Child Development

Since its inception in 1930,  Child Development  has been devoted to original contributions on topics in child development from the fetal period through adolescence. It is a vital source of information not only for researchers and theoreticians, but for a broad range of psychiatrists and psychologists, educators, and social workers across the field.  Child Development is published on a bimonthly basis.

: 4.6
: 5.9

Psychology, Developmental: 13 out of 76.5
Psychology, Educational: 7 out of 60

SRCD members receive full digital access  Child Development . If you are an SRCD member and would like to access the publication, please  log in to your SRCD member account . Not an SRCD member? Learn more about the benefits of membership .

Contact: [email protected]

View Issues Online

1990 – Present     1930-1989 (in JSTOR)     Articles Published Online to Early View     Special Sections

For Authors and Reviewers

SUBMIT or Review a Manuscript

  • Submission Guidelines
  • Author Guidelines on Scientific Integrity and Openness
  • Author Guidelines for Registered Reports
  • Reviewer Guidelines
  • Reviewer Guidelines for Registered Reports
  • Special Issues and Sections
  • Revised Manuscript Requirements
  • Request Permissions
  • Statement on Antiracism, Equity, and Inclusion in SRCD Publications

Statement on Anti-racism, Equity, and Inclusion in SRCD Publications

As our highly regarded journals are perhaps the most public-facing element and explicitly gatekeeping mechanism of the SRCD enterprise, we must critically examine the extent to which our journals, Editors and Editorial Boards, reviewers and review standards, and Publications Committee activities implement practices and policies grounded in inclusive and anti-racist principles. Read the full statement.

SRCD Generative Artificial Intelligence (AI) Policy.

Wiley Developmental Science Publishing Network

Child Development participates in the Wiley Developmental Science Publishing Network . This exciting collaboration between a number of high quality journals simplifies and speeds up the publication process, helping authors find a home for their research. At the Editors’ judgement, suitable papers not accepted by one journal may be recommended for referral to another journal(s) in the network. Authors decide whether to accept the referral, with the option to transfer their paper with or without revisions. Once the referral is accepted, submission happens automatically, along with any previous reviewer reports, thereby relieving pressure on the peer review process. While a transfer does not guarantee acceptance, it is more likely to lead to a successful outcome for authors by helping them to find a route to publication quickly and easily.

Please review the Child Development Author Guidelines  for journal submission details.

Current Calls for Child Development

Call for reviewers to serve as statistical consulting editors:.

Statistical Consulting Editors (SCEs) act as volunteer ‘super reviewers’ across the Child Development Editorial Board with a focus on statistical data. Similar to a regular reviewer, Statistical Consulting Editors receive invitations to review papers based on editor need, however they are also identified as a ‘go-to’ reviewer of statistical materials amongst editors and agree to handle ~10 papers annually.

Contact Interim Editor-in-Chief, Dr. Shauna Cooper ( [email protected] ),   and the SRCD Publications Office ( [email protected] )  today to learn more and volunteer your expertise as a Child Development Statistical Consulting Editor.

Square image with the Child Development Journal logo on a blue background. Interim Editor is Shauna M. Cooper from University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

Child Development Editorial Board

Meet the Associate editors meet the consulting editors  Meet the statistical consulting editors read the full editorial on the wiley online library

Advertisement

Advertisement

Theories of Child Development and Their Impact on Early Childhood Education and Care

  • Published: 29 October 2021
  • Volume 51 , pages 15–30, ( 2023 )

Cite this article

research paper child development

  • Olivia N. Saracho   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-4108-7790 1  

142k Accesses

16 Citations

Explore all metrics

Developmental theorists use their research to generate philosophies on children’s development. They organize and interpret data based on a scheme to develop their theory. A theory refers to a systematic statement of principles related to observed phenomena and their relationship to each other. A theory of child development looks at the children's growth and behavior and interprets it. It suggests elements in the child's genetic makeup and the environmental conditions that influence development and behavior and how these elements are related. Many developmental theories offer insights about how the performance of individuals is stimulated, sustained, directed, and encouraged. Psychologists have established several developmental theories. Many different competing theories exist, some dealing with only limited domains of development, and are continuously revised. This article describes the developmental theories and their founders who have had the greatest influence on the fields of child development, early childhood education, and care. The following sections discuss some influences on the individuals’ development, such as theories, theorists, theoretical conceptions, and specific principles. It focuses on five theories that have had the most impact: maturationist, constructivist, behavioral, psychoanalytic, and ecological. Each theory offers interpretations on the meaning of children's development and behavior. Although the theories are clustered collectively into schools of thought, they differ within each school.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

Explore related subjects

  • Medical Ethics

The author is grateful to Mary Jalongo for her expert editing and her keen eye for the smallest details.

Although Watson was the first to maintain explicitly that psychology was a natural science, behaviorism in both theory and practice had originated much earlier than 1913. Watson offered a vital incentive to behaviorism, but several others had started the process. He never stated to have created “behavioral psychology.” Some behaviorists consider him a model of the approach rather than an originator of behaviorism (Malone, 2014 ). Still, his presence has significantly influenced the status of present psychology and its development.

Alschuler, R., & Hattwick, L. (1947). Painting and personality . University of Chicago Press.

Google Scholar  

Axline, V. (1974). Play therapy . Ballentine Books.

Berk, L. (2021). Infants, children, and adolescents . Pearson.

Bijou, S. W. (1975). Development in the preschool years: A functional analysis. American Psychologist, 30 (8), 829–837. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0077069

Article   Google Scholar  

Bijou, S. W. (1977). Behavior analysis applied to early childhood education. In B. Spodek & H. J. Walberg (Eds.), Early childhood education: Issues and insights (pp. 138–156). McCutchan Publishing Corporation.

Boghossion, P. (2006). Behaviorism, constructivism, and Socratic pedagogy. Educational Philosophy and Theory, 38 (6), 713–722. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-5812.2006.00226.x

Bower, B. (1986). Skinner boxing. Science News, 129 (6), 92–94. https://doi.org/10.2307/3970364

Briner, M. (1999). Learning theories . University of Colorado.

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1974). Developmental research, public policy, and the ecology of childhood. Child Development, 45 (1), 1–5. https://doi.org/10.2307/1127743

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development . Harvard University Press.

Bruner, J. S. (1960). The process of education . Harvard University Press.

Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning . Harvard University Press.

Bruner, J. (2004). A short history of psychological theories of learning. Daedalus, 133 (1), 13–20. https://doi.org/10.1162/001152604772746657

Coles, R., Hunt, R., & Maher, B. (2002). Erik Erikson: Faculty of Arts and Sciences Memorial Minute. Harvard Gazette Archives . http://www.hno.harvard.edu/gazette/2002/03.07/22-memorialminute.html

Editors of Encyclopaedia Britannica. (2020). Erik Erikson . https://www.britannica.com/biography/Erik-Erikson

Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society . Norton.

Freud, A. (1935). Psychoanalysis for teachers and parents . Emerson Books.

Friedman, L. J. (1999). Identity’s architect: A biography of Erik H . Scribner Publishing Company.

Gesell, A. (1928). In infancy and human growth . Macmillan Co.

Book   Google Scholar  

Gesell, A. (1933). Maturation and the patterning of behavior. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A handbook of child psychology (pp. 209–235). Russell & Russell/Atheneum Publishers. https://doi.org/10.1037/11552-004

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Gesell, A., & Ilg, F. L. (1946). The child from five to ten . Harper & Row.

Gesell, A., Ilg, F. L., & Ames, L. B. (1978). Child behavior . Harper & Row.

Gesell, A., & Thompson, H. (1938). The psychology of early growth, including norms of infant behavior and a method of genetic analysis . Macmillan Co.

von Glasersfeld, E. (1995). Radical constructivism: A way of knowing and learning . Falmer.

von Glasersfeld, E. (2005). Introduction: Aspects of constructivism. In C. T. Fosnot (Ed.), Constructivism: Theory, perspectives and practice (pp. 3–7). Teachers College.

Graham, S., & Weiner, B. (1996). Theories and principles of motivation. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 63–84). Macmillan Library Reference.

Gray, P. O., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2017). Psychology (8th ed.). Worth Publishers.

Hilgard, E. R. (1987). Psychology in America: A historical survey . Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Hunt, J. . Mc. V. (1961). Intelligence and experience . Ronald Press.

Jenkins, E. W. (2000). Constructivism in school science education: Powerful model or the most dangerous intellectual tendency? Science and Education, 9 , 599–610. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008778120803

Jones, M. G., & Brader-Araje, L. (2002). The impact of constructivism on education: Language, discourse, and meaning. American Communication Studies, 5 (3), 1–1.

Kamii, C., & DeVries, R. (1978/1993.) Physical knowledge in preschool education: Implications of Piaget’s theory . Teachers College Press.

King, P. H. (1983). The life and work of Melanie Klein in the British Psycho-Analytical Society. The International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 64 (Pt 3), 251–260. PMID: 6352537.

Malone, J. C. (2014). Did John B. Watson really “Found” Behaviorism? The Behavior Analyst , 37 (1) ,  1–12. https://doi-org.proxy-um.researchport.umd.edu/10.1007/s40614-014-0004-3

Miller, P. H. (2016). Theories of developmental psychology (6th ed.). Worth Publishers.

Morphett, M. V., & Washburne, C. (1931). When should children begin to read? Elementary School Journal, 31 (7), 496–503. https://doi.org/10.1086/456609

Murphy, L. (1962). The widening world of childhood . Basic Books.

National Association for the Education of Young Children. (No date). Build your public policy knowledge/Head Start . https://www.naeyc.org/our-work/public-policy-advocacy/head-start

Reichling, L. (2017). The Skinner Box. Article Library. https://blog.customboxesnow.com/the-skinner-box/

Peters, E. M. (2015). Child developmental theories: A contrast overview. Retrieved from https://learningsupportservicesinc.wordpress.com/2015/11/20/child-developmental-theories-a-contrast-overview/

Piaget, J. (1963). The origins of intelligence in children . Norton.

Piaget, J. (1967/1971). Biology and knowledge: An essay on the relations between organic regulations and cognitive processes . Trans. B. Walsh. University of Chicago Press.

Safran, J. D., & Gardner-Schuster, E. (2016). Psychoanalysis. In H. S. Friedman (Ed.), Encyclopedia of mental health (2nd ed., pp. 339–347). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-397045-9.00189-0

Saracho, O. N. (2017). Literacy and language: New developments in research, theory, and practice. Early Child Development and Care, 187 (3–4), 299–304. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2017.1282235

Saracho, O. N. (2019). Motivation theories, theorists, and theoretical conceptions. In O. N. Saracho (Ed.), Contemporary perspectives on research in motivation in early childhood education (pp. 19–42). Information Age Publishing.

Saracho, O. N. (2020). An integrated play-based curriculum for young children. Routledge/Taylor and Francis Group . https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429440991

Saracho, O. N., & Evans, R. (2021). Theorists and their developmental theories. Early Child Development and Care, 191 (7–8), 993–1001.

Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development and individual differences. Child Development, 63 (1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130897

Schunk, D. (2021). Learning theories: An educational perspective (8th ed.). Pearson.

Shabani, K., Khatib, M., & Ebadi, S. (2010). Vygotsky’s zone of proximal development: Instructional implications and teachers’ professional development. English Language Teaching, 3 (4), 237–248.

Skinner, B. F. (1914). About behaviorism . Jonathan Cape Publishers.

Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis . D. Appleton-Century Co.

Skinner, B. F. (1953/2005). Science and human behavior . Macmillan. Later published by the B. F. Foundation in Cambridge, Massachusetts.

Spodek, B., & Saracho, O. N. (1994). Right from the start: Teaching children ages three to eight . Allyn & Bacon.

Steiner, J. (2017). Lectures on technique by Melanie Klein: Edited with critical review by John Steiner (1st ed.). Routledge.

Strickland, C. E., & Burgess, C. (1965). Health, growth and heredity: G. Stanley Hall on natural education . Teachers College Press.

Thorndike, E. L. (1906). The principles of teaching . A. G. Seiler.

Torre, D. M., Daley, B. J., Sebastian, J. L., & Elnicki, D. M. (2006). Overview of current learning theories for medical educators. The American Journal of Medicine, 119 (10), 903–907. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amjmed.2006.06.037

Vygotsky, L. S. (1934/1962). Thought and language . The MIT Press. (Original work published in 1934).

Vygotsky, L. S. (1971). Psychology of art . The MIT Press.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes . Harvard University Press.

Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20 (2), 158–177. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0074428

Weber, E. (1984). Ideas influencing early childhood education: A theoretical analysis . Teachers College Press.

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

University of Maryland, College Park, USA

Olivia N. Saracho

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Olivia N. Saracho .

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Saracho, O.N. Theories of Child Development and Their Impact on Early Childhood Education and Care. Early Childhood Educ J 51 , 15–30 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-021-01271-5

Download citation

Accepted : 22 September 2021

Published : 29 October 2021

Issue Date : January 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-021-01271-5

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Child development
  • Early childhood education
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

Warning: The NCBI web site requires JavaScript to function. more...

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it's official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you're on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Browse Titles

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

Committee on the Science of Children Birth to Age 8: Deepening and Broadening the Foundation for Success; Board on Children, Youth, and Families; Institute of Medicine; National Research Council; Allen LR, Kelly BB, editors. Transforming the Workforce for Children Birth Through Age 8: A Unifying Foundation. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US); 2015 Jul 23.

Cover of Transforming the Workforce for Children Birth Through Age 8

Transforming the Workforce for Children Birth Through Age 8: A Unifying Foundation.

  • Hardcopy Version at National Academies Press

4 Child Development and Early Learning

The domains of child development and early learning are discussed in different terms and categorized in different ways in the various fields and disciplines that are involved in research, practice, and policy related to children from birth through age 8. To organize the discussion in this report, the committee elected to use the approach and overarching terms depicted in Figure 4-1 . The committee does not intend to present this as a single best set of terms or a single best categorical organization. Indeed, it is essential to recognize that the domains shown in Figure 4-1 are not easily separable and that a case can be made for multiple different categorizations. For example, different disciplines and researchers have categorized different general cognitive processes under the categorical term “executive function.” General cognitive processes also relate to learning competencies such as persistence and engagement. Similarly, self-regulation has both cognitive and emotional dimensions. It is sometimes categorized as a part of executive function, as a part of socioemotional competence, or as a part of learning competencies. Attention and memory could be considered a part of general cognitive processes, as embedded within executive function, or linked to learning competencies related to persistence. Mental health is closely linked to socioemotional competence, but is also inseparable from health.

Report's organizational approach for the domains of child development and early learning.

The challenge of cleanly separating these concepts highlights a key attribute of all of these domains, which is that they do not develop or operate in isolation. Each enables and mutually supports learning and development in the others. Therefore, the importance of the interactions among the domains is emphasized throughout this chapter. For example, socioemotional competence is important for self-regulation, as are certain cognitive skills, and both emotional and cognitive self-regulation are important for children to be able to exercise learning competencies. Similarly, although certain skills and concept knowledge are distinct to developing proficiency in particular subject areas, learning in these subject areas also both requires and supports general cognitive skills such as reasoning and attention, as well as learning competencies and socioemotional competence. In an overarching example of interactions, a child's security both physically and in relationships creates the context in which learning is most achievable across all of the domains.

It is less important that all fields of research, practice, and policy adhere to the exact same categorizations, and more important that all conduct their work in a way that is cognizant and inclusive of all the elements that contribute to child development and early learning, and that all fields recognize that they are interactive and mutually reinforcing rather than hierarchical. This point foreshadows a theme that is addressed more fully in subsequent chapters. Because different fields and sectors may not use the same categorizations and vocabulary for these domains and skills, developing practices and policies that support more consistent and continuous development and early learning across birth through age 8 will require a concerted effort to communicate clearly and come to a mutual understanding of the goals for children. To communicate across fields and between research and practice communities requires being aware of the different categorical frameworks and terms that are used and being able to discuss the various concepts and content—and their implications—with clarity across those different frameworks. Practitioners and policy makers will be aided in achieving greater precision and clarity in their actions and decisions if those conducting and communicating future research keep this challenge in mind across domains, especially in those cases in which the taxonomy is most variable (e.g., self-regulation, executive function, general learning competencies).

With these caveats in mind, the remainder of this chapter addresses in turn the domains of child development and early learning depicted in Figure 4-1 : cognitive development, including learning of specific subjects; general learning competencies; socioemotional development; and physical development and health. The final section examines a key overarching issue: the effects on child development and early learning of the stress and adversity that is also an important theme in the discussion of the interaction between biology and environment in Chapter 3 .

  • COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

This section highlights what is known about cognitive development in young children. It begins with key concepts from research viewpoints that have contributed to recent advances in understanding of the developing mind, and then presents the implications of this knowledge for early care and education settings. The following section addresses the learning of specific subjects, with a focus on language and mathematics.

Studies of early cognitive development have led researchers to understand the developing mind as astonishingly competent, active, and insightful from a very early age. For example, infants engage in an intuitive analysis of the statistical regularities in the speech sounds they hear en route to constructing language ( Saffran, 2003 ). Infants and toddlers derive implicit theories to explain the actions of objects and the behavior of people; these theories form the foundation for causal learning and more sophisticated understanding of the physical and social worlds. Infants and young children also are keenly responsive to what they can learn from the actions and words directed to them by other people. This capacity for joint attention may be the foundation that enables humans to benefit from culturally transmitted knowledge ( Tomasello et al., 2005 ). Infants respond to cues conveying the communicative intentions of an adult (such as eye contact and infant-directed speech) and tune in to what the adult is referring to and what can be learned about it. This “natural pedagogy” ( Csibra, 2010 ; Csibra and Gergely, 2009 ) becomes more sophisticated in the sensitivity of preschoolers to implicit pedagogical guides in adult speech directed to them ( Butler and Markman, 2012a , b , 2014 ). Young children rely so much on what they learn from others that they become astute, by the preschool years, in distinguishing adult speakers who are likely to provide them with reliable information from those who are not ( Harris, 2012 ; Jaswal, 2010 ; Koenig and Doebel, 2013 ). This connection of relationships and social interactions to cognitive development is consistent with how the brain develops and how the mind grows, and is a theme throughout this chapter.

Much of what current research shows is going on in young children's minds is not transparent in their behavior. Infants and young children may not show what they know because of competing demands on their attention, limitations in what they can do, and immature self-regulation. This is one of the reasons why developmental scientists use carefully designed experiments for elucidating what young children know and understand about the world. By designing research procedures that eliminate competing distractions and rely on simple responses (such as looking time and expressions of surprise), researchers seek to uncover cognitive processes that might otherwise be more difficult to see. Evidence derived in this experimental manner, such as the examples in the sections that follow, can be helpful in explaining young children's rapid growth in language learning, imitation, problem solving, and other skills.

Implicit Theories

One of the most important discoveries about the developing mind is how early and significantly very young children, even starting in infancy, are uniting disparate observations or discrete facts into coherent conceptual systems ( Carey, 2009 ; Gopnik and Wellman, 2012 ; Spelke and Kinzler, 2007 ). From very early on, children are not simply passive observers, registering the superficial appearance of things. Rather, they are building explanatory systems—implicit theories—that organize their knowledge. Such implicit theories contain causal principles and causal relations; these theories enable children to predict, explain, and reason about relevant phenomena and, in some cases, intervene to change them. As early as the first year of life, babies are developing incipient theories about how the world of people, other living things, objects, and numbers operates. It is important to point out that these foundational theories are not simply isolated forms of knowledge, but play a profound role in children's everyday lives and subsequent education.

One major example of an implicit theory that is already developing as early as infancy is “theory of mind,” which refers to the conceptual framework people use to reason about the mental lives of others as well as themselves. This example is discussed in detail below. Some additional illustrative examples of the development of implicit theories are provided in Box 4-1 .

Examples of the Development of Implicit Theories. Even babies hold some fundamental principles about how objects move about in space and time (Baillargeon et al., 2009). For example, babies are surprised (as measured by their increased looking time) if (more...)

Theory of Mind

People intuitively understand others' actions as motivated by desires, goals, feelings, intentions, thoughts, and other mental states, and we understand how these mental states affect one another (for example, an unfulfilled desire can evoke negative feelings and a motivation to continue trying to achieve the goal). One remarkable discovery of research on young children is that they are developing their own intuitive “map” of mental processes like these from very early in life ( Baillargeon et al., 2010 ; Saxe, 2013 ; Wellman and Woolley, 1990 ). Children's developing theory of mind transforms how they respond to people and what they learn from them. Infants and young children are beginning to understand what goes on in people's minds, and how others' feelings and thoughts are similar to and different from their own.

Infants first have a relatively simple theory of mind. They are aware of some basic characteristics: what people are looking at is a sign of what they are paying attention to; people act intentionally and are goal directed; people have positive and negative feelings in response to things around them; and people have different perceptions, goals, and feelings. Children add to this mental map as their awareness grows. From infancy on, developing theory of mind permeates everyday social interactions—affecting what and how children learn, how they react to and interact with other people, how they assess the fairness of an action, and how they evaluate themselves.

One-year-olds, for example, will look in their mother's direction when faced with someone or something unfamiliar to “read” mother's expression and determine whether this is a dangerous or benign unfamiliarity. Infants also detect when an adult makes eye contact, speaks in an infant-directed manner (such as using higher pitch and melodic intonations), and responds contingently to the infant's behavior. Under these circumstances, infants are especially attentive to what the adult says and does, thus devoting special attention to social situations in which the adult's intentions are likely to represent learning opportunities.

Other examples also illustrate how a developing theory of mind underlies children's emerging understanding of the intentions of others. Take imitation, for example. It is well established that babies and young children imitate the actions of others. Children as young as 14 to 18 months are often imitating not the literal observed action but the action they thought the actor intended—the goal or the rationale behind the action ( Gergely et al., 2002 ; Meltzoff, 1995 ). Word learning is another example in which babies' reasoning based on theory of mind plays a crucial role. By at least 15 months old, when babies hear an adult label an object, they take the speaker's intent into account by checking the speaker's focus of attention and deciding whether they think the adult indicated the object intentionally. Only when babies have evidence that the speaker intended to refer to a particular object with a label will they learn that word ( Baldwin, 1991 ; Baldwin and Moses, 2001 ; Baldwin and Tomasello, 1998 ).

Babies also can perceive the unfulfilled goals of others and intervene to help them; this is called “shared intentionality.” Babies as young as 14 months old who witness an adult struggling to reach for an object will interrupt their play to crawl over and hand the object to the adult ( Warneken and Tomasello, 2007 ). By the time they are 18 months old, shared intentionality enables toddlers to act helpfully in a variety of situations; for example, they pick up dropped objects for adults who indicate that they need assistance (but not for adults who dropped the object intentionally) ( Warneken and Tomasello, 2006 ). Developing an understanding of others' goals and preferences and how to facilitate them affects how young children interpret the behavior of people they observe and provides a basis for developing a sense of helpful versus undesirable human activity that is a foundation for later development of moral understanding (cf. Bloom, 2013 ; Hamlin et al., 2007 ; Thompson, 2012 , 2015 ).

Developing Implicit Theories: Implications for Adults

The research on the development of implicit theories in children has important implications for how adults work with and educate young children. Failure to recognize the extent to which they are construing information in terms of their lay theories can result in educational strategies that oversimplify material for children. Educational materials guided by the assumption that young children are “concrete” thinkers—that they cannot deal with abstraction or reason hypothetically—leads educators to focus on simple, descriptive activities that can deprive children of opportunities to advance their conceptual frameworks. Designing effective materials in a given domain or subject matter requires knowing what implicit theories children hold, what core causal principles they use, and what misconceptions and gaps in knowledge they have, and then using empirically validated steps to help lead them to a more accurate, more advanced conceptual framework.

Statistical Learning

Statistical learning refers to the range of ways in which children, even babies, are implicitly sensitive to the statistical regularities in their environment, although they are not explicitly learning or applying statistics. Like the development of implicit theories, this concept of statistical learning counters the possible misconception of babies as passive learners and bears on the vital importance of their having opportunities to observe and interact with the environment. Several examples of statistical learning are provided in Box 4-2 .

Examples of Statistical Learning. Infants can use information about the statistics of syllables in the speech they hear to help them parse words. How do we know from hearing prettybaby that baby is more likely to be a word than tyba ? One way is that the (more...)

Understanding Causal Inference

Children's intuitive understanding of causal inference has long been recognized as a fundamental component of conceptual development. Young children, although not explicitly or consciously experimenting with causality, can experience observations and learning that allow them to conclude that a particular variable X causes (or prevents) an effect Y. Recent advances in the field have documented the ways young children can implicitly use the statistics of how events covary to infer causal relations, make predictions, generate explanations, guide their exploration, and enable them to intervene in the environment. The understanding of causal inference also provides an example of how different cognitive abilities—such as a sensitivity to statistical regularities and the development of implicit theories based on observation and learning (discussed in the two preceding sections and Box 4-2 )—interact with and can mutually support each other. There is now a substantial literature on young children's implicit ability to use what they observe in different conditions to understand the relations between variables. Several examples of young children developing the ability to understand causal inference are provided in Box 4-3 .

Examples of Understanding Causal Inference. One of the first studies of children's understanding of causal inference showed that children can rule out one variable and isolate another (Gopnik et al., 2001). Preschool children were presented with a machine (more...)

Sensitivity to Teaching Cues

Csibra and Gergely (2009) argue that humans are equipped with a capacity to realize when someone is communicating something for their benefit and that they construe that information differently than when they merely witness it. As noted previously in the discussion of developing theory of mind, children as early as infancy devote special attention to social situations that are likely to represent learning opportunities because adults communicate that intention. Information learned in such communicative contexts is treated as more generalizable and robust than that learned in a noncommunicative context.

In one study, for example, 9-month-old babies saw an adult either reach for an object (a noncommunicative act) or point to an object (a communicative act). The entire display was then screened from view, and after a brief delay, the curtains were opened, and babies saw either the same object in a new location or a new object in the same location. The short delay imposed a memory requirement, and for babies this young, encoding both the location and the identity of the object taxes their memory. The location of the object will typically be more salient and memorable to babies than the object's properties, but the prediction of this study was that babies who saw the adult point to the object would construe the pointing as a communicative act—“this adult is showing me something”—and would thus be more likely to encode the properties as opposed to the location of the object. Babies' looking times served as a measure of their surprise at or interest in an unexpected event. As predicted, babies appeared to encode different aspects of the event in the different conditions. When they had previously witnessed the adult reaching for the object, they were surprised when the object was in a new location but showed no renewed interest when there was a different object in the old location. In contrast, when babies first saw an adult point to the object, they were surprised when a new object appeared in the old location but not when the old object had changed locations ( Yoon et al., 2008 ).

Infants' Sensitivity to Teaching Cues: Implications for Adults

Babies have the capacity to realize when someone is communicating something for their benefit and therefore to construe information differently than when they merely witness it. When adults use face-to-face contact, call a baby's name, and point for the baby's benefit, these signals lead babies to recognize that someone is teaching them something, and this awareness can affect how and what they learn.

The significance of eye contact and other communication cues also is evident in research on whether, how, and when young children learn from video and other forms of digital media. Experiments conducted with 24-month-olds, for example, revealed that they can learn from a person on a video screen if that person is communicating with them through a webcam-like environment, but they showed no evidence of learning from a prerecorded video of that person. The webcam environment included social cues, such as back-and-forth conversation and other forms of social contact that are not possible in prerecorded video. Other studies found that toddlers learned verbs better during Skype video chats than during prerecorded video chats that did not allow for authentic eye contact or back-and-forth interaction ( Roseberry et al., 2014 ; Troseth et al., 2006 ). (See also Chapter 6 for more on technology and learning.)

The benefits of communicative pedagogical contexts for the conceptual development of preschool children also have been investigated. In one set of studies, 4-year-old children were exposed to a novel object's function either by seeing an adult deliberately use the object or by seeing the adult deliberately use the object after maintaining eye contact with the child and saying “watch this.” In both conditions, children noticed the object's property and attempted to elicit it from other similar objects. But when those objects were doctored to be nonfunctional, the children in the nonpedagogical condition quickly abandoned their attempts to elicit the property and played with the objects in some other way. Children who saw the same evidence but with direct communication for their benefit persisted in trying to elicit the property from other objects ( Butler and Markman, 2012a , b ). In other words, children's conviction that other similar objects should have the same unforeseen property was bolstered by their belief that the adult was performing the function for their benefit. Moreover the intentional (but nonpedagogical) condition versus the pedagogical condition produced strikingly different conceptions of the function ( Butler and Markman, 2014 ). Four- and 5-year-old children witnessed an object's function and were then given a set of objects to play with. Some objects were identical in appearance to the first object, while some differed in color (in one study) or shape (in another). Half of the objects of each color (or shape) had the unforeseen property, and half did not. Children were told they could play with the objects for a while and then should put them away in their appropriate boxes when done. The goal was to see whether children would sort the objects by the salient perceptual property (color or shape) or by function. Children in the pedagogical condition viewed the function as definitive and classified the objects by systematically testing each to see whether it had the function, while children in the nonpedagogical condition sorted by the salient color or shape. Thus, identical evidence is construed differently when children believe it has been produced for their benefit.

Effects of Adult Language on Cognition

Understanding the power of language is important for people who interact with children. Simple labels can help children unify disparate-looking things into coherent categories; thus labeling is a powerful way to foster conceptual development. Labels also can reify categories or concepts in ways that may or may not be intended. For example, frequently hearing “boys and girls” line up for recess, quiet down, etc. implicitly reinforces gender as an important dimension, compared with saying “children.” Box 4-4 presents examples of linguistic distinctions that affect children's construction of conceptual systems.

Examples of the Effects of Adult Language on Cognition. Some kinds of categories—two round balls, for example—are fairly easy to form, such that even babies treat the objects as similar. But many objects that adults view as members of (more...)

Effects of Language Used by Adults on Children's Cognitive Development: Implications for Adults

Awareness of the benefits and pitfalls of the language used by adults is important for people who interact with children. The language used by adults affects cognitive growth and learning in children in many subtle ways. Labeling is a powerful way to foster conceptual development. Simple labels can help children unify disparate things into coherent categories, but can also have the unintended consequence of reinforcing categories or concepts that are not desirable.

Conclusions About Cognitive Development and Early Learning Learning begins prenatally, and children are not only “ready to learn” but already actively learning from the time they are born. From birth, children's minds are active and inquisitive, and early thinking is insightful and complex. Many of the foundations of sophisticated forms of learning, including those important to academic success, are established in the earliest years of life. Development and early learning can be supported continuously as a child develops, and early knowledge and skills inform and influence future learning. When adults understand how the mind develops, what progress children make in their cognitive abilities, and how active inquiry and learning are children's natural inclination, they can foster cognitive growth by supporting children's active engagement with new experiences and providing developmentally appropriate stimulation of new learning through responsive, secure, and sustained caregiving relationships.

Implications for Care and Education Settings and Practitioners

The research findings on cognitive development in young children summarized above reflect an evolving understanding of how the mind develops during the early years and should be part of the core knowledge that influences how care and education professionals support young children's learning, as discussed in Chapter 7 . Many of these concepts describe cognitive processes that are implicit. By contrast with the explicit knowledge that older children and adults can put into words, implicit knowledge is tacit or nonconscious understanding that cannot readily be consciously described (see, e.g., Mandler, 2004 ). Examples of implicit knowledge in very young children include many of the early achievements discussed above, such as their implicit theories of living things and of the human mind and their nonconscious awareness of the statistical frequency of the associations among speech sounds in the language they are hearing. Infants' and young children's “statistical learning” does not mean that they can count, nor are their “implicit theories” consciously worked out. Not all early learning is implicit, of course. Very young children are taking significant strides in their explicit knowledge of language, the functioning of objects, and the characteristics of people and animals in the world around them. Thus early learning occurs on two levels: the growth of knowledge that is visible and apparent, and the growth of implicit understanding that is sometimes more difficult to observe.

This distinction between implicit and explicit learning can be confusing to early childhood practitioners (and parents), who often do not observe or recognize evidence for the sophisticated implicit learning—or even the explicit learning—taking place in the young children in their care. Many of the astonishingly competent, active, and insightful things that research on early cognitive development shows are going on in young children's minds are not transparent in their behavior. Instead, toddlers and young children seem highly distractable, emotional, and not very capable of managing their impulses. All of these observations about young children are true, but at the same time, their astonishing growth in language skills, their very different ways of interacting with objects and living things, and their efforts to share attention (such as through pointing) or goals (such as through helping) with an adult suggest that the cognitive achievements demonstrated in experimental settings have relevance to their everyday behavior.

This point is especially important because the cognitive abilities of young children are so easily underestimated. In the past, for example, the prevalent belief that infants lack conceptual knowledge meant that parents and practitioners missed opportunities to explore with them cause and effect, number, or symbolic play. Similarly, the view that young children are egocentric caused many adults to conclude that there was little benefit to talking about people's feelings until children were older—this despite the fact that most people could see how attentive young children were to others' emotions and how curious about their causes.

