fig.  5 ). Subsequently, a computer software which captures different variables such as the distance traveled, the number of entries, and the time spent on each arm of the apparatus. Here, there is visual information at different levels of representation ranging from reconfiguration of raw video data to representations that analyze the data around the variables in question (Fig.  6 ). The practice of levels of visual representations is not particular to the biological sciences. For instance, they are commonplace in nanotechnological practices:

Visual illustration of apparatus

Video processing of experimental set-up

Computer software for video input and variable recording

In the visualization processes, instruments are needed that can register the nanoscale and provide raw data, which needs to be transformed into images. Some Imaging Techniques have software incorporated already where this transformation automatically takes place, providing raw images. Raw data must be translated through the use of Graphic Software and software is also used for the further manipulation of images to highlight what is of interest to capture the (inferred) phenomena -- and to capture the reader. There are two levels of choice: Scientists have to choose which imaging technique and embedded software to use for the job at hand, and they will then have to follow the structure of the software. Within such software, there are explicit choices for the scientists, e.g. about colour coding, and ways of sharpening images. (Ruivenkamp and Rip 2010 , pp.14–15)

On the text that accompanies the video, the authors highlight the role of visualization in their experiment:

Visualization of the protocol will promote better understanding of the details of the entire experimental procedure, allowing for standardization of the protocols used in different laboratories and comparisons of the behavioral phenotypes of various strains of mutant mice assessed using this test.

The software that takes the video data and transforms it into various representations allows the researchers to collect data on mouse behavior more reliably. For instance, the distance traveled across the arms of the apparatus or the time spent on each arm would have been difficult to observe and record precisely. A further aspect to note is how the visualization of the experiment facilitates control of bias. The authors illustrate how the olfactory bias between experimental procedures carried on mice in sequence is avoided by cleaning the equipment.

Our discussion highlights the role of visualization in science, particularly with respect to presenting visualization as part of the scientific practices. We have used case studies from the history of science highlighting a scientist’s account of how visualization played a role in the discovery of DNA and the magnetic field and from a contemporary illustration of a science journal’s practices in incorporating visualization as a way to communicate new findings and methodologies. Our implicit aim in drawing from these case studies was the need to align science education with scientific practices, particularly in terms of how visual representations, stable or dynamic, can engage students in the processes of science and not only to be used as tools for cognitive development in science. Our approach was guided by the notion of “knowledge-as-practice” as advanced by Knorr Cetina ( 1999 ) who studied scientists and characterized their knowledge as practice, a characterization which shifts focus away from ideas inside scientists’ minds to practices that are cultural and deeply contextualized within fields of science. She suggests that people working together can be examined as epistemic cultures whose collective knowledge exists as practice.

It is important to stress, however, that visual representations are not used in isolation, but are supported by other types of evidence as well, or other theories (i.e., in order to understand the helical form of DNA, or the structure, chemistry knowledge was needed). More importantly, this finding can also have implications when teaching science as argument (e.g., Erduran and Jimenez-Aleixandre 2008 ), since the verbal evidence used in the science classroom to maintain an argument could be supported by visual evidence (either a model, representation, image, graph, etc.). For example, in a group of students discussing the outcomes of an introduced species in an ecosystem, pictures of the species and the ecosystem over time, and videos showing the changes in the ecosystem, and the special characteristics of the different species could serve as visual evidence to help the students support their arguments (Evagorou et al. 2012 ). Therefore, an important implication for the teaching of science is the use of visual representations as evidence in the science curriculum as part of knowledge production. Even though studies in the area of science education have focused on the use of models and modeling as a way to support students in the learning of science (Dori et al. 2003 ; Lehrer and Schauble 2012 ; Mendonça and Justi 2013 ; Papaevripidou et al. 2007 ) or on the use of images (i.e., Korfiatis et al. 2003 ), with the term using visuals as evidence, we refer to the collection of all forms of visuals and the processes involved.