In light of these observations, how do early educators contribute to the cognitive growth of children in their first 3 years? One way is by providing appropriate support for the learning that is occurring in these very young children (see, e.g., Copple et al., 2013 ). Using an abundance of child-directed language during social interaction, playing counting games (e.g., while stacking blocks), putting into words what a classroom pet can do or why somebody looks sad, exploring together what happens when objects collide, engaging in imitative play and categorization (sorting) games—these and other shared activities can be cognitively provocative as long as they remain within the young child's capacities for interest and attention. They also build on understandings that young children are implicitly developing related to language; number; object characteristics; and implicit theories of animate and inanimate objects, physical causality, and people's minds. The purpose of these and other activities is not just to provide young children with cognitive stimulation, but also to embed that stimulation in social interaction that provokes young children's interest, elicits their curiosity, and provides an emotional context that enables them to focus their thinking on new discoveries. The central and consistent feature of all these activities is the young child's shared activity with an adult who thoughtfully capitalizes on his or her interests to provoke cognitive growth. The implications for instructional practices and curricula for educators working with infants and toddlers are discussed further in Chapter 6 .

Another way that educators contribute to the cognitive growth of infants and toddlers is through the emotional support they provide ( Jamison et al., 2014 ). Emotional support is afforded by the educator's responsiveness to young children's interests and needs (including each child's individual temperament), the educator's development of warm relationships with children, and the educator's accessibility to help when young children are exploring on their own or interacting with other children ( Thompson, 2006 ). Emotional support of this kind is important not only as a positive accompaniment to the task of learning but also as an essential prerequisite to the cognitive and attentional engagement necessary for young children to benefit from learning opportunities. Because early capacities to self-regulate emotion are so limited, a young child's frustration or distress can easily derail cognitive engagement in new discoveries, and children can lose focus because their attentional self-regulatory skills are comparably limited. An educator's emotional support can help keep young children focused and persistent, and can also increase the likelihood that early learning experiences will yield successful outcomes. Moreover, the secure attachments that young children develop with educators contribute to an expectation of adult support that enables young children to approach learning opportunities more positively and confidently. Emotional support and socioemotional development are discussed further later in this chapter.

The characteristics of early learning call for specific curricular approaches and thoughtful professional learning for educators, but it is also true that less formal opportunities to stimulate early cognitive growth emerge naturally in children's everyday interactions with a responsive adult. Consider, for example, a parent or other caregiver interacting with a 1-year-old over a shape-sorting toy. As they together are choosing shapes of different colors and the child is placing them in the appropriate (or inappropriate) cutout in the bin, the adult can accompany this task with language that describes what they are doing and why, and narrates the child's experiences of puzzlement, experimentation, and accomplishment. The adult may also be using number words to count the blocks as they are deposited. The baby's attention is focused by the constellation of adult behavior—infant-directed language, eye contact, and responsiveness—that signals the adult's teaching, and this “pedagogical orientation” helps focus the young child's attention and involvement. The back-and-forth interaction of child and adult activity provides stimulus for the baby's developing awareness of the adult's thinking (e.g., she looks at each block before commenting on it or acting intentionally on it) and use of language (e.g., colors are identified for each block, and generic language is used to describe blocks in general). In this interaction, moreover, the baby is developing both expectations for what this adult is like—safe, positive, responsive—and skills for social interaction (such as turn taking). Although these qualities and the learning derived from them are natural accompaniments to child-focused responsive social interaction with an adult caregiver, the caregiver's awareness of the child's cognitive growth at this time contributes significantly to the adult's ability to intentionally support new discovery and learning.

As children further develop cognitively as preschoolers, their growth calls for both similar and different behavior by the adults who work with them. While the educator's emotional support and responsiveness remain important, children from age 3 to 5 years become different kinds of thinkers than they were as infants and toddlers ( NRC, 2001 ). First, they are more consciously aware of their knowledge—much more of their understanding is now explicit. This means they are more capable of deliberately enlisting what they know into new learning situations, although they are not yet as competent or strategic in doing so as they will be in the primary grades. When faced with a problem or asked a question, they are more capable of offering an answer based on what they know, even when their knowledge is limited. Second, preschoolers are more competent in learning from their deliberate efforts to do so, such as trial-and-error or informal experimentation. While their success in this regard pales by comparison with the more strategic efforts of a grade-schooler, their “let's find out” approach to new challenges reflects their greater behavioral and mental competence in figuring things out. Third, preschoolers also are intuitive and experiential, learning by doing rather than figuring things out “in the head.” This makes shared activities with educators and peers potent opportunities for cognitive growth.

Nonetheless, the potential to underestimate the cognitive abilities of young children persists in the preschool and kindergarten years. In one study, for example, children's actual performance was six to eight times what was estimated by their own preschool teachers and other experts in consulting, teacher education, educational research, and educational development ( Claessens et al., 2014 ; Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, 1996 ). Such underestimation represents a lost opportunity that can hinder children's progress. A study in kindergarten revealed that teachers spent most of their time in basic content that children already knew, yet the children benefited more from advanced reading and mathematics content ( Claessens et al., 2014 )—an issue discussed in depth in Chapter 6 . Unfortunately, when care and education professionals underestimate children's abilities to understand and learn subject-matter content, the negative impact is greatest on those with the fewest prior learning experiences ( Bennett et al., 1984 ; Clements and Sarama, 2014 ).

Conversely, when educators practice in a way that is cognizant of the cognitive progress of children at this age, they can more deliberately enlist the preschool child's existing knowledge and skills into new learning situations. One example is interactive storybook reading, in which children describe the pictures and label their elements while the adult and child ask and answer questions of each other about the narrative. Language and literacy skills also are fostered at this age by the adult's use of varied vocabulary in interaction with the child, as well as by extending conversation on a single topic (rather than frequently switching topics), asking open-ended questions of the child, and initiating conversation related to the child's experiences and interests ( Dickinson, 2003 ; Dickinson and Porche, 2011 ; Dickinson and Tabors, 2001 ). In each case, dialogic conversation about text or experience draws on while also extending children's prior knowledge and language skills. Language and literacy skills are discussed further in a subsequent section of this chapter, as well as in Chapter 6 .

Another implication of these cognitive changes is that educators can engage preschool children's intentional activity in new learning opportunities. Children's interest in learning by doing is naturally suited to experimental inquiry related to science or other kinds of inquiry-based learning involving hypothesis and testing, especially in light of the implicit theories of living things and physical causality that children bring to such inquiry ( Samarapungavan et al., 2011 ). In a similar manner, board games can provide a basis for learning and extending number concepts. In several experimental demonstrations, when preschool children played number board games specifically designed to foster their mental representations of numerical quantities, they showed improvements in number line estimates, count-on skill, numerical identification, and other important quantitative concepts ( Laski and Siegler, 2014 ).

Other research has shown that instructional strategies that promote higher-level thinking, creativity, and even abstract understanding, such as talking about ideas or about future events, is associated with greater cognitive achievement by preschool-age children (e.g., Diamond et al., 2013 ; Mashburn et al., 2008 ). For example, when educators point out how cardinal numbers can be used to describe diverse sets of elements (four blocks, four children, 4 o'clock), it helps them generalize an abstract concept (“fourness”) that describes a set rather than the characteristics of each element alone. These activities also can be integrated into other instructional practices during a typical day.

Another implication of the changes in young children's thinking during the preschool years concerns the motivational features of early learning. Preschool-age children are developing a sense of themselves and their competencies, including their academic skills ( Marsh et al., 1998 , 2002 ). Their beliefs about their abilities in reading, counting, vocabulary, number games, and other academic competencies derive from several sources, including spontaneous social comparison with other children and feedback from teachers (and parents) concerning their achievement and the reasons they have done well or poorly. These beliefs influence, in turn, children's self-confidence, persistence, intrinsic motivation to succeed, and other characteristics that may be described as learning skills (and are discussed more extensively later in this chapter). Consequently, how teachers provide performance feedback to young children and support for their self-confidence in learning situations also is an important predictor of children's academic success ( Hamre, 2014 ).

In the early elementary years, children's cognitive processes develop further, which accordingly influences the strategies for educators in early elementary classrooms. Primary grade children are using more complex vocabulary and grammar. They are growing in their ability to make mental representations, but they still have difficulty grasping abstract concepts without the aid of real-life references and materials ( Tomlinson, 2014 ). This is a critical time for children to develop confidence in all areas of life. Children at this age show more independence from parents and family, while friendship, being liked and accepted by peers, becomes more important. Being in school most of the day means greater contact with a larger world, and children begin to develop a greater understanding of their place in that world ( CDC, 2014 ).

Children's growing ability to self-regulate their emotions also is evident in this period (discussed more extensively later in this chapter). Children understand their own feelings more and more, and learn better ways to describe experiences and express thoughts and feelings. They better understand the consequences of their actions, and their focus on concern for others grows. They are very observant, are willing to play cooperatively and work in teams, and can resolve some conflicts without seeking adult intervention ( CDC, 2014 ). Children also come to understand that they can affect others' perception of their emotions by changing their affective displays ( Aloise-Young, 1993 ). Children who are unable to self-regulate have emotional difficulties that may interfere with their learning. Just as with younger children, significant adults in a child's life can help the child learn to self-regulate ( Tomlinson, 2014 ).

Children's increasing self-regulation means they have a greater ability to follow instructions independently in a manner that would not be true of preschool or younger children. Educators can rely on the growing cognitive abilities in elementary school children in using instructional approaches that depend more independently on children's own discoveries, their use of alternative inquiry strategies, and their greater persistence in problem solving. Educators in these settings are scaffolding the skills that began to develop earlier, so that children are able to gradually apply those skills with less and less external support. This serves as a bridge to succeeding in upper primary grades, so if students lack necessary knowledge and skills in any domain of development and learning, their experience during the early elementary grades is crucial in helping them gain those competencies.

Building on many of the themes that have emerged from this discussion, the following sections continue by looking in more depth at cognitive development with respect to learning specific subjects and then at other major elements of development, including general learning competencies, socioemotional development, and physical development and health.

  • LEARNING SPECIFIC SUBJECTS

Interrelationships among different kinds of skills and abilities contribute to young children's acquisition of content knowledge and competencies, which form a foundation for later academic success. These skills and abilities include the general cognitive development discussed above, the general learning competencies that allow children to control their own attention and thinking; and the emotion regulation that allows children to control their own emotions and participate in classroom activities in a productive way (the latter two are discussed in sections later in this chapter). Still another important category of skills and abilities, the focus of this section, is subject-matter content knowledge and skills, such as competencies needed specifically for learning language and literacy or mathematics.

Content knowledge and skills are acquired through a developmental process. As children learn about a topic, they progress through increasingly sophisticated levels of thinking with accompanying cognitive components. These developmental learning paths can be used as the core of a learning trajectory through which students can be supported by educators who understand both the content and those levels of thinking. Each learning trajectory has three parts: a goal (to develop a certain competence in a topic), a developmental progression (children constructing each level of thinking in turn), and instructional activities (tasks and teaching practices designed to enable thinking at each higher level). Learning trajectories also promote the learning of skills and concepts together—an effective approach that leads to both mastery and more fluent, flexible use of skills, as well as to superior conceptual understanding ( Fuson and Kwon, 1992 ; National Mathematics Advisory Panel, 2008 ). See Chapter 6 for additional discussion of using learning trajectories and other instructional practices.

Every subject area requires specific content knowledge and skills that are acquired through developmental learning processes. It is not possible to cover the specifics here for every subject area a young child learns. To maintain a feasible scope, this chapter covers two core subject areas: (1) language and literacy and (2) mathematics. This scope is not meant to imply that learning in other areas, such as science, engineering, social studies, or the arts, is unimportant or less subject specific. Rather, these two were selected because they are foundational for other subject areas and for later academic achievement, and because how they are learned has been well studied in young children compared with many other subject areas.

Language and Literacy

Children's language development and literacy development are central to each other. The development of language and literacy includes knowledge and skills in such areas as vocabulary, syntax, grammar, phonological awareness, writing, reading, comprehension, and discourse skills. The following sections address the development of language and literacy skills, including the relationship between the two; the role of the language-learning environment; socioeconomic disparities in early language environments; and language and literacy development in dual language learners.

Development of Oral Language Skills

Language skills build in a developmental progression over time as children increase their vocabulary, average sentence length, complexity and sophistication of sentence structure and grammar, and ability to express new ideas through words ( Kipping et al., 2012 ). Catts and Kamhi (1999) define five features of language that both work independently and interact as children develop language skills: phonology (speech sounds of language), semantics (meanings of words and phrases), morphology (meaningful parts of words and word tenses), syntax (rules for combining and ordering words in phrases), and pragmatics (appropriate use of language in context). The first three parameters combined (phonology, semantics, and morphology) enable listening and speaking vocabulary to develop, and they also contribute to the ability to read individual words. All five features of language contribute to the ability to understand sentences, whether heard or read (O' Connor, 2014 ). Thus, while children's development of listening and speaking abilities are important in their own right, oral language development also contributes to reading skills.

Developing oral communication skills are closely linked to the interactions and social bonds between adults and children. As discussed earlier in this chapter, parents' and caregivers' talk with infants stimulates—and affects—language comprehension long before children utter their first words. This comprehension begins with pragmatics—the social aspects of language that include facial and body language as well as words, such that infants recognize positive (and negative) interactions. Semantics (understanding meanings of words and clusters of words that are related) soon follows, in which toddlers link objects and their attributes to words. Between the ages of 2 and 4, most children show dramatic growth in language, particularly in understanding the meanings of words, their interrelationships, and grammatical forms ( Scarborough, 2001 ).

Karmiloff and Karmiloff-Smith (2001) suggest that children build webs among words with similar semantics, which leads to broader generalizations among classes of related words. When adults are responsive to children's questions and new experiences, children expand their knowledge of words and the relationships among them. Then, as new words arise from conversation, storytelling, and book reading, these words are linked to existing webs to further expand the store of words children understand through receptive language and use in their own conversation. The more often adults use particular words in conversation with young children, the sooner children will use those words in their own speech ( Karmiloff and Karmiloff-Smith, 2001 ). Research has linked the size of vocabulary of 2-year-olds to their reading comprehension through fifth grade ( Lee, 2011 ).

One of the best-documented methods for improving children's vocabularies is interactive storybook reading between children and their caregivers (O' Connor, 2014 ). Conversations as stories are read improve children's vocabulary ( Hindman et al., 2008 ; Weizman and Snow, 2001 ), especially when children are encouraged to build on the possibilities of storybooks by following their interests ( Whitehurst et al., 1988 ; Zucker et al., 2013 ). Book reading stimulates conversation outside the immediate context—for example, children ask questions about the illustrations that may or may not be central to the story. This introduces new words, which children attach to the features of the illustrations they point out and incorporate into book-centered conversations. This type of language, removed from the here and now, is decontextualized language. Children exposed to experiences not occurring in their immediate environment are more likely to understand and use decontextualized language ( Hindman et al., 2008 ). Repeated routines also contribute to language development. As books are read repeatedly, children become familiar with the vocabulary of the story and their conversations can be elaborated. Routines help children with developmental delays acquire language and use it more intelligibly ( van Kleek, 2004 ).

Conversation around a story's content and emphasis on specific words in the text (i.e., the phonological and print features of words alongside their meanings) have long-term effects ( Zucker et al., 2013 ). The quality of adult readers' interactions with children appears to be especially important to children's vocabulary growth (see also Coyne et al., 2009 ; Justice et al., 2005 ). In a study with preschool children, Zucker and colleagues (2013) found that teachers' intentional talk during reading had a longer-lasting effect on the children's language skills than the frequency of the teachers' reading to the children. Moreover, the effect of the teachers' talk during reading was not moderated by the children's initial vocabulary or literacy abilities. The long-term effect of high-quality teacher–child book-centered interactions in preschool lasted through the end of first grade.

New research shows that the effects of interactive reading also hold when adapted to the use of digital media as a platform for decontextualized language and other forms of language development. A study of videobooks showed that when adults were trained to use dialogic questioning techniques with the videos, 3-year-olds learned new words and recalled the books' storylines ( Strouse et al., 2013 ). However, a few studies of e-books also have shown that the bells and whistles of the devices can get in the way of those back-and-forth conversations if the readers and the e-book designers are not intentional about using the e-books to develop content knowledge and language skills ( Parish-Morris et al., 2013 ). (See also the discussion of effective use of technology in instruction in Chapter 6 .)