Another aspect that was identified through the case studies is that of the visual reasoning (an integral part of Faraday’s investigations). Both the verbalization and visualization were part of the process of generating new knowledge (Gooding 2006 ). Even today, most of the textbooks use the lines of force (or just field lines) as a geometrical representation of field, and the number of field lines is connected to the quantity of flux. Often, the textbooks use the same kind of visual imagery than in what is used by scientists. However, when using images, only certain aspects or features of the phenomena or data are captured or highlighted, and often in tacit ways. Especially in textbooks, the process of producing the image is not presented and instead only the product—image—is left. This could easily lead to an idea of images (i.e., photos, graphs, visual model) being just representations of knowledge and, in the worse case, misinterpreted representations of knowledge as the results of Pocovi and Finlay ( 2002 ) in case of electric field lines show. In order to avoid this, the teachers should be able to explain how the images are produced (what features of phenomena or data the images captures, on what ground the features are chosen to that image, and what features are omitted); in this way, the role of visualization in knowledge production can be made “visible” to students by engaging them in the process of visualization.

The implication of these norms for science teaching and learning is numerous. The classroom contexts can model the generation, sharing and evaluation of evidence, and experimental procedures carried out by students, thereby promoting not only some contemporary cultural norms in scientific practice but also enabling the learning of criteria, standards, and heuristics that scientists use in making decisions on scientific methods. As we have demonstrated with the three case studies, visual representations are part of the process of knowledge growth and communication in science, as demonstrated with two examples from the history of science and an example from current scientific practices. Additionally, visual information, especially with the use of technology is a part of students’ everyday lives. Therefore, we suggest making use of students’ knowledge and technological skills (i.e., how to produce their own videos showing their experimental method or how to identify or provide appropriate visual evidence for a given topic), in order to teach them the aspects of the nature of science that are often neglected both in the history of science and the design of curriculum. Specifically, what we suggest in this paper is that students should actively engage in visualization processes in order to appreciate the diverse nature of doing science and engage in authentic scientific practices.

However, as a word of caution, we need to distinguish the products and processes involved in visualization practices in science:

If one considers scientific representations and the ways in which they can foster or thwart our understanding, it is clear that a mere object approach, which would devote all attention to the representation as a free-standing product of scientific labor, is inadequate. What is needed is a process approach: each visual representation should be linked with its context of production (Pauwels 2006 , p.21).

The aforementioned suggests that the emphasis in visualization should shift from cognitive understanding—using the products of science to understand the content—to engaging in the processes of visualization. Therefore, an implication for the teaching of science includes designing curriculum materials and learning environments that create a social and epistemic context and invite students to engage in the practice of visualization as evidence, reasoning, experimental procedure, or a means of communication (as presented in the three case studies) and reflect on these practices (Ryu et al. 2015 ).

Finally, a question that arises from including visualization in science education, as well as from including scientific practices in science education is whether teachers themselves are prepared to include them as part of their teaching (Bybee 2014 ). Teacher preparation programs and teacher education have been critiqued, studied, and rethought since the time they emerged (Cochran-Smith 2004 ). Despite the years of history in teacher training and teacher education, the debate about initial teacher training and its content still pertains in our community and in policy circles (Cochran-Smith 2004 ; Conway et al. 2009 ). In the last decades, the debate has shifted from a behavioral view of learning and teaching to a learning problem—focusing on that way not only on teachers’ knowledge, skills, and beliefs but also on making the connection of the aforementioned with how and if pupils learn (Cochran-Smith 2004 ). The Science Education in Europe report recommended that “Good quality teachers, with up-to-date knowledge and skills, are the foundation of any system of formal science education” (Osborne and Dillon 2008 , p.9).

However, questions such as what should be the emphasis on pre-service and in-service science teacher training, especially with the new emphasis on scientific practices, still remain unanswered. As Bybee ( 2014 ) argues, starting from the new emphasis on scientific practices in the NGSS, we should consider teacher preparation programs “that would provide undergraduates opportunities to learn the science content and practices in contexts that would be aligned with their future work as teachers” (p.218). Therefore, engaging pre- and in-service teachers in visualization as a scientific practice should be one of the purposes of teacher preparation programs.

Achieve. (2013). The next generation science standards (pp. 1–3). Retrieved from http://www.nextgenscience.org/ .

Google Scholar  

Barber, J, Pearson, D, & Cervetti, G. (2006). Seeds of science/roots of reading . California: The Regents of the University of California.

Bungum, B. (2008). Images of physics: an explorative study of the changing character of visual images in Norwegian physics textbooks. NorDiNa, 4 (2), 132–141.

Bybee, RW. (2014). NGSS and the next generation of science teachers. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 25 (2), 211–221. doi: 10.1007/s10972-014-9381-4 .

Article   Google Scholar  

Chambers, D. (1983). Stereotypic images of the scientist: the draw-a-scientist test. Science Education, 67 (2), 255–265.

Cochran-Smith, M. (2004). The problem of teacher education. Journal of Teacher Education, 55 (4), 295–299. doi: 10.1177/0022487104268057 .

Conway, PF, Murphy, R, & Rath, A. (2009). Learning to teach and its implications for the continuum of teacher education: a nine-country cross-national study .

Crick, F. (1988). What a mad pursuit . USA: Basic Books.

Dimopoulos, K, Koulaidis, V, & Sklaveniti, S. (2003). Towards an analysis of visual images in school science textbooks and press articles about science and technology. Research in Science Education, 33 , 189–216.

Dori, YJ, Tal, RT, & Tsaushu, M. (2003). Teaching biotechnology through case studies—can we improve higher order thinking skills of nonscience majors? Science Education, 87 (6), 767–793. doi: 10.1002/sce.10081 .

Duschl, RA, & Bybee, RW. (2014). Planning and carrying out investigations: an entry to learning and to teacher professional development around NGSS science and engineering practices. International Journal of STEM Education, 1 (1), 12. doi: 10.1186/s40594-014-0012-6 .

Duschl, R., Schweingruber, H. A., & Shouse, A. (2008). Taking science to school . Washington DC: National Academies Press.

Erduran, S, & Jimenez-Aleixandre, MP (Eds.). (2008). Argumentation in science education: perspectives from classroom-based research . Dordrecht: Springer.

Eurydice. (2012). Developing key competencies at school in Europe: challenges and opportunities for policy – 2011/12 (pp. 1–72).

Evagorou, M, Jimenez-Aleixandre, MP, & Osborne, J. (2012). “Should we kill the grey squirrels?” A study exploring students’ justifications and decision-making. International Journal of Science Education, 34 (3), 401–428. doi: 10.1080/09500693.2011.619211 .

Faraday, M. (1852a). Experimental researches in electricity. – Twenty-eighth series. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 142 , 25–56.

Faraday, M. (1852b). Experimental researches in electricity. – Twenty-ninth series. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 142 , 137–159.

Gilbert, JK. (2010). The role of visual representations in the learning and teaching of science: an introduction (pp. 1–19).

Gilbert, J., Reiner, M. & Nakhleh, M. (2008). Visualization: theory and practice in science education . Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.

Gooding, D. (2006). From phenomenology to field theory: Faraday’s visual reasoning. Perspectives on Science, 14 (1), 40–65.

Gooding, D, Pinch, T, & Schaffer, S (Eds.). (1993). The uses of experiment: studies in the natural sciences . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Hogan, K, & Maglienti, M. (2001). Comparing the epistemological underpinnings of students’ and scientists’ reasoning about conclusions. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38 (6), 663–687.

Knorr Cetina, K. (1999). Epistemic cultures: how the sciences make knowledge . Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Korfiatis, KJ, Stamou, AG, & Paraskevopoulos, S. (2003). Images of nature in Greek primary school textbooks. Science Education, 88 (1), 72–89. doi: 10.1002/sce.10133 .

Latour, B. (2011). Visualisation and cognition: drawing things together (pp. 1–32).

Latour, B, & Woolgar, S. (1979). Laboratory life: the construction of scientific facts . Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Lehrer, R, & Schauble, L. (2012). Seeding evolutionary thinking by engaging children in modeling its foundations. Science Education, 96 (4), 701–724. doi: 10.1002/sce.20475 .