Alongside developing depth of vocabulary (including the meaning of words and phrases and their appropriate use in context), other important parameters of language development are syntax (rules for combining and ordering words in phrases, as in rules of grammar) and morphology (meaningful parts of words and word tenses). Even before the age of 2, toddlers parse a speech stream into grammatical units ( Hawthorne and Gerken, 2014 ). Long before preschool, most children join words together into sentences and begin to use the rules of grammar (i.e., syntax) to change the forms of words (e.g., adding s for plurals or ed for past tense). Along with these morphemic changes to words, understanding syntax helps children order the words and phrases in their sentences to convey and to change meaning. Before children learn to read, the rules of syntax help them derive meaning from what they hear and convey meaning through speech. Cunningham and Zibulsky (2014 , p. 45) describe syntactic development as “the ability to understand the structure of a sentence, including its tense, subject, and object.”

Although syntactic understanding develops for most children through conversation with adults and older children, children also use these rules of syntax to extract meaning from printed words. This becomes an important reading skill after first grade, when text meaning is less likely to be supported with pictures. Construction of sentences with passive voice and other complex, decontextualized word forms are more likely to be found in books and stories than in directive conversations with young children. An experimental study illustrates the role of exposure to syntactic structures in the development of language comprehension ( Vasilyeva et al., 2006 ). Four-year-olds listened to stories in active or passive voice. After listening to ten stories, their understanding of passages containing these syntactic structures was assessed. Although students in both groups understood and could use active voice (similar to routine conversation), those who listened to stories with passive voice scored higher on comprehension of this structure.

Children's understanding of morphology—the meaningful parts of words—begins in preschool for most children, as they recognize and use inflected endings to represent verb tense (e.g., -ing, -ed, -s) and plurals, and continues in the primary grades as children understand and use prefixes and suffixes. By second and third grade, children's use of morphemes predicts their reading comprehension ( Nagy et al., 2006 ; Nunes et al., 2012 ).

Development of Literacy Skills

Literacy skills follow a developmental trajectory such that early skills and stages lead into more complex and integrated skills and stages ( Adams, 1990 ). For example, phonemic awareness is necessary for decoding printed words ( Ball and Blachman, 1991 ; Bradley and Bryant, 1983 ; O'Connor et al., 1995 ), but it is not sufficient. Students need to understand the alphabetic principle (that speech sounds can be represented by letters of the alphabet, which is how speech is captured in print) before they can use their phonemic awareness (the ability to hear and manipulate sounds in spoken words) to independently decode words they have never seen before ( Byrne and Fielding-Barnsley, 1989 ; O'Connor and >Jenkins, 1995 ). Thus, instruction that combines skill development for 4- to 6-year-old children in phonemic awareness, letter knowledge, and conceptual understanding and use of these skills is more effective than teaching the skills in isolation ( Byrne and Fielding-Barnsley, 1989 ; O'Connor and Jenkins, 1995 ).

Seminal theories and studies of reading describe an inextricable link between language development and reading achievement (e.g., Byrne and Fielding-Barnsley, 1995 ; Gough and Tunmer, 1986 ; Hoover and Gough, 1990 ; Johnston and Kirby, 2006 ; Joshi and Aaron, 2000 ; Tunmer and Hoover, 1993 ; Vellutino et al., 2007 ). Early oral language competencies predict later literacy ( Pearson and Hiebert, 2010 ). Not only do young children with stronger oral language competencies acquire new language skills faster than students with poorly developed oral language competencies ( Dickinson and Porche, 2011 ), but they also learn key literacy skills faster, such as phonemic awareness and understanding of the alphabetic principle ( Cooper et al., 2002 ). Both of these literacy skills in turn facilitate learning to read in kindergarten and first grade. By preschool and kindergarten listening and speaking abilities have long-term impacts on children's reading and writing abilities in third through fifth grade ( Lee, 2011 ; Nation and Snowling, 1999 ; Sénéchal et al., 2006 ).

Vocabulary development (a complex and integrative feature of language that grows continuously) and reading words (a skill that most children master by third or fourth grade) ( Ehri, 2005 ) are reciprocally related, and both reading words accurately and understanding what words mean contribute to reading comprehension ( Gough et al., 1996 ). Because comprehending and learning from text depend largely upon a deep understanding of the language used to communicate the ideas and concepts expressed, oral language skills (i.e., vocabulary, syntax, listening comprehension) are at the core of this relationship between language and reading ( NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2005 ; Perfetti, 1985 ; Perfetti and Hart, 2002 ). For example, children with larger speaking vocabularies in preschool may have an easier time with phoneme awareness and the alphabetic principle because they can draw on more words to explore the similarities among the sounds they hear in spoken words and the letters that form the words ( Metsala and Walley, 1998 ). Each word a child knows can influence how well she or he understands a sentence that uses that word, which in turn can influence the acquisition of knowledge and the ability to learn new words. A stronger speaking and listening vocabulary provides a deeper and wider field of words students can attempt to match to printed words. Being bogged down by figuring out what a given word means slows the rate of information processing and limits what is learned from a sentence. Thus, differences in early vocabulary can have cascading, cumulative effects ( Fernald et al., 2013 ; Huttenlocher, 1998 ). The transition from speaking and listening to reading and writing is not a smooth one for many children. Although a well-developed vocabulary can make that transition easier, many children also have difficulty learning the production and meanings of words. Longitudinal studies of reading disability have found that 70 percent of poor readers had a history of language difficulties ( Catts et al., 1999 ).

Conclusion About the Development of Language and Literacy Skills The oral language and vocabulary children learn through interactions with parents, siblings, and caregivers and through high-quality interactions with educators provide the foundation for later literacy and for learning across all subject areas, as well as for their socioemotional well-being. The language interactions children experience at home and in school influence their developing minds and their understanding of concepts and ideas.

Role of the Language-Learning Environment

Today's science of reading development focuses more broadly than on teaching children to read the actual words on a page. As stressed throughout this report, young children's development entails a back-and-forth process of social interactions with knowledgeable others in their environment ( Bruner, 1978 ; NRC and IOM, 2000 ; Vygotsky, 1978 , 1986 ), and research has focused on the language of these interactions, examining how children's linguistic experiences influence aspects of their development over time, including their literacy development. The daily talk to which children are exposed and in which they participate is essential for developing their minds—a key ingredient for building their knowledge of the world and their understanding of concepts and ideas. In turn, this conceptual knowledge is a cornerstone of reading success.

The bulk of the research on early linguistic experiences has investigated language input in the home environment, demonstrating the features of caregivers' (usually the mother's) speech that promote language development among young children. The evidence accumulated emphasizes the importance of the quantity of communicative input (i.e., the number of words and sentences spoken) as well as the quality of that input, as measured by the variety of words and syntactic structures used (for relevant reviews, see Rowe, 2012 ; Vasilyeva and Waterfall, 2011 ). Because children's language development is sensitive to these inputs, variability in children's language-based interactions in the home environment explains some of the variance in their language development.

A smaller but growing and compelling research base is focused on how children's literacy skills are influenced by language use in early care and education settings and schools—for example, linguistic features of these settings or elementary school teachers' speech and its relationship to children's reading outcomes ( Greenwood et al., 2011 ). This research has particularly relevant implications for educational practices (discussed further in Chapter 6 ).

The language environment of the classroom can function as a support for developing the kind of language that is characteristic of the school curriculum—for example, giving children opportunities to develop the sophisticated vocabulary and complex syntax found in texts, beginning at a very early age ( Schleppegrell, 2003 ; Snow and Uccelli, 2009 ). Moreover, advances in cognitive science suggest that it is not enough to be immersed in environments that offer multiple opportunities for exposure to varied and rich language experiences. Rather, the process also needs to be socially mediated through more knowledgeable persons who can impart their knowledge to the learner; again, social interaction is a critical component of cognitive development and learning. Early childhood settings and elementary classrooms thus not only present opportunities for exposure to varied language- and literacy-rich activities (whether written or spoken), but also provide a person who is expert in mediating the learning process—the educator.

Research demonstrates that teachers' use of high-quality language is linked to individual differences in language and literacy skills; this work likewise shows the substantial variation in the quality of teacher talk in early childhood classrooms (e.g., Bowers and Vasilyeva, 2011 ; Gámez and Levine, 2013 ; Greenwood et al., 2011 ; Huttenlocher et al., 2002 ). For example, Huttenlocher and colleagues (2002) found greater syntactic skills in preschoolers exposed to teachers who used more syntactically complex utterances. Another study found for monolingual English-speaking children that fourth-grade reading comprehension levels were predicted by exposure to sophisticated vocabulary in preschool. These effects were mediated by children's vocabulary and literacy skills in kindergarten ( Dickinson and Porche, 2011 ).

In classroom studies focused on the linguistic environment, the level of analysis has involved broad measures of language use, such as amount of talk (i.e., teacher–student interactions by minute: Connor et al., 2006 ), amount of instruction (i.e., in teacher-managed versus child-managed instruction: Connor et al., 2007 ), type of interaction style (i.e., didactic versus cognitively demanding talk or the amount of extended discourse: Dickinson and Smith, 1991 ; Jacoby and Lesaux, 2014 ; Smith and Dickinson, 1994 ), or instructional moves made by the teacher (e.g., modeling: see review in Lawrence and Snow, 2011 ). A commonly included measurement that has been linked to children's literacy development is extended discourse, defined as talk that “requires participants to develop understandings beyond the here and now and that requires the use of several utterances or turns to build a linguistic structure, such as in explanations, narratives, or pretend” ( Snow et al., 2001 , p. 2). Children are better prepared to comprehend narrative texts they encounter in school if their early language environments provide more exposure to and opportunities to participate in extended discourse. This is because extended discourse and narrative texts share similar patterns for communicating ideas ( Uccelli et al., 2006 ).

Engaging groups of children in effective extended discourse involves asking and discussing open-ended questions and encouraging turn taking, as well as monitoring the group to involve nonparticipating children ( Girolametto and Weitzman, 2002 ). In addition to using interactive storybook and text reading as a platform for back-and-forth conversations (often referred to as interactive or dialogic reading, as described in the preceding section) ( Mol et al., 2009 ; Zucker et al., 2013 ), engaging children in extended discourse throughout classroom activities (e.g., small-group learning activities, transitions and routines [ van Kleek, 2004 ], dramatic play [ Mages, 2008 ; Morrow and Schickedanz, 2006 ]) is fundamental to providing a high-quality language-learning environment ( Jacoby and Lesaux, 2014 ).

In an example of the influence of the quantity and quality of teachers' language input in linguistically diverse classrooms, Bowers and Vasilyeva (2011) found that the total number of words produced by teachers and the diversity of their speech (which was entirely in English) were related to vocabulary gains for children from both English-only households and households in which English was not the primary language, respectively. Thus, they found that preschool dual language learners benefited only from increased quantities of language exposure and showed a negative relationship between vocabulary growth and teachers' syntactic complexity. By contrast, the English-only children—who presumably had more developed English language proficiency skills—benefited from the diversity of teachers' vocabulary and syntactic complexity. These findings are consistent with the notion that to promote language learning, different inputs are needed at different developmental stages ( Dickinson and Freiberg, 2009 ; Gámez and Lesaux, 2012 ). Children benefit from hearing simplified speech during very early word learning ( Furrow et al., 1979 ). With more exposure to language and more advanced vocabulary development, they benefit from speech input that is more complex (i.e., Hoff and Naigles, 2002 ). Hoff (2006) suggests that if input is too complex, children filter it out without negative consequences—as long as sufficient beneficial input is available to them. On the other hand, “children have no way to make up for input that is too simple” ( Hoff, 2006 , p. 75).

An important consideration in light of these findings is that recent research in early childhood classrooms serving children from low-income backgrounds suggests that daily high-quality language-building experiences may be rare for these children. For example, in a Head Start organization serving large numbers of Latino children a recent observational study found a preschool environment lacking in the frequent and high-quality teacher–child language interactions that are needed to support language and literacy development ( Jacoby and Lesaux, 2014 ). Literacy instruction was highly routine based and with low-level language structures. Extended discourse was infrequently used; only 22 percent of observed literacy-based lessons included at least one instance of extended discourse between a teacher and a child or group of children. Instead, teachers asked questions that yielded short answers or linked only to the here and now (e.g., What day is it today? What is the weather today? ). These features of infrequent extended discourse and predominantly routine-based literacy instruction were remarkably stable across teachers and classrooms. Other research investigating teacher talk in Head Start preschool classrooms has produced similar findings (e.g., Dickinson et al., 2008 ).

This is consistent with findings that there are sizable cultural and socioeconomic differences in high-quality language-promoting experiences in the home and in the classroom environment in early childhood ( Dickinson, 2003 ; Dickinson and Porche, 2011 ; Dickinson and Tabors, 2001 ; Raikes et al., 2006 ), just as such differences have been found in the number of words children hear by the time they enter school ( Bradley and Corwyn, 2002 ; Fernald et al., 2013 ; Hart and Risley, 1995 ; Schneidman et al., 2013 ; Weisleder and Fernald, 2013 ). At the same time, for children from low-resource backgrounds oral language skills show an even stronger connection to later academic outcomes than for children from high-resource backgrounds. Given these findings, rich linguistic experiences at early ages may therefore be especially important for these children. Even small improvements in the literacy environment can have especially strong effects for children who are raised in low-income households ( Dearing et al., 2001 ; Dickinson and Porche, 2011 ).

In sum, the language environment has important effects on children's learning, and children benefit from extensive opportunities to listen to and use complex spoken language ( National Early Literacy Panel, 2008 ). Teachers' use of high-quality language is linked to individual differences in language and literacy skills, and there is considerable variation in the quantity and quality of teachers' language use across classrooms. The quality of the classroom language environment is a lever for lasting improvements in children's language and literacy development, and it is important to tailor classroom talk to match the developmental stage of children's language acquisition.

Creating a Rich Language Environment: Implications for Adults

Improving language environments for young children requires daily learning opportunities that focus on the diversity and complexity of language used with young children. Practically speaking, this can be achieved through extended discourse, with multiple exchanges or turns that go beyond the immediate “here and now” using explanations, narratives, or pretend. Extended discourse can take place throughout all activities and in specific interactions, especially using book reading as a platform for back-and-forth conversations.

Further research is needed to advance understanding of language-based classroom processes and how dynamic and ongoing interactions facilitate or impede children's literacy. Such studies could advance existing research in at least two ways. In particular, it could further elucidate how language-based social processes in the classroom affect literacy development for the many students who enter schools and other care and education settings with limited proficiency in English. The majority of published studies focused on language-based interactions are focused on English-only learners, despite the fact that social processes can be experienced differently by different groups, even within the same setting ( Rogoff and Angelillo, 2002 ; Tseng and Seidman, 2007 ). Gámez and Levine (2013) suggest that future research examine the influence of dual-language input on dual language learners' language development; the nature of teacher talk during different parts of the instructional day, including joint book reading, and how these language experiences predict dual language learners' language skills; and the impact of classroom talk interventions—those that aim to manipulate the frequency and complexity of teachers' language—on both the language environment and dual language learners' language development.

In addition, prior research has measured a two-way process in a largely unidirectional manner—measuring speech only from parent to child or educator to student. It would be more valuable going forward if research were guided by the notion that the language-based interactions between students and educators mediate instruction, and were therefore to explore how communicative feedback loops, both adult–child and child–peer interactions, influence children's learning and development. Taking into account the student's contribution to the classroom language environment is particularly important in light of evidence that teachers modify their speech to conform to their students' limited language proficiency levels, potentially leading to a lower-quality language environment that impedes students' language growth ( Ellis, 2008 ; see Huttenlocher et al., 2010 ; Justice et al., 2013 ). More specifically, Justice and colleagues (2013) suggest that future research examine teacher–child language interactions in a multidimensional way to explore how syntactic complexity, cognitive demand, and even linguistic form (e.g., questions, comments) relate to each other; the links between children's use of complex syntax in classroom-based interactions and their future general language ability; and interventions designed to enhance classroom language interactions, focusing on both proximal and distal outcomes for children. Finally, greater understanding is needed of the ways in which the classroom language processes described in this section might act as a foundational mediator of the efficacy of interventions focused on learning outcomes in other domains and subject areas.

Alongside student–educator interactions, studies show that peer-to-peer interactions in the classroom may also have positive impacts on children's vocabulary and expressive language abilities. Children spend a significant amount of time interacting with other children in classroom settings, and a 2009 study examining the language growth and abilities of 4-year-olds in prekindergarten classrooms found that peers who have higher language abilities positively affect other children's language development. This study also found that children with advanced language skills will receive greater benefits from interacting with peers who also have advanced language skills ( Mashburn et al., 2009 ). These findings are similar to another study showing that peer interactions in the classroom, along with the ability level of the peers, have positive effects on the child's cognitive, prereading, expressive language skills ( Henry and Rickman, 2007 ). In order to achieve these benefits, however, the preschool classrooms need to be designed so that peers can interact with one another, and include activities such as reading books and engaging in play together. Children with teachers who organize the day with optimal amounts of time for peer-to-peer interactions may achieve greater language growth ( Mashburn et al., 2009 ).