Longino, H. E. (2002). The fate of knowledge . Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Lynch, M. (2006). The production of scientific images: vision and re-vision in the history, philosophy, and sociology of science. In L Pauwels (Ed.), Visual cultures of science: rethinking representational practices in knowledge building and science communication (pp. 26–40). Lebanon, NH: Darthmouth College Press.

Lynch, M. & S. Y. Edgerton Jr. (1988). ‘Aesthetic and digital image processing representational craft in contemporary astronomy’, in G. Fyfe & J. Law (eds), Picturing Power; Visual Depictions and Social Relations (London, Routledge): 184 – 220.

Mendonça, PCC, & Justi, R. (2013). An instrument for analyzing arguments produced in modeling-based chemistry lessons. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 51 (2), 192–218. doi: 10.1002/tea.21133 .

National Research Council (2000). Inquiry and the national science education standards . Washington DC: National Academies Press.

National Research Council (2012). A framework for K-12 science education . Washington DC: National Academies Press.

Nersessian, NJ. (1984). Faraday to Einstein: constructing meaning in scientific theories . Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff Publishers.

Book   Google Scholar  

Nersessian, NJ. (1992). How do scientists think? Capturing the dynamics of conceptual change in science. In RN Giere (Ed.), Cognitive Models of Science (pp. 3–45). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Nersessian, NJ. (2008). Creating scientific concepts . Cambridge: The MIT Press.

Osborne, J. (2014). Teaching scientific practices: meeting the challenge of change. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 25 (2), 177–196. doi: 10.1007/s10972-014-9384-1 .

Osborne, J. & Dillon, J. (2008). Science education in Europe: critical reflections . London: Nuffield Foundation.

Papaevripidou, M, Constantinou, CP, & Zacharia, ZC. (2007). Modeling complex marine ecosystems: an investigation of two teaching approaches with fifth graders. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 23 (2), 145–157. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2729.2006.00217.x .

Pauwels, L. (2006). A theoretical framework for assessing visual representational practices in knowledge building and science communications. In L Pauwels (Ed.), Visual cultures of science: rethinking representational practices in knowledge building and science communication (pp. 1–25). Lebanon, NH: Darthmouth College Press.

Philips, L., Norris, S. & McNab, J. (2010). Visualization in mathematics, reading and science education . Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.

Pocovi, MC, & Finlay, F. (2002). Lines of force: Faraday’s and students’ views. Science & Education, 11 , 459–474.

Richards, A. (2003). Argument and authority in the visual representations of science. Technical Communication Quarterly, 12 (2), 183–206. doi: 10.1207/s15427625tcq1202_3 .

Rothbart, D. (1997). Explaining the growth of scientific knowledge: metaphors, models and meaning . Lewiston, NY: Mellen Press.

Ruivenkamp, M, & Rip, A. (2010). Visualizing the invisible nanoscale study: visualization practices in nanotechnology community of practice. Science Studies, 23 (1), 3–36.

Ryu, S, Han, Y, & Paik, S-H. (2015). Understanding co-development of conceptual and epistemic understanding through modeling practices with mobile internet. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 24 (2-3), 330–355. doi: 10.1007/s10956-014-9545-1 .

Sarkar, S, & Pfeifer, J. (2006). The philosophy of science, chapter on experimentation (Vol. 1, A-M). New York: Taylor & Francis.

Schwartz, RS, Lederman, NG, & Abd-el-Khalick, F. (2012). A series of misrepresentations: a response to Allchin’s whole approach to assessing nature of science understandings. Science Education, 96 (4), 685–692. doi: 10.1002/sce.21013 .

Schwarz, CV, Reiser, BJ, Davis, EA, Kenyon, L, Achér, A, Fortus, D, et al. (2009). Developing a learning progression for scientific modeling: making scientific modeling accessible and meaningful for learners. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 46 (6), 632–654. doi: 10.1002/tea.20311 .

Watson, J. (1968). The Double Helix: a personal account of the discovery of the structure of DNA . New York: Scribner.

Watson, J, & Berry, A. (2004). DNA: the secret of life . New York: Alfred A. Knopf.

Wickman, PO. (2004). The practical epistemologies of the classroom: a study of laboratory work. Science Education, 88 , 325–344.

Wu, HK, & Shah, P. (2004). Exploring visuospatial thinking in chemistry learning. Science Education, 88 (3), 465–492. doi: 10.1002/sce.10126 .

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge all reviewers for their valuable comments that have helped us improve the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

University of Nicosia, 46, Makedonitissa Avenue, Egkomi, 1700, Nicosia, Cyprus

Maria Evagorou

University of Limerick, Limerick, Ireland

Sibel Erduran

University of Tampere, Tampere, Finland

Terhi Mäntylä

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Maria Evagorou .

Additional information

Competing interests.

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

ME carried out the introductory literature review, the analysis of the first case study, and drafted the manuscript. SE carried out the analysis of the third case study and contributed towards the “Conclusions” section of the manuscript. TM carried out the second case study. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 ), which permits use, duplication, adaptation, distribution, and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Evagorou, M., Erduran, S. & Mäntylä, T. The role of visual representations in scientific practices: from conceptual understanding and knowledge generation to ‘seeing’ how science works. IJ STEM Ed 2 , 11 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40594-015-0024-x

Download citation

Received : 29 September 2014

Accepted : 16 May 2015

Published : 19 July 2015

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s40594-015-0024-x

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Visual representations
  • Epistemic practices
  • Science learning

visual representation in tok

Introduction to Visual Representations

  • First Online: 01 January 2009

Cite this chapter

visual representation in tok

  • Riccardo Mazza 1  

5198 Accesses

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Unable to display preview.  Download preview PDF.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

University of Lugano, , Switzerland

Riccardo Mazza ( Dr )

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2009 Springer-Verlag London

About this chapter

Mazza, R. (2009). Introduction to Visual Representations. In: Introduction to Information Visualization. Springer, London. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-84800-219-7_1

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-84800-219-7_1

Published : 15 February 2009

Publisher Name : Springer, London

Print ISBN : 978-1-84800-218-0

Online ISBN : 978-1-84800-219-7

eBook Packages : Computer Science Computer Science (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

Visual Representations: 2023 TOK Essay Title 5

Today I enjoyed tea and Talked some TOK with Kevin Hoye (IB English Literature and TOK Teacher) about 2023 TOK Essay Title 5: Are visual representations always helpful in the communication of knowledge? Discuss with reference to the human sciences and mathematics. We talked a lot about different ways into understanding this question.. Links to several examples discussed can be found on www.TOKTalk.org Thank you to the random street Shanghai musician, once again for the music bringing us in and out of this track. Guest: Kevin Hoye

Information

  • Show TOK Talk
  • Published 25 October 2022 at 07:09 UTC
  • Length 32 min
  • Rating Clean

To listen to explicit episodes, sign in.

Apple Podcasts

Stay up to date with this show

Sign in or sign up to follow shows, save episodes and get the latest updates.

Africa, Middle East, and India

  • Brunei Darussalam
  • Burkina Faso
  • Côte d’Ivoire
  • Congo, The Democratic Republic Of The
  • Guinea-Bissau
  • Niger (English)
  • Congo, Republic of
  • Saudi Arabia
  • Sierra Leone
  • South Africa
  • Tanzania, United Republic Of
  • Turkmenistan
  • United Arab Emirates

Asia Pacific

  • Indonesia (English)
  • Lao People's Democratic Republic
  • Malaysia (English)
  • Micronesia, Federated States of
  • New Zealand
  • Papua New Guinea
  • Philippines
  • Solomon Islands
  • Bosnia and Herzegovina
  • France (Français)
  • Deutschland
  • Luxembourg (English)
  • Moldova, Republic Of
  • North Macedonia
  • Portugal (Português)
  • Türkiye (English)
  • United Kingdom