Language and Literacy Development in Dual Language Learners 1

For children whose home language is not the predominant language of their school, educators and schools need to ensure the development of English proficiency. Both parents and preschool teachers can be particularly useful in improving these children's depth of vocabulary ( Aukrust, 2007 ; Roberts, 2008 ). At the same time, children can be helped to both build and maintain their first language while adding language and literacy skills in English ( Espinosa, 2005 ). In support of this as a long-term goal are the potential advantages of being bilingual, including maintaining a cultural and linguistic heritage and conferring an advantage in the ability to communicate with a broader population in future social, educational, and work environments. Additionally, an emerging field of research, albeit with mixed results to date, explores potential advantages of being bilingual that are linked more directly to cognitive development, starting in early childhood and extending to preserving cognitive function and delaying the symptoms of dementia in the elderly ( Bialystok, 2011 ; de Bruin et al., 2015 ).

Bilingual or multilingual children are faced with more communicative challenges than their monolingual peers. A child who frequently experiences failure to be understood or to understand may be driven to pay more attention to context, paralinguistic cues, and gestures in order to interpret an utterance, and thus become better at reading such cues. The result may be improved development of theory of mind and understanding of pragmatics ( Yow and Markman, 2011a , b ). In addition, the need to continually suppress one language for another affords ongoing practice in inhibitory or executive control, which could confer advantages on a range of inhibitory control tasks in children and helps preserve this fundamental ability in aging adults ( Bialystok, 2011 ; Bialystok and Craik, 2010 ; Bialystok et al., 2009 ).

One challenge in the education of dual language learners is that they sometimes are classified along with children with special needs. One reason for this is the lack of good assessment tools to help distinguish the nature of the difficulties experienced by dual language learners—whether due to a learning disability or to the fact that learning a second language is difficult, takes time, and develops differently in different children ( Hamayan et al., 2013 ).

Mathematics

Children's early knowledge of mathematics is surprisingly important, and it strongly predicts later success in mathematics ( Denton and West, 2002 ; Koponen et al., 2013 ; Passolunghi et al., 2007 ). Mathematics knowledge in preschool predicts mathematics achievement even into high school ( National Mathematics Advisory Panel, 2008 ; NRC, 2009 ; Stevenson and Newman, 1986 ). Mathematics ability and language ability also are interrelated as mutually reinforcing skills ( Duncan et al., 2007 ; Farran et al., 2005 ; Lerkkanen et al., 2005 ; O'Neill et al., 2004 ; Praet et al., 2013 ; Purpura et al., 2011 ). Indeed, mathematical thinking reaches beyond competence with numbers and shapes to form a foundation for general cognition and learning ( Clements and Sarama, 2009 ; Sarama et al., 2012 ), and problems with mathematics are the best predictor of failure to graduate high school. Mathematics therefore appears to be a core subject and a core component of thinking and learning ( Duncan and Magnuson, 2011 ; Duncan et al., 2007 ).

Given its general importance to academic success ( Sadler and Tai, 2007 ), children need a robust foundation in mathematics knowledge in their earliest years. Multiple analyses suggest that mathematics learning should begin early, especially for children at risk for later difficulties in school ( Byrnes and Wasik, 2009 ; Clements and Sarama, 2014 ). Well before first grade, children can learn the skills and concepts that support more complex mathematics understanding later. Particularly important areas of mathematics for young children to learn include number, which includes whole number, operations, and relations; geometry; spatial thinking; and measurement. Children also need to develop proficiency in processes for both general and specific mathematical reasoning ( NRC, 2009 ).

If given opportunities to learn, young children possess a remarkably broad, complex, and sophisticated—albeit informal—knowledge of mathematics ( Baroody, 2004 ; Clarke et al., 2006 ; Clements et al., 1999 ; Fuson, 2004 ; Geary, 1994 ; Thomson et al., 2005 ). In their free play, almost all preschoolers engage in substantial amounts of premathematical activity. They count objects; compare magnitudes; and explore patterns, shapes, and spatial relations. Importantly, this is true regardless of a child's income level or gender ( Seo and Ginsburg, 2004 ). Preschoolers can also, for example, learn to invent solutions to simple arithmetic problems ( Sarama and Clements, 2009 ).

High-quality mathematics education can help children realize their potential in mathematics achievement ( Doig et al., 2003 ; Thomson et al., 2005 ). However, without such education starting, and continuing throughout, the early years, many children will be on a trajectory in which they will have great difficulty catching up to their peers ( Rouse et al., 2005 ). As discussed further in Chapter 6 , early childhood classrooms typically are ill suited to helping children learn mathematics and underestimate their ability to do so. In some cases, children can even experience a regression on some mathematics skills during prekindergarten and kindergarten ( Farran et al., 2007 ; Wright, 1994 ). Mathematics needs to be conceptualized as more than skills, and its content as more than counting and simple shapes. Without building a robust understanding of mathematics in the early years, children too often come to believe that math is a guessing game and a system of rules without reason ( Munn, 2006 ).

Both education and experience can make a difference, as evidenced by data from the latest international Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study, which added data collection on early mathematics education ( Mullis et al., 2012 ). Students with higher mathematics achievement at fourth and sixth grades had parents who reported that they often engaged their children in early numeracy activities and that their children had attended preprimary education and started school able to do early numeracy tasks (e.g., simple addition and subtraction). Those children who had attended preschool or kindergarten had higher achievement, while the 13 percent who had attended no preprimary school had much lower average mathematics achievement ( Mullis et al., 2012 ).

Developmental Progression of Learning Mathematics

Children move through a developmental progression in specific mathematical domains, which informs learning trajectories as important tools for supporting learning and teaching. Recent work based on empirical research and emphasizing a cognitive science perspective conceptualizes learning trajectories for mathematics as “descriptions of children's thinking and learning in a specific mathematical domain, and a related, conjectured route through a set of instructional tasks designed to engender those mental processes or actions hypothesized to move children through a developmental progression of levels of thinking, created with the intent of supporting children's achievement of specific goals in that mathematical domain” ( Clements and Sarama, 2004 , p. 83).

Box 4-5 illustrates the concept of a developmental progression through the example of subitizing , an oft-neglected mathematical goal for young children. Research shows that subitizing, the rapid and accurate recognition of the number in a small group, is one of the main abilities very young children should develop ( Palmer and Baroody, 2011 ; Reigosa-Crespo et al., 2013 ). Through subitizing, children can discover critical properties of number, such as conservation and compensation ( Clements and Sarama, 2014 ; Maclellan, 2012 ) and develop such capabilities as unitizing and arithmetic. Subitizing is not the only way children think and learn about number. Counting is the other method of quantification. It is the first and most basic mathematical algorithm and one of the more critical early mathematics competencies ( Aunola et al., 2004 ; National Mathematics Advisory Panel, 2008 ). Chapter 6 includes examples from a complete learning trajectory—goal, developmental progression, and instructional activities—for counting ( Clements and Sarama, 2014 ).

Subitizing: A Developmental Progression. A quantitative, or numerical, “sense” is innate or develops early. For example, very young children possess approximate number systems (ANSs) that allow them to discriminate large and small sets, (more...)

Children with Special Needs

Children with special needs in learning mathematics fall into two categories. Those with mathematical difficulties struggle to learn mathematics for any reason; this category may apply to as many as 35-40 percent of students ( Berch and Mazzocco, 2007 ). Those with specific mathematics learning disabilities are more severe cases; these students have a memory or cognitive deficit that interferes with their ability to learn math ( Geary, 2004 ). This category may apply to about 6-7 percent ( Berch and Mazzocco, 2007 ; Mazzocco and Myers, 2003 ). In one study, this classification persisted in third grade for 63 percent of those classified as having mathematics learning disabilities in kindergarten ( Mazzocco and Myers, 2003 ).

Mathematics learning disabilities, while assumed to have a genetic basis, currently are defined by students' behaviors—yet with ongoing debate among experts about what those behaviors are. One consistent finding is that students with mathematics learning disabilities have difficulty retrieving basic arithmetic facts quickly. This has been hypothesized to be the result of an inability to store or retrieve facts and impairments in visual-spatial representation. As early as kindergarten, limited working memory and speed of cognitive processing may be problems for these children ( Geary et al., 2007 ). Many young children with learning disabilities in reading show a similar rapid-naming deficit for letters and words ( Siegel and Mazabel, 2013 ; Steacy et al., 2014 ). Another possibility is that a lack of higher-order, or executive, control of verbal material causes difficulty learning basic arithmetic facts or combinations. For example, students with mathematics learning disabilities may have difficulty inhibiting irrelevant associations. An illustration of this would be hearing “5 + 4” and saying “6” because it follows 5.

One explanation for the difficulty students with mathematics learning disabilities have learning basic arithmetic combinations might be delays in understanding counting. These students may not fully understand counting nor recognize errors in counting as late as second grade. They persist in using immature counting strategies, such as counting “one-by-one” on their fingers, throughout elementary school ( Geary et al., 1992 ; Ostad, 1998 ). Other experts, however, claim that a lack of specific competencies, such as subitizing, is more important ( Berch and Mazzocco, 2007 ).

Some evidence suggests that it is possible to predict which kindergartners are at risk for mathematics learning disabilities based on skill including reading numerals, number constancy, magnitude judgments of one-digit numbers, or mental addition of one-digit numbers ( Mazzocco and Thompson, 2005 ). However, until more is known, students should be classified as having mathematics learning disabilities only with great caution and after good mathematics instruction has been provided. Such labeling in the earliest years could do more harm than good ( Clements and Sarama, 2012 ).

Interrelationships Between Mathematics and Language

It can appear that language is less of a concern in mathematics compared to other subjects because it is assumed to be based on numbers or symbols, but this is not the case ( Clements et al., 2013a ). In fact, children learn math mainly from oral language, rather than from mathematical symbolism or textbooks ( Janzen, 2008 ). In addition, “talking math” is more than just using mathematics terms ( Clements and Sarama, 2014 ). Therefore, both oral language and literacy in general, as well as the “language of mathematics,” are important for learning ( Vukovic and Lesaux, 2013 ). Vocabulary and knowledge of print are both predictors of later numeracy ( Purpura et al., 2011 ). Similarly, growth in mathematics from kindergarten to third grade is related to both early numerical skills and phonological processing ( Vukovic, 2012 ). In one study of linguistically and ethnically diverse children aged 6-9 years, language ability predicted gains in geometry, probability, and data analysis but not in arithmetic or algebra (controlling for reading ability, visual–spatial working memory, and gender) ( Vukovic and Lesaux, 2013 ). Thus, language may affect how children make meaning of mathematics but not its complex arithmetic procedures.

Moreover, there is an important bidirectional relationship between learning in mathematics and language ( Sarama et al., 2012 ). Each has related developmental milestones. Children learn number words at the same time as other linguistic labels. Most children recognize by the age of 2 which words are for numbers and use them only in appropriate contexts ( Fuson, 1988 ). Each also has related developmental patterns, with learning progressing along similar paths. In both, children recognize the whole before its parts. In learning language, this is word before syllable, syllable before rime-onset, and rime-onset before phoneme (see also Anthony et al., 2003 ; Ziegler and Goswami, 2005 ). Similarly in mathematics, numbers are first conceptualized as unbreakable categories and then later as composites (e.g., 5 is composed of 3 and 2) ( Butterworth, 2005 ; Sarama and Clements, 2009 ). By 6 years old in most cultures, children have been exposed to symbol representations that are both alphabetic and numerical, and they begin to be able to segment words into phonemes and numbers into singletons (e.g., understanding that 3 is 1 and 1 and 1) ( Butterworth, 2005 ; Sarama and Clements, 2009 ; Wagner et al., 1993 ). The ability to identify the component nature of words and numbers predicts the ability to read ( Adams, 1990 ; Stanovich and Siegel, 1994 ) and to compute ( Geary, 1990 , 1993 ). In addition to these similarities in typical developmental pathways, many children with learning disabilities experience deficits in competencies related to both language/literacy and numeracy ( Geary, 1993 ; Hecht et al., 2001 ; NRC, 1998 ).

Furthermore, there appear to be shared competencies between the two subject areas. For example, preschoolers' narrative abilities (i.e., their abilities to convey all the main events of a story and offer a perspective on its events) have been shown to predict mathematics achievement 2 years later ( O'Neill et al., 2004 ). Beginning mathematics scores have been shown to be highly predictive of subsequent achievement in both reading and mathematics although beginning reading skills (such as letter recognition, word identification, and word sounds) were shown to be highly predictive of later reading (advanced competencies such as evaluation) but not mathematics learning ( Duncan et al., 2007 ).

A causal relationship between rich mathematics learning and developing language and literacy skills is supported by a randomized study of the effects of a math curriculum called Building Blocks on prekindergarten children's letter recognition and oral language skills. Building Blocks children performed the same as the children in the control group on letter recognition and on three oral language subscales but outperformed them on four subscales: ability to recall key words, use of complex utterances, willingness to reproduce narratives independently, and inference ( Sarama et al., 2012 ). These skills had no explicit relation to the math curriculum. Similarly, a study of 5- to 7-year-olds showed that an early mathematics and logical-mathematical intervention increased later scores in English by 14 percentile points ( Shayer and Adhami, 2010 ).

Time on task (or time on instruction) does affect learning, which naturally leads to consideration of potential conflicts or tradeoffs between time spent on different subjects (e.g., Bodovski and Farkas, 2007 ). Indeed, a frequent concern is that introducing a mathematics curriculum may decrease the time devoted to language and literacy, impeding children's development in those areas, which are heavily emphasized in early learning goals (see Clements and Sarama, 2009 ; Farran et al., 2007 ; Lee and Ginsburg, 2007 ; Sarama and Clements, 2009 ). However, this assumes that mathematics activities will not have a positive effect on language and literacy. Yet as described here, evidence from both educational and psychological research suggests the potential for high-quality instruction in each to have mutual benefits for learning in both subjects. Rich mathematical activities, such as discussing multiple solutions and solving narrative story problems, can help lay the groundwork for literacy through language development, while rich literacy activities can help lay the groundwork for mathematics development ( Sarama et al., 2012 ).

Children Who Are Dual Language Learners

For mathematics learning in children who are dual language learners, the language, not just the vocabulary, of mathematics need to be addressed ( Clements and Sarama, 2014 ). Challenges for dual language learners include both technical vocabulary, which can range in how similar or distinct terms are from everyday language, and the use of complex noun phrases. On the other hand, bilingual children often can understand a mathematical idea more readily because, after using different terms for it in different languages, they comprehend that the mathematical idea is abstract, and not tied to a specific term (see Secada, 1992 ).

There is evidence that the best approach is to teach these young children in their first language ( Celedón-Pattichis et al., 2010 ; Espada, 2012 ). At a minimum, their teachers need to connect everyday language with the language of math ( Janzen, 2008 ). It is also essential to build on the resources that bilingual children bring to learning mathematics—all cultures have “funds of knowledge” (culturally developed and historically accumulated bodies of knowledge and skills) that can be used to develop mathematical contexts and understandings ( Moll et al., 1992 ). Instructional practices for teaching mathematics with dual language learners are discussed further in Chapter 6 .

Conclusions About Learning Specific Subjects For subject-matter content knowledge and proficiency, children learn best when supported along a trajectory with three components: (1) their understanding of the subject-matter content itself, (2) their progress through predictable developmental levels and patterns of thinking related to their understanding of the content, and (3) instructional tasks and strategies that adults who work with children can employ to promote that learning at each level. For example: Almost all topics in mathematics follow predictable learning trajectories that include number counting and subitizing, number relationships and magnitude comparison, arithmetic operations, geometry and spatial sense, and measurement. Learning trajectories in literacy include specific developmental sequences in children's learning of phonological awareness and phonics (letter-sound correspondences), which together contribute to children's understanding of how spoken words are captured in reading and writing and thus to their advancement through broader levels of early literacy. Some principles of how children learn along a trajectory hold across subject-matter domains, but there are also substantive differences among subjects in the specific skills children need and in the learning trajectories. Both generalizable principles and subject-specific distinctions have implications for the knowledge and competencies needed to work with children. An important factor in children's learning of subject-matter content is how each of the components of learning trajectories both requires and develops aspects of learning that are not content specific, such as critical reasoning, executive function, self-regulation, learning skills, positive dispositions toward learning, and relationships.
  • GENERAL LEARNING COMPETENCIES

Educators, developmental scientists, and economists have long known that academic achievement is a result of both the growth of specific knowledge and the development of general learning competencies that regulate how children enlist cognitive resources when they encounter learning challenges, motivate advances in learning, and strengthen children's self-confidence as learners.

These general learning competencies have been labeled and categorized in various ways. Considerable recent research on some of these learning competencies has been conducted using the concept of “executive function,” which generally refers to a set of supervisory functions that regulate and control cognitive activity that affects learning ( Vitiello et al., 2011 ) and allow children to persevere with tasks, including learning tasks, even when facing fatigue, distraction, or decreased motivation. In the field of human development “mastery motivation” in infancy typically is indexed by the baby's persistence, focus, and curiosity in exploration and problem solving ( Morgan et al., 1990 ; Wang and Barrett, 2013 ). In preschool-age children, these skills often are conceptualized as the quality of the child's “approaches to learning,” which include motivation, engagement, and interest in learning activities. Heckman (2007) has used the term “noncognitive skills” to refer to many of these learning competencies, including self-control, persistence, self-discipline, motivation, and self-esteem, as well as future orientedness (i.e., the capacity to substitute long-term goals for immediate satisfactions). This label is used in contrast to the “cognitive skills” that are more often measured to predict children's later success, although there is considerable research that the “noncognitive skills” also support learning and achievement (see, e.g., Cunha and Heckman, 2010 ; Heckman, 2007 ), and they are highly relevant to cognitive skills in such areas as language, mathematics, science, and other traditional academic fields.

Here the alternative conceptualizations for these important aspects of child development and early learning are grouped as “learning competencies” to reflect their importance for early learning. Individual differences in these competencies are important determinants of learning and academic motivation, and children's experiences at home and in the classroom contribute to some of these differences. This section examines these competencies as well as their interrelationships with the previously discussed subject-matter domains of language and literacy and mathematics.

General Cognitive Skills

Several cognitive control processes are important for planning and executing goal-directed activity, which is needed for successful learning (e.g., Blair, 2002 ; Lyon and Krasnegor, 1996 ). These processes include, for example, short-term and working memory, attention control and shifting, cognitive flexibility (changing thinking between different concepts and thinking about multiple concepts simultaneously), inhibitory control (suppressing unproductive responses or strategies), and cognitive self-regulation. These processes also are closely related to emotion regulation, which is discussed later in the section on socioemotional development, and which also contributes to children's classroom success.

As noted previously, many general cognitive processes often are referred to collectively as “executive function,” although not everyone defines this construct in the same way (e.g., Miyake et al., 2000 ; Raver, 2013 ), and different disciplines and researchers differ as to which cognitive skills it includes. Other theoretical frameworks exist as well. For example, cognitive control and complexity theory postulates that executive function is an outcome, not an explanatory construct, and is the result of children's creation and application of rules (driven perhaps by an increase in reflection afforded by experience-dependent maturation of the prefrontal cortex) ( Müller et al., 2008 ; Zelazo and Carlson, 2012 ; Zelazo and Lyons, 2012 ). As with the overall domains of development displayed earlier in Figure 4-1 , the committee did not attempt to reconcile those different perspectives.

This variation in perspectives makes it difficult to parse the literature produced by different fields of research and practice. In general, however, executive function appears to improve most rapidly in young children ( Best et al., 2011 ; Blair, 2002 ; Hughes and Ensor, 2011 ; Romine and Reynolds, 2005 ; Schoemaker et al., 2014 ; Zelazo and Carlson, 2012 ). Executive function processes appear to be partially dependent on the development of the prefrontal cortex (the site of higher-order cognitive processes), notably through the preschool and kindergarten age range ( Bassett et al., 2012 ; Blair, 2002 ).

Short-Term and Working Memory

Short-term memory is the ability for short-term recall, such as of a sentence or important details from conversation and reading. Working memory allows children to hold in their memory information from multiple sources, whether heard or read, so they can use and link that information. Updating working memory is the ability to keep and use relevant information while engaging in another cognitively demanding task ( Conway et al., 2003 ; DeYoung, 2011 ).

Attention Control and Shifting

Attention control is the ability to focus attention and disregard distracting stimuli (e.g., a continuous performance task that requires a child to identify when some familiar object appears onscreen and ignore other objects that appear, or a task that requires ignoring extraneous information in a mathematics word problem). Attention shifting is a related process of switching a “mental set” while simultaneously ignoring distractions (e.g., counting by different units—tens and ones). Attention shifting and cognitive flexibility are often grouped.

Cognitive Flexibility

Cognitive flexibility capacities develop gradually throughout early childhood and have significant influences on children's social and academic competence. Cognitive flexibility is important, for example, for reading ( Duke and Block, 2012 ). Children who are better able to consider, at the same time, both letter-sound and semantic (meaning) information about words have better reading comprehension ( Cartwright, 2002 ; Cartwright et al., 2010 ). Reading comprehension also appears to improve when children are taught about words with multiple meanings (e.g., spell or plane ), and sentences with multiple meanings (e.g., “The woman chased the man on a motorcycle.”) ( Yuill, 1996 ; Zipke et al., 2009 ). In addition, interventions in young children that focus on cognitive flexibility have shown significant benefits for reading comprehension ( Cartwright, 2008 ).

Inhibitory Control

Inhibitory control involves controlling a dominant response (e.g., the first answer that comes to mind) so as to think about better strategies or ideas. The skill of simple response inhibition (withholding an initial, sometimes impulsive, response) develops during infancy through toddlerhood. Infants also develop some control of cognitive conflict in tasks in which an item of interest to them is first hidden in one location and then another, and the child must resist the response of searching in the first location ( Diamond, 1991 ; Müller et al., 2008 ; Rothbart and Rueda, 2005 ) (see Marcovitch and Zelazo, 2009 , for a model of possible mechanisms). Later in their first year, children can resolve conflict between their line of sight and their line of reaching ( Diamond, 1991 ). By about 30 months, they can successfully complete a spatial conflict task ( Rothbart and Rueda, 2005 ). From 3 to 5 years of age, complex response inhibition and response shifting develop, with attention shifting developing at about age 4 ( Bassett et al., 2012 ). The most rapid increase in inhibitory control is between 5 and 8 years of age, although moderate improvements are seen up to young adulthood ( Best et al., 2011 ).

Inhibitory control supports children's learning across subject-matter areas. As one example of its importance for mathematics, when the initial reading of a problem is not the correct one, children need to inhibit their impulse to answer (incorrectly) and carefully examine the problem. Consider the following problem: “There were six birds in a tree. Three birds already flew away. How many birds were there from the start?” Children have to inhibit the immediate desire to subtract prompted by the words “flew away” and perform addition instead.

Cognitive Self-Regulation

Cognitive self-regulation is what helps children plan ahead, focus attention, and remember past experiences. The construct of self-regulation and related concepts have a long history in psychology (e.g., Glaser, 1991 ; Markman, 1977 , 1981 ; Piaget and Szeminska, 1952 ; Sternberg, 1985 ; Vygotsky, 1978 ; Zelazo et al., 2003 ) and education (e.g., McGillicuddy-De Lisi, 1982 ; Steffe and Tzur, 1994 ). Most recently, researchers and educators have used the broad term self-regulation to refer to the processes involved in intentionally controlling attention, thinking, impulses, emotions, and behavior. In this way, self-regulation can be thought of in relation to several aspects of development, including the cognitive processes discussed here and the social and emotional processes discussed later in this chapter. Developmental psychobiological research and neuroimaging indicate that these subclasses are both neurally and behaviorally distinct while also being related and correlated ( Bassett et al., 2012 ; Hofmann et al., 2012 ; Hongwanishkul et al., 2005 ; Neuenschwander et al., 2012 ; Willoughby et al., 2011 ). Together, these types of self-regulation allow children to persevere with tasks even when facing difficulties in problem solving or learning, fatigue, distraction, or decreased motivation ( Blair and Razza, 2007 ; Neuenschwander et al., 2012 ). It is thus unsurprising that kindergarten teachers believe self-regulation is as important as academics ( Bassok and Rorem, 2014 ).

Both cognitive self-regulation and emotional self-regulation (discussed later in this chapter) contribute to socioemotional development and also play a role in learning. Although the relationship between various features of cognitive self-regulation and academic achievement has been well documented for older students (e.g., Bielaczyc et al., 1995 ; Zimmerman, 2002 ), less was known until recently about how self-regulation developed in the early years contributes to the later development of cognitive and emotional self-regulation and academic achievement ( NRC and IOM, 2000 ).

Children's self-regulation and their ability to successfully function in school settings are related in two ways. First, emotional self-regulation enables children to benefit from learning in various social contexts, including their capacities to manage emotions in interactions with educators as well as peers (e.g., one-on-one, in cooperative pairs, in large and small groups). It also assists them in conforming to classroom rules and routines. Second, cognitive self-regulation enables children to develop and make use of cognitive processes that are necessary for academic learning ( Anghel, 2010 ).

Although most studies have focused on specific effects of either cognitive or emotional self-regulation, evidence suggests that the two are interconnected. This link is probably due to the commonality of the neurological mechanisms governing both emotional and cognitive self-regulation. For example, children lacking emotion regulation are likely also to have problems with regulating cognitive processes, such as attention ( Derryberry and Reed, 1996 ; LeDoux, 1996 ). Moreover, earlier patterns in the development of emotion control have been shown to be predictive of children's later ability to exercise control over their cognitive functioning ( Blair, 2002 ).

Several studies have shown positive correlations between self-regulation and achievement in young children (e.g., Bierman et al., 2008b ; Blair and Razza, 2007 ; Blair et al., 2010 ; Bull et al., 1999 ; Cameron et al., 2012 ; Neuenschwander et al., 2012 ; Roebers et al., 2012 ; Welsh et al., 2010 ), although there are exceptions ( Edens and Potter, 2013 ). Preschoolers' cognitive self-regulation, including inhibitory control and attention shifting, were found to be related to measures of literacy and mathematics ability in kindergarten ( Blair and Razza, 2007 ). In another study, children with higher self-regulation, including attention, working memory, and inhibitory control, achieved at higher levels in literacy, language, and mathematics ( McClelland et al., 2007 ). Interventions in the area of self-regulation have shown positive effects for reading achievement ( Best et al., 2011 ; Bierman et al., 2008a ; Blair and Diamond, 2008 ; Blair and Razza, 2007 ; Diamond and Lee, 2011 ). Among struggling first graders in an effective reading intervention, those who were retained in grade showed significantly weaker self-regulation skills ( Dombek and Connor, 2012 ). Cognitive self-regulation appears to be strongly associated with academic learning ( Willoughby et al., 2011 ), but emotional self-regulation also contributes through children's adjustment to school and attitudes toward learning. In addition, both cognitive and emotional self-regulation contribute to variance in attention, competence motivation, and persistence ( Bassett et al., 2012 ; Willoughby et al., 2011 ).

In addition, differences in self-regulation competencies raise important issues related to disparities in educational achievement. Children in poverty can have lower self-regulation competencies (e.g., Blair and Razza, 2007 ; Blair et al., 2010 ; Bull and Scerif, 2001 ; Hackman and Farah, 2009 ; Jenks et al., 2012 ; Kishiyama et al., 2009 ; Masten et al., 2012 ; Mazzocco and Hanich, 2010 ; McLean and Hitch, 1999 ; Raver, 2013 ). One reason is the effect of chronic stress on behavioral and biological capacities for self-control (see discussion of chronic stress and adversity later in this chapter). This risk is exacerbated for children who are also dual language learners ( Wanless et al., 2011 ). Students with special needs are another population who may require focused interventions to develop self-regulation competencies ( Harris et al., 2005 ; Jenks et al., 2012 ; Lyon and Krasnegor, 1996 ; Mazzocco and Hanich, 2010 ; McLean and Hitch, 1999 ; Raches and Mazzocco, 2012 ; Toll et al., 2010 ; Zelazo et al., 2002 ). Students who are gifted and talented may also have exceptional needs in this domain (e.g., Mooji, 2010 ).

Adults who work with children have the opportunity to provide environments, experiences, and curricula that can help develop the competencies needed, including for children whose skills were not optimally developed in the earliest years. Importantly, the goal of such interventions is not to “train” children to suppress behaviors and follow rules. Rather, effective educators and programs provide learning activities and environments that increase children's capacity and disposition to set a goal (e.g., join a pretend play activity, complete a puzzle); develop a plan or strategy; and muster their social, emotional, and cognitive faculties to execute that plan. The science of how children develop and learn indicates that integrating academic learning and self-regulation is a sound approach.

Executive Functions and Learning in Specific Subjects

As already noted and shown in several examples, executive function processes are closely related to achievement in both language and literacy and mathematics ( Best et al., 2011 ; Blair and Razza, 2007 ; Blair et al., 2010 ; Neuenschwander et al., 2012 ), and this has also been shown in science ( Nayfeld et al., 2013 ). In some research, executive function has been correlated similarly with both reading and mathematics achievement across a wide age span (5 to 17 years), suggesting its significant role in academic learning ( Best et al., 2011 ; Blair and Razza, 2007 ; Neuenschwander et al., 2012 ). In contrast, some studies have found that executive function is more strongly associated with mathematics than with literacy or language ( Barata, 2010 ; Blair et al., 2010 ; Ponitz et al., 2009 ; von Suchodoletz and Gunzenhauser, 2013 ). A strong relationship between executive function and mathematics may reflect that mathematics relies heavily on working memory and attention control, requiring the ability to inhibit an automatic response to a single aspect of a problem, to hold relevant information in mind, and to operate on it while shifting attention appropriately among different elements of a problem ( Welsh et al., 2010 ). This relationship is especially important given that mathematics curricula increasingly require higher-order skills, which executive function competencies provides ( Baker et al., 2010 ).

Some research indicates that most executive function competencies correlate significantly with mathematics achievement ( Bull and Scerif, 2001 ), while other studies suggest a greater role for particular executive function competencies in the learning of mathematics for young children—especially inhibitory control ( Blair and Razza, 2007 ) or working memory ( Bull et al., 2008 ; Geary, 2011 ; see also, Geary et al., 2012 ; cf. Neuenschwander et al., 2012 ; Szűcs et al., 2014 ; Van der Ven et al., 2012 ). These latter two competencies have been shown to predict success in mathematics in primary school students ( Toll et al., 2010 ). Working memory tasks have also been shown to predict mathematics learning disabilities, even more so than early mathematical abilities ( Toll et al., 2010 ). Several studies have identified lack of inhibition and working memory as specific deficits for children of lower mathematical ability, resulting in difficulty with switching to and evaluating new strategies for dealing with a particular task ( Bull and Scerif [2001] and Lan and colleagues [2011] found similar results). Persistence, another learning skill that is interrelated with cognitive processes, also has been linked to mathematics achievement for both 3- and 4-year-olds ( Maier and Greenfield, 2008 ).

Executive function competencies may be differentially associated with distinct areas of mathematics. For example, executive function was found to be correlated more with solving word problems than with calculation ( Best et al., 2011 ), and appears to play a role in acquiring new mathematics procedures and developing automatic access to arithmetic facts ( LeFevre et al., 2013 ). Different aspects of working memory also may be related to different mathematical areas ( Simmons et al., 2012 ). Parallel observations have been made for executive function and reading, with executive function playing a larger role in reading comprehension than in decoding.

In addition to the role of executive function in learning mathematics, mathematics activities also contribute to developing executive function. Some mathematics activities may require children to suppress prepotent responses, manipulate abstract information, and remain cognitively flexible. Importantly, neuroimaging studies suggest that executive function may be developed through learning mathematics in challenging activities but not in exercising mathematics once learned ( Ansari et al., 2005 ; Butterworth et al., 2011 ).

Cognitive Skills and Executive Function in Children with Special Needs

Some students with special needs may have a specific lack of certain executive function competencies ( Harris et al., 2005 ; Jenks et al., 2012 ; Lyon and Krasnegor, 1996 ; McLean and Hitch, 1999 ; Raches and Mazzocco, 2012 ; Schoemaker et al., 2014 ; Toll et al., 2010 ; Zelazo et al., 2002 ). Most of the research on executive function deficits in relation to disabilities that affect young children has focused on specific disorders, particularly attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). An early theory posited that ADHD is a lack of the behavioral inhibition required for proficiency with executive functions such as self-regulation of affect, motivation, and arousal; working memory; and synthesis analysis of internally represented information ( Barkley, 1997 ). Research has found that children diagnosed with ADHD are more likely than children without ADHD to have two or more deficits in executive function ( Biederman et al., 2004 ; cf. Shuai et al., 2011 ). A meta-analysis of studies of one measure of executive function, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, suggests that the performance of individuals with ADHD is fairly consistently poorer than that of individuals without clinical diagnoses ( Romine et al., 2004 ). In another study, children with ADHD were found not to have learning problems but rather problems in a measure of inhibitory control, which affected arithmetic calculation (as well as written language) ( Semrud-Clikeman, 2012 ). Other evidence suggests that children diagnosed with ADHD may have deficits not in executive processes themselves but in motivation or response to contingencies, that is, the regulation of effort allocation ( Huang-Pollock et al., 2012 ).