Latin America and the Caribbean

  • Antigua and Barbuda
  • Argentina (Español)
  • Bolivia (Español)
  • Virgin Islands, British
  • Cayman Islands
  • Chile (Español)
  • Colombia (Español)
  • Costa Rica (Español)
  • República Dominicana
  • Ecuador (Español)
  • El Salvador (Español)
  • Guatemala (Español)
  • Honduras (Español)
  • Nicaragua (Español)
  • Paraguay (Español)
  • St. Kitts and Nevis
  • Saint Lucia
  • St. Vincent and The Grenadines
  • Trinidad and Tobago
  • Turks and Caicos
  • Uruguay (English)
  • Venezuela (Español)

The United States and Canada

  • Canada (English)
  • Canada (Français)
  • United States
  • Estados Unidos (Español México)
  • الولايات المتحدة
  • États-Unis (Français France)
  • Estados Unidos (Português Brasil)
  • 美國 (繁體中文台灣)

IMAGES

  1. ToK Essay#5 May 23: Visual Representations

    visual representation in tok

  2. TOK presentation: Visual Representation by John Reid on Prezi

    visual representation in tok

  3. IB TOK VISUAL REPRESENTATION by Pachia Lee on Prezi

    visual representation in tok

  4. Intro to TOK

    visual representation in tok

  5. ToK Essay #5 May 23: Visual Representations

    visual representation in tok

  6. PPT

    visual representation in tok

VIDEO

  1. The Power of Representation: Filling in the Blanks and Eliminating Bias

  2. Visual Question Answering

  3. Visual Data Representation Techniques: Combining Art and Design

  4. VERTICAL VIDEO in DaVinci Resolve

  5. Visual question answering & reasoning over vision & language: Beyond limits of statistical learning?

  6. How to Use Perspective Mirroring Techniques [Lightshare Academy] 02

COMMENTS

  1. ToK Essay #5 May 23: Visual Representations

    The video guide to ToK Essay #5 May 23: Visual Representations has been posted on YouTube today (and is linked below). This essay was a real challenge for me to summarise in a short video. When I wrote the Essay Guide Notes I found it to be a very wide ranging subject, and the notes ran to over 6,300 words ! Visual Representations. ...

  2. ToK Essay#5 May 23: Visual Representations

    This is an overview guide for IB Theory of Knowledge Essay #5 May 23, the essay about visual representations.If you want more information about the points ra...

  3. 2023 Essay Title 5: Visual Representations

    Today I enjoyed tea and talked some TOK with Kevin Hoye (IB English Literature and TOK Teacher) about 2023 TOK Essay Title 5: Are visual representations always helpful in the communication of knowledge? Discuss with reference to the human sciences and mathematics. W e talked a lot about different ways into understanding this question, beginning ...

  4. IB TOK Essay example: Are visual representations always ...

    The student effectively communicates the chosen title "Are visual representations always helpful in the communication of knowledge? Discuss with reference to the human sciences and mathematics" at the beginning of the work, ensuring clarity for the TOK examiner. Throughout the essay, the focus remains on the helpfulness of visual ...

  5. TOK Essay Title #5 (May 2023)

    TOK Essay Title #5. Are visual representations always helpful in the communication of knowledge? Discuss with reference to the human sciences and mathematics. Here is the evidence from my video: The role of visual representations in scientific practices: from conceptual understanding and knowledge generation to 'seeing' how science works.

  6. ToK Essay 5 May 2023: What is meant by "helpful in the ...

    With visual representation of the knowledge that shaping must be done hypothetically, however with spoken communication of knowledge it can be done organically in response to the receiver. A fourth position in their paper which can be applied to ToK Essay 5 May 2023 is the type of knowledge best described by visual representations.

  7. IB TOK Essay example: Are visual representations always ...

    TOK. Essay. May 2023. A (Breakdown) Examiner's summary. Criterion A [9/10]:The student effectively communicates the chosen title "Are visual representations always helpful in the communication of knowledge? Discuss with reference to the human sciences and mathematics" at the beginning of the work, ensuring clarity for the TOK examiner. ...