Having ADHD with deficits in executive function, compared to ADHD alone, is associated with an increased risk for grade retention and a decrease in academic achievement ( Biederman et al., 2004 ). The relationship between ADHD and executive functions may also depend on subtype. One study found that children with an inattention ADHD subtype showed deficits in several executive function competencies ( Tymms and Merrell, 2011 ), whereas children with the hyperactive-impulsive ADHD subtype may have fewer executive function deficits ( Shuai et al., 2011 ) and may even have strengths that could be developed in appropriate educational environments.

Deficits in executive function have been studied in other developmental disorders as well, albeit often in less detail. They include autism ( Bühler et al., 2011 ; Hill, 2004 ; Zelazo et al., 2002 ); attention and disruptive behavior problems ( Fahie and Symons, 2003 ; Hughes and Ensor, 2011 ); intellectual disabilities ( Nader-Grosbois and Lefèvre, 2011 ; Neece et al., 2011 ; Vieillevoye and Nader-Grosbois, 2008 ); cerebral palsy ( Jenks et al., 2012 ); Turner syndrome ( Mazzocco and Hanich, 2010 ); developmental dyslexia ( Brosnan et al., 2002 ; cf. Romine and Reynolds, 2005 ); and mathematics learning disabilities ( Toll et al., 2010 ).

Other Learning Skills and Dispositions

Other learning skills that are important to early academic achievement include persistence, curiosity, self-confidence, intrinsic motivation, time perspective (e.g., the willingness to prioritize long-term goals over immediate gratifications), and self-control. The growth of emotional and cognitive self-regulation is also fundamentally related to many of these developing learning skills. In addition, social experiences, discussed later in this chapter, are important for the growth of these learning skills. Note also that although these skills are referred to sometimes as dispositions, they are fostered through early experience and can be supported through intentional caregiving and instructional practices; they are not simply intrinsic traits in the child.

A capacity for focused engagement in learning is apparent from very early in life, although it is also true that these learning competencies develop significantly throughout early childhood as processes of neurobiological development interact with children's social experiences to enable greater persistence, focused attention, delayed gratification, and other components of effective learning and problem solving. As a consequence, very young children are likely to approach new learning situations with enthusiasm and self-confidence but at young ages may not necessarily bring persistence or creativity in confronting and solving challenging problems. Older preschoolers, by contrast, are more self-regulated learners. They approach new learning opportunities with initiative and involvement, and they are more persistent and more likely to solve problems creatively, by proposing their own ideas ( NRC, 2001 ).

Considerable research confirms the importance of these skills to early learning. Individual differences in infants' “mastery motivation” skills—persistence, focus, and curiosity in exploration and problem solving—predict later cognitive abilities and achievement motivation ( Busch-Rossnagel, 2005 ; Morgan et al., 1990 ; Wang and Barrett, 2013 ). In preschool-age children, learning skills that include motivation, engagement, and interest in learning activities have been found in longitudinal studies to predict children's cognitive skills at school entry ( Duncan et al., 2005 , 2007 ). Similarly, these characteristics continue to be associated with reading and mathematics achievement in the early elementary grades ( Alexander et al., 1993 ). Differences in these learning skills are especially associated with academic achievement for children in circumstances of economic disadvantage who face various kinds of self-regulatory challenges ( Blair and Raver, 2012 ; Howse et al., 2003a ).

Much of school success requires that children prioritize longer-term rewards requiring current effort over immediate satisfactions. The classic demonstration of this skill comes from a series of studies led by Walter Mischel beginning in the 1960s. Young children were offered the option of choosing an immediate, smaller reward or a larger reward if they waited to receive it later. For several years developmental outcomes for these children were tracked, which revealed that children who were better able to delay gratification at age 4 scored higher on measures of language skills, academic achievement, planful behaviors, self-reliance, capacity to cope with stress and frustration, and social competence measured in adolescence and adulthood ( Mischel et al., 1988 ). Other studies have reported consistent findings. Early development in the ability to prioritize future, long-term goals over short-term lesser gains improves children's chances of academic achievement and securing and maintaining employment ( Rachlin, 2000 ). Conversely, the inability to delay gratification is associated with young children's aggressive behavior, conduct problems, poorer peer relationships, and academic difficulty during preschool and the transition to elementary school ( Olson and Hoza, 1993 ) as well as later outcomes, including academic failure, delinquency, and substance abuse in adolescence ( Lynam et al., 1993 ; Wulfert et al., 2002 ).

The ways that children view themselves as learners are also important. Young children's self-perceived capability to master learning challenges develops early and exerts a continuing influence on their academic success. Early self-evaluations of competence are based on the positive and negative evaluations of children's behavior and competence by parents ( Stipek et al., 1992 ). Parent and educator expectations for children's success remain important. High parent expectations for children's school achievement are associated with children's later academic performance, and this is also true of educator expectations. In one longitudinal study, teacher expectations for children's math achievement in grades 1 and 3 directly predicted children's scores on standardized achievement tests 2 years later, and expectations for reading achievement had indirect associations with later reading scores. There was also evidence in this study that expectations were especially influential for academically at-risk students ( Hinnant et al., 2009 ).

Messages from parents and educators are also important in shaping how children attribute their own success and failure which, in turn, predicts their future effort and expectations of success. Children develop implicit theories in the early years about who they are as a person and what it means to be intelligent. Some children come to view intelligence as a fixed trait (i.e., one is either smart or not), whereas others see it as a more malleable trait that can be changed through effort and persistence. Educators and parents who approach learning goals by promoting and rewarding effort, persistence, and willingness to take on challenging problems increase children's motivation and their endorsement of effort as a path to success. In contrast, children receiving messages that intelligence is stable and cannot be improved through hard work are discouraged from pursuing difficult tasks, particularly if they view their abilities as low ( Heyman and Dweck, 1992 ). These patterns of “helpless” versus “mastery-oriented” motivation are learned in the preschool years and remain stable over time ( Smiley and Dweck, 1994 ).

These perceptions and patterns of motivation can be especially significant as children learn academic subjects, such as mathematics ( Clements and Sarama, 2012 ). People in the United States have many negative beliefs and attitudes about mathematics ( Ashcraft, 2006 ). One deeply embedded cultural belief is that achievement in mathematics depends mainly on native aptitude or ability rather than effort. Research shows that the belief in the primacy of native ability hurts students and, further, it is simply untrue.

Throughout their school careers, students who believe—or are helped to understand—that they can learn if they try working longer on tasks have better achievement than those who believe that either one “has it” (or “gets it”) or does not ( McLeod and Adams, 1989 ; Weiner, 1986 ). Researchers have estimated that students should be successful about 70 percent of the time to maximize motivation ( Middleton and Spanias, 1999 ). If students are directly assured that working hard to figure out problems, including making errors and being frustrated, are part of the learning process it can diminish feelings of embarrassment and other negative emotions at being incorrect. In contrast, students' learning can be impeded if educators define success only as rapid, correct responses and accuracy only as following the educator's example ( Middleton and Spanias, 1999 ). In addition, students will build positive feelings about mathematics if they experience it as a sense-making activity. Most young students are motivated to explore numbers and shapes and have positive feelings about mathematics ( Middleton and Spanias, 1999 ). However, after only a couple of years in typical schools, they begin to believe that only some people have the ability to do math.

A related pattern relating perceptions and emotions to learning is seen with students who experience mathematics anxiety. Primary grade students who have strong math anxiety, even alongside strong working memory, have been found to have lower mathematics achievement because working memory capacity is co-opted by math anxiety ( Beilock, 2001 ; Ramirez et al., 2013 ). Research has shown that primary grade students who “feel panicky” about math have increased activity in brain regions that are associated with fear, which decreases activity in brain regions associated with problem solving ( Young et al., 2012 ). Early identification and treatment of math anxiety may prevent children with high potential from avoiding mathematics and mathematics courses ( Ramirez et al., 2013 ).

  • SOCIOEMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

The development of social and emotional competence is an important part of child development and early learning. Socioemotional competence has been described as a multidimensional construct that contributes to the ability to understand and manage emotions and behavior; to make decisions and achieve goals; and to establish and maintain positive relationships, including feeling and showing empathy for others. Although their importance is widely recognized, universal agreement is lacking on how to categorize and define these areas of development. The Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning offers a summary construct with five interrelated groups of competencies that together encompass the areas typically considered to be part of socioemotional competence (see Figure 4-2 ).

Elements of socioemotional competence. SOURCE: Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning (http://www.casel.org/social-and-emotional-learning/core-competencies, accessed March 24, 2015).

Socioemotional competence increasingly is viewed as important for a child's early school adjustment and for academic success at both the preschool and K-12 levels ( Bierman et al., 2008a , b ; Denham and Brown, 2010 ; Heckman et al., 2013 ; La Paro and Pianta, 2000 ; Leerkes et al., 2008 ). A growing body of research addresses the relationship between dimensions of socioemotional competence and cognitive and other skills related to early learning and later academic achievement ( Bierman et al., 2008a , b ; Graziano et al., 2007 ; Howse et al., 2003b ; Miller et al., 2006 ). Socioemotional development early in life also increasingly is understood to be critically important for later mental well-being, and for contributing to subsequent mental health problems when there are enduring disturbances in socioemotional functions ( IOM and NRC, 2009 ; Leckman and March, 2011 ).

There are several reasons why socioemotional development is important to early learning and academic success. As discussed in detail later in this section, early learning is a social activity in which these skills are important to the interactions through which learning occurs and is collaboratively shared. Socioemotional competence gives children the capacity to engage in academic tasks by increasing their ability to interact constructively with teachers, work collaboratively with and learn from peers, and dedicate sustained attention to learning ( Denham and Brown, 2010 ). Further, behavioral and emotional problems not only impede early learning but also pose other risks to long-term success. Substantial research has examined the relationship between delays and deficits in children's social skills and challenging behavior, such as serious problems getting along with peers or cooperating with educators ( Zins et al., 2007 ). When challenging behavior is not resolved during the early years, children with persistent early socioemotional difficulties experience problems in socialization, school adjustment, school success, and educational and vocational adaptation in adolescence and adulthood (e.g., Dunlap et al., 2006 ; Lane et al., 2008 ; Nelson et al., 2004 ). Thus attention to socioemotional competence also is important from the perspective of addressing early emerging behavior problems before they become more serious.

A variety of evidence-based approaches can be implemented to strengthen socioemotional competence for young children ( Domitrovich et al., 2012 ; IOM and NRC, 2009 ). These approaches typically entail strategies designed to improve children's emotion identification and understanding combined with the development of social problem-solving skills; practice in simple emotion regulation strategies; and coaching in prosocial behavior through strategies that can involve role playing, modeling, and reinforcement of socially competent behavior. Importantly, as discussed further in Chapter 6 , these strategies can be incorporated into daily classroom practice to provide children with everyday socioemotional learning.

Relational Security and Emotional Well-Being

As noted earlier in the discussion of self-regulation, socioemotional competencies contribute to the development of relationships with parents, educators, and peers. The development of positive relationships enables young children to participate constructively in learning experiences that are inherently social. The emotional support and security provided by positive relationships contributes in multifaceted ways to young children's learning success. Research on the security of attachment between young children and their parents illustrates this point, and provides a basis for considering the nature of children's relationships with educators and peers.

A secure parent–child attachment is widely recognized as foundational for healthy development, and the evolving understanding of the importance of attachment encompasses research in developmental psychology and developmental neuroscience (as discussed in Chapter 3 ) ( Schore and Schore, 2008 ; Thompson, 2013 ). Research has shown that securely attached children receive more sensitively responsive parental care, and in turn develop greater social skills with adults and peers and greater social and emotional understanding of others, show more advanced moral development, and have a more positive self-concept (see Thompson, 2013 , for a review). Securely attached children also have been found to be more advanced in cognitive and language development and to show greater achievement in school ( de Ruiter and van IJzendoorn, 1993 ; van Ijzendoorn et al., 1995 ; West et al., 2013 ). This association has been found for infants, preschool-age children, and older children, suggesting that it is fairly robust.

Most researchers believe that the association between attachment security and cognitive competence derives not from a direct link between the two, but from a number of processes mediating a secure attachment and the development of cognitive and language skills ( O'Connor and McCartney, 2007 ). The mediators that have been studied include the following:

  • Early confidence and competence at exploration—One of the functions of a secure attachment is to enable infants and young children to better explore the environment, confident in the caregiver's support and responsiveness if things go awry. An extensive research literature, focused primarily on young children, confirms this expectation ( van Ijzendoorn et al., 1995 ). Early in life, exploratory interest is likely to lead to new discoveries and learning.
  • Maternal instruction and guidance—Consistent with the sensitivity that initially contributes to a secure attachment, considerable research has shown that the mothers of securely attached children continue to respond supportively in ways that promote the child's social and cognitive achievements ( Thompson, in press) . In particular, these mothers talk more elaboratively with their children in ways that foster the children's deeper understanding and in so doing help support the children's cognitive growth ( Fivush et al., 2006 ). Furthermore, increased mother–child conversation is likely to foster the child's linguistic skills.
  • Children's social competence with adults and peers—Securely attached children develop enhanced social skills and social understanding that enhance their competence in interactions with peers and adults in learning environments. In this light, their greater cognitive and language competencies may derive, at least in part, from more successful interactions with social partners in learning contexts. (See the detailed discussion of social interaction as a forum for cognitive growth later in this section.)
  • Self-regulatory competence—Several studies suggest that securely attached children are more skilled in the preschool and early grade school years at self-regulation, especially as it is manifested in greater social competence and emotion regulation. Self-regulatory competence also may extend to children's greater attentional focus, cognitive self-control, and persistence in learning situations. In one recent report, the association of attachment security with measures of school engagement in the early primary grades was mediated by differences in children's social self-control; attentional impulsivity also varied with the security of attachment ( Drake et al., 2014 ; Thompson, 2013 ).
  • Stress management—One of the functions of a secure attachment is that it supports the social buffering of stress by providing children with an adult who regularly assists them in challenging circumstances. The social buffering of stress may be an especially important aspect of how a secure attachment contributes to cognitive competence for children in disadvantaged circumstances when stress is likely to be chronic and potentially overwhelming (see Gunnar and Donzella, 2002 , for a review; Nachmias et al., 1996 ) (see also the discussion of chronic stress and adversity later in this chapter).

In addition to the substantial research on parent–child attachment and the development of cognitive competence, a smaller but significant research literature focuses on the development of attachments between children and educators and how those attachments contribute to children's success in structured learning environments (e.g., Ahnert et al., 2006 ; Birch and Ladd, 1998 ; Howes and Hamilton, 1992 ; Howes et al., 1998 ; Ladd et al., 1999 ; Mitchell-Copeland et al., 1997 ; Pianta and Stuhlman, 2004a , b ). In some respects, the processes connecting children's learning achievement with the supportive, secure relationships they develop with educators are similar to those observed with parent–child attachments. As with their parents and other caregivers, children develop attachments to their educators, and the quality of those relationships has a significant and potentially enduring influence on their classroom success ( Hamre and Pianta, 2001 ). Secure, warm relationships with educators facilitate young children's self-confidence when learning and assist in their self-regulatory competence, and there is evidence that children with such relationships in the classroom learn more than those who have more difficult relationships with educators ( NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2003 ; Pianta and Stuhlman, 2004b ).

In one study, preschoolers identified as academically at risk based on demographic characteristics and reports of problems by their kindergarten teachers were followed to the end of first grade ( Hamre and Pianta, 2005 ). The children with first-grade teachers who provided high amounts of instructional and emotional support had achievement scores comparable to those of their low-risk peers. Support was measured by teacher behaviors such as verbal comments promoting effort, persistence, and mastery; conversations using open-ended questions; encouragement of child responsibility; sensitivity; and a positive classroom climate. O'Connor and McCartney (2007) likewise found that positive educator–child relationships from preschool through third grade were associated with higher third-grade achievement, and that much of this achievement derived from how positive relationships promoted children's classroom engagement.