  8. Visual Representations: 2023 TOK Essay Title 5

    TOK Talk. Education. Today I enjoyed tea and Talked some TOK with Kevin Hoye (IB English Literature and TOK Teacher) about 2023 TOK Essay Title 5: Are visual representations always helpful in the communication of knowledge? Discuss with reference to the human sciences and mathematics. We talked a lot about different ways into understanding this ...

  9. 5. the Arts

    The Arts include a range of disciplines including visual arts, theatre, dance, music, film and literature. The ways in which meaning is encoded, evoked and played out in these various forms of representation do not always overlap and may have no exact counterpart elsewhere. The arts, from the sublime to the mediocre (and even the banal) can ...

  10. Tok essay

    Visual representations, such as graphs, diagrams, and maps, are commonly used in the human sciences to present data, illustrate concepts, and document the daily life of people and groups. In this essay, I will provide examples of visual representation in various subfields of human science, including psychology, sociology, anthropology ...

  11. IB TOK Essay examples

    Are visual representations always helpful in the communication of knowledge? Discuss with reference to the human sciences and mathematics. TOK Essay A. For artists and natural scientists, which is more important: what can be explained or what cannot be explained? ... TOK Essay: 5. "How can we distinguish between good and bad interpretations ...

  12. IB TOK VISUAL REPRESENTATION by Pachia Lee on Prezi

    Three Connections to my Educational Experiences with International Mindedness 1. My involvement with the Roosevelt Asian Student Association 2. Taking a second language as an elective (Spanish) IB Theory of Knowledge Visual Representation of TOK 3. My engagement in my History

  13. Are visual representations always helpful in the communication of

    This cannot be done without the use of visual representation, for an example while introducing right angle triangles, if the mathematician presents the claim that 'The angle BAC, together with the angles BCA and ABC are equal to two right angles, or twice the angle ABC.', (Euclid ix) the communication of this knowledge greatly fails at conveying its message.

  14. Are visual representations always helpful in the communication of

    These visual representations were used to tell stories and record experiences before the evolution of written language. Visual representations are often essential in the field of mathematics since they are used to convey complex concepts and skills to a variety of audiences, including students, teachers, researchers, and the public.

  15. The role of visual representations in scientific practices: from

    The use of visual representations (i.e., photographs, diagrams, models) has been part of science, and their use makes it possible for scientists to interact with and represent complex phenomena, not observable in other ways. Despite a wealth of research in science education on visual representations, the emphasis of such research has mainly been on the conceptual understanding when using ...

  16. TOK Presentation: Visual Representations of data

    Infogram. Data Visualization. Infographics. Charts. Blog. May 31, 2024. How to create and deliver a winning team presentation. May 24, 2024. What are AI writing tools and how can they help with making presentations?

  17. Data and information visualization

    Data visualization refers to the techniques used to communicate data or information by encoding it as visual objects (e.g., points, lines, or bars) contained in graphics. The goal is to communicate information clearly and efficiently to users. It is one of the steps in data analysis or data science. According to Vitaly Friedman (2008) the "main ...

  18. Archetypes, Stereotypes and Media Representation in a Multi-cultural

    In and of themselves, archetypes are valuable and necessary components of a story. However, it is when these archetypes are married to stereotypes that concerns arise for media management in a multi-cultural society. 3. Stereotypes An archetype is the basic character that moves a story forward, such as Prince Charming as the hero and Cinderella ...

  19. Chapter 1 Introduction to Visual Representations

    There are numerous situations in which we use visual representations to under-stand the various data. This could involve anything from last week's stock market trends to a travel itinerary or even the weather forecast for various geographical ar-eas. Thanks to our visual perception ability, a visual representation is often more

  20. ‎TOK Talk: Visual Representations: 2023 TOK Essay Title 5 on Apple Podcasts

    Today I enjoyed tea and Talked some TOK with Kevin Hoye (IB English Literature and TOK Teacher) about 2023 TOK Essay Title 5: Are visual representations always helpful in the communication of knowledge? Discuss with reference to the human sciences and mathematics. ... ‎Show TOK Talk, Ep Visual Representations: 2023 TOK Essay Title 5 - 25 Oct ...