Positive educator–child relationships are especially important during the transition to school, when children's initial expectations about school and adjustment to its social demands take shape ( Ladd et al., 1999 ; Silver et al., 2005 ). Children who develop more positive relationships with their teachers in kindergarten are more positive about attending school, more excited about learning, and more self-confident. In the classroom they achieve more compared with children who experience more conflicted or troubled relationships with their teachers ( Birch and Ladd, 1997 ; NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2003 ; Pianta and Stuhlman, 2004b ). A positive relationship with educators may be especially important for children who are at risk of academic difficulty because such a relationship can provide support for self-confidence and classroom involvement ( Pianta et al., 1995 ).

A similar association is seen for peer relationships. Children who experience greater friendship and peer acceptance tend to feel more positive about coming to school, participate more in activities in the classroom, and achieve more in kindergarten ( Ladd et al., 1996 , 1997 ). Peer rejection is associated with less classroom participation, poorer academic performance, and a desire to avoid school ( Buhs and Ladd, 2001 ).

Taken together, research documenting the association between the security of attachment and the development of cognitive and language competence, as well as the stronger academic performance of securely attached children, highlights the multiple ways in which supportive relationships contribute to early learning. In particular, such relationships with parents, educators, and even peers provide immediate support that helps children focus their energies on learning opportunities, and they also foster the development of social and cognitive skills that children enlist in learning.

Emotion Regulation and Self-Management

Another element of socioemotional competence, touched on earlier in the section on general learning competencies, is self-regulation of emotion, or emotion regulation, which can affect learning behaviors and relationships with adults and peers. As noted in that earlier discussion, emotion regulation is closely intertwined with cognitive self-regulation and executive function. Emotion regulation processes include emotional and motivational responses to situations involving risk and reward (e.g., Kerr and Zelazo, 2004 ). They are frequently inhibitory; that is, they include the ability to suppress one response (e.g., grabbing a toy from another) so as to respond in a better way (asking for or sharing the toy). The development of emotion regulation and other forms of self-management in the early years is based on slowly maturing regions of the prefrontal cortex that continue to develop throughout adolescence and even early adulthood. Thus, early learners are maturationally challenged to manage their attention, emotions, and behavioral impulses effectively in a care setting or classroom.

Because they have difficulty cooperating or resolving conflicts successfully, children who lack effective self-regulation do not participate in a productive way in classroom activities—including learning activities ( Broidy et al., 2003 ; Ladd et al., 1999 ; Saarni et al., 1998 ). Children with poor emotion regulation skills may act disruptively and aggressively; they then receive less support from their peers, which in turn may undermine their learning ( Valiente et al., 2011 ). Poor emotion regulation also diminishes positive educator–child interactions, which, as discussed in the previous section, has been shown to predict poor academic performance and behavior problems ( Hamre and Pianta, 2001 ; Neuenschwander et al., 2012 ; Raver and Knitzer, 2002 ).

Coupled with joint attention and delay of gratification, self-regulation skills are linked to social competence and ease the transition to kindergarten ( Huffman et al., 2000 ; McIntyre et al., 2006 ). Children with difficulty regulating emotion in preschool and kindergarten often display inappropriate behavior, fail to pay attention (affecting whether they recall and process information), and have difficulty following instructions, all of which contribute to learning problems ( Eisenberg et al., 2010 ). Unfortunately, these difficulties tend to be common in preschool and kindergarten. They are an important determinant of whether educators and parents regard young children as “ready for school” ( Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2000 ).

Some researchers also suggest that emotion regulation in preschool and kindergarten serves as an early indicator of later academic success ( Graziano et al., 2007 ; Howse et al., 2003b ; Trentacosta and Izard, 2007 ). In preschool, McClelland and colleagues (2007) found not only that emotion regulation predicted early skills in literacy and mathematics but also that growth in emotion regulation in 4-year-olds over a 1-year period was linked to greater gains in literacy, vocabulary, and math compared with children showing less growth. Reading disability and problem behavior may be a “chicken or egg” problem: students who have behavior problems in first grade are more likely to have reading difficulties in third grade and students who have reading difficulties in first grade are more likely to exhibit behavior problems in third grade ( Morgan et al., 2008 ). Thus a particularly effective learning environment may be one that provides both effective reading instruction and support for behavioral self-regulation ( Connor et al., 2014 ).

Young children are better enabled to exercise self-regulation in the company of educators who have developmentally appropriate expectations for their self-control, provide predictable routines, and offer guidance that scaffolds their developing skills of self-management, especially in the context of carefully designed daily practices in a well-organized setting ( Bodrova and Leong, 2012 ). Indeed, in an intervention for academically at-risk young children, the Chicago School Readiness Project gave Head Start teachers specialized training at the beginning of the year in classroom management strategies to help lower-income preschoolers better regulate their own behavior. At the end of the school year, these children showed less impulsiveness, fewer disruptive behaviors, and better academic performance compared with children in classrooms with teachers who received a different training regimen ( Raver et al., 2009 , 2011 ).

Conclusion About the Ability to Self-Regulate The ability to self-regulate both emotion and cognitive processes is important for learning and academic achievement, affecting children's thinking, motivation, self-control, and social interactions. Children's progress in this ability from birth through age 8 is influenced by the extent to which relationships with adults, learning environments, and learning experiences support this set of skills, and their progress can be impaired by stressful and adverse circumstances.

Social and Emotional Understanding

As described earlier in this chapter, even infants and toddlers have an implicit theory of mind for understanding how certain mental states are associated with people's behavior. From their simple and straightforward awareness that people act intentionally and are goal directed; that people have positive and negative feelings in response to things around them; and that people have different perceptions, goals, and feelings, young children develop increasingly sophisticated understanding of the mental experiences that cause people to act as they do ( Wellman, 2011 ). They realize, for example, that people's beliefs about reality can be accurate or may be mistaken, and this realization leads to their understanding that people can be deceived, that the child's own thoughts and feelings need not be disclosed, and that not everybody can be believed ( Lee, 2013 ; Mills, 2013 ). They appreciate that people's thinking may be biased by expectations, prior experiences, and desires that cause them to interpret the same situation in very different ways ( Lalonde and Chandler, 2002 ). They also begin to appreciate how personality differences among people can cause different individuals to act in the same situation in very different ways ( Heyman and Gelman, 1999 ).

These remarkable advances in social understanding are important to children's developing socioemotional skills for interacting with educators and peers. These advances also are fostered by children's classroom experiences. Children learn about how people think and feel from directly observing; asking questions; and conversing about people's mental states with trusted informants, such as parents ( Bartsch and Wellman, 1995 ; Dunn, 2002 ; Thompson et al., 2003 ). Similarly, interactions with educators and peers provide young children with apt lessons in mutual understanding and perspective taking, cooperation, conflict management, personality differences and similarities, and emotional understanding in an environment where these skills are developing. This is especially so when educators can use children's experiences as forums for developing social and emotional understanding, such as when they explain why peers are feeling the way they do, suggest strategies for resolving conflict over resources or a point of view, or involve children in collective decision making involving different opinions.

Self-Awareness and Early Learning

How young children think of themselves as learners, and in particular their self-perceived efficacy in mastering new understanding, is an early developing and continuously important influence on their academic success. Young children become increasingly sensitive to the positive and negative evaluations of their behavior by parents, which serve as the basis for their self-evaluations ( Stipek et al., 1992 ). In one study, mothers who provided positive evaluations, gentle guidance, and corrective feedback during teaching tasks with their 2-year-olds had children who, 1 year later, were more persistent and less likely to avoid difficult challenges. By contrast, mothers who were intrusively controlling of their toddlers had children who, 1 year later, responded with shame when they had difficulty ( Kelley et al., 2000 ). Gunderson and colleagues (2013) found that 14- to 38-month-old children whose parents praised their efforts during unstructured home observations were more likely, as third graders, to believe that abilities are malleable and can be improved.

An extensive research literature documents the effects of parents' and educators' performance feedback on children's self-concept and motivation to succeed. Most of this research was conducted with older children and adolescents because of their more sophisticated understanding of differences in ability (see Wigfield et al., 2006 , for a review); however, preschoolers and early primary grade students are also sensitive to success and failure and to their imputed causes. In a study by Cimpian and colleagues (2007) , for example, 4-year-old children were represented by puppets whose performance was praised by a teacher using either generic feedback (“You are a good drawer.”) to imply trait-based (ability-centered) success or nongeneric feedback (“You did a good job drawing.”) to imply situation-based (effort-centered) success. The children did not differ in their self-evaluations after hearing praise of either kind, but when their puppet subsequently made a mistake and was criticized for it, the 4-year-olds who had heard generic feedback evaluated their performance and the situation more negatively than did children hearing nongeneric feedback, suggesting that they interpreted criticism as reflecting deficits in their ability. Similar results have been reported with kindergarteners by Kamins and Dweck (1999) and by Zentall and Morris (2010) , with the latter indicating that task persistence as well as self-evaluation were strengthened by the use of nongeneric performance feedback.

Parent and educator expectations for children's academic success also are important influences. High parental expectations for children's school achievement are associated with children's later academic performance, and this association often is mediated by the greater involvement of parents in the preschool or school program and other practices that support children's school success ( Baroody and Dobbs-Oates, 2009 ; Englund et al., 2004 ; Mantzicopoulos, 1997 ). The role of educator expectations in children's success is illustrated by a longitudinal study in which teacher expectations for children's math achievement in grades 1 and 3 directly predicted children's scores on standardized achievement tests 2 years later; teacher expectations for reading achievement had indirect associations with later reading scores. The results of this study also suggest that teacher expectations were especially influential for academically at-risk students ( Hinnant et al., 2009 ).

Social Interaction as a Forum for Cognitive Growth

A wider perspective on the importance of socioemotional skills for academic success is gained by considering the importance of social experiences for early learning. Contemporary research has led developmental scientists to understand the mind's development as deriving jointly from the child's naturally inquisitive activity and the catalysts of social experience. Sometimes these social experiences are in formal teaching and other pedagogical experiences, but often they take the form of adults and children sharing in activities that provide the basis for early learning, in a kind of “guided participation” (e.g., Rogoff, 1991 ). These activities can be as simple as the one-sided “conversation” parents have with their infant or toddler from which language skills develop, or the shared sorting of laundry into piles of similar color, or labeling of another child's feelings during an episode of peer conflict. In short, considerable early learning occurs in the course of a young child's ordinary interactions with a responsive adult.

Social experiences provide emotional security and support that enables learning and can also contribute to the development of language, number skills, problem solving, and other cognitive and learning skills that are foundational for school readiness and academic achievement. Through their interactions with children, adults provide essential stimulation that provides rapidly developing mental processes with catalysts that provoke further learning. Conversely, the lack of these catalysts contributes to learning disparities by the time that children become preschoolers. These processes are well illustrated by considering the growth of language and literacy skills and of mathematical understanding.

It is difficult to think of any child developing language apart from social interactions with others. As discussed earlier in this chapter, variability in these experiences, beginning in infancy, helps account for socioeconomic disparities in language and mathematical skills that are apparent by the time children enter school. In a widely cited study, Hart and Risley (1995) recorded 1 hour of naturally occurring speech in the homes of 42 families at monthly intervals beginning when children were 7-9 months old and continuing until they turned 3 years. They found that by age 3, children living in the most socioeconomically advantaged families had a working vocabulary that was more than twice the size of that of children growing up in the most disadvantaged families. The latter group of children also was adding words more slowly than their advantaged counterparts. The differences in children's vocabulary size were associated, in part, with how many words were spoken to them during the home observations, with a much richer linguistic environment being characteristic of the most advantaged homes. In addition, words were used in functionally different ways, with a much higher ratio of affirmative-to-prohibitive language being used in the most advantaged homes and a much lower ratio (i.e., below 1) being characteristic of the most disadvantaged homes. Differences in the language environment in which children grew up were, in other words, qualitative as well as quantitative in nature. Further research with a subset of 29 families in this sample showed that 3-year-olds' vocabulary size significantly predicted their scores on standardized tests of language skill in third grade ( Hart and Risley, 1995 ).

A later study by Fernald and colleagues (2013) confirms and extends these findings. A sample of 48 English-learning infants from families varying in socioeconomic status was followed from 18 to 24 months. At 18 months, significant differences between infants from higher- and lower-income families were already seen in vocabulary size and in real-time language processing efficiency. By 24 months, a 6-month gap was found between the two groups in processing skills related to language development. A companion study by Weisleder and Fernald (2013) with 29 lower-income Spanish-speaking families found that infants who experienced more child-directed speech at 19 months had larger vocabularies and greater language processing efficiency at 24 months. But adult speech that was simply overheard by infants (i.e., not child directed) at 19 months had no association with later language ( Schneidman et al., 2013 ). These studies indicate that child-directed speech, and perhaps the social interaction that accompanies it, is what strengthens infants' language processing efficiency. As in the Hart and Risley (1995) study, differences in family language environments were both qualitative and quantitative in nature. These findings are important in light of the association between the socioeconomic status of children's families and their language skills ( Bradley and Corwyn, 2002 ).

The findings of these studies are consistent with those of studies of the social experiences in and outside the home that promote language learning in early childhood. (See also the section on language and literacy under “Learning Specific Subjects” earlier in this chapter.) According to one longitudinal study, language and literacy skills in kindergarten were predicted by several aspects of the language environment at home and in classrooms in the preschool years. The characteristics of adult language that stimulated young children's language development included adult use of varied vocabulary during conversations with children; extended discourse on a single topic (rather than frequent topic switching); and diversity of language-related activities, including storybook reading, conversation related to children's experiences and interests, language corrections, and pretend play ( Dickinson, 2003 ; Dickinson and Porche, 2011 ; Dickinson and Tabors, 2001 ). These elements of the early childhood social environment predicted both kindergarten language skills and fourth-grade language and reading abilities. Other studies show that extensive use of descriptive language (e.g., labeling and commenting on people's actions) related to the child's current experience contributes to the quality of children's language development. Shared storybook reading also has been found to enhance the language skills of young children in lower-income homes ( Raikes et al., 2006 ). Stated differently, what matters is not just how much language young children are exposed to but the social and emotional contexts of language shared with an adult.

Language and literacy development is a major focus of instruction in prekindergarten and K-3 classrooms, and the instructional strategies used by teachers are both more formal and more sophisticated than those used in early childhood classrooms. Duke and Block (2012) have noted that in primary grade classrooms, vocabulary, reading comprehension, and conceptual and content knowledge are not adequately emphasized. The practices that would enhance early reading skills are embedded in children's social experiences with educators and peers in the classroom. They involve children interacting with partners throughout reading activity, and teachers explaining and discussing vocabulary terms and encouraging children to make personal connections with the concepts in the text.

Number Concepts and Mathematics

Language and literacy skills are the best-studied area in which early social experiences are influential, but they are not the only skills for which social interactions are important. Social experiences also are important for mathematics, such as for developing an understanding of numbers as well as early number and spatial/geometric language. Infants have an approximate number system that enables them to distinguish different quantities provided that the numerical ratio between them is not small, and this discrimination ability improves with increasing age (see Box 4-5 earlier in this chapter). There is some evidence that early individual differences in this ability are consistent during the first year and predict later mathematical abilities, although the reason for this remains unclear ( Libertus and Brannon, 2010 ; Starr et al., 2013 ). Toddlers also are beginning to comprehend certain number principles, such as one-to-one correspondence ( Slaughter et al., 2011 ). How adults talk about number is important. In one study, everyday parent–child discourse was recorded for 90 minutes every 4 months when the child was between 14 and 30 months old. The amount of parents' spontaneous “number talk” in these conversations (e.g., counting objects, references to time) was predictive of children's cardinal number knowledge (i.e., the knowledge that “four” refers to sets with four items) at 46 months ( Levine et al., 2010 ). Particularly important was when parents counted or labeled fairly large sets of objects within the child's view, providing concrete referents for parent–child interaction over number ( Gunderson and Levine, 2011 ).

Klibanoff and colleagues (2006) found that in early childhood, teachers' “math language”—that is, the frequency of their verbal references to number and geometric concepts—varied greatly for different teachers, but it significantly predicted progress in preschoolers' mathematical knowledge over the course of the school year. Similarly, another study found that parents' number-related activities at home with their young children were highly variable, but parents who engaged in more of these activities had children with stronger mathematical skill on standardized tests ( Blevins-Knabe and Musun-Miller, 1996 ). These practices in the classroom and at home help explain the significant socioeconomic disparities in number understanding by the time children arrive at school ( Klibanoff et al., 2006 ; Saxe et al., 1987 ). In addition to spoken references to numerical and geometric concepts, adults stimulate developing mathematical understanding when they incorporate these concepts into everyday activities, including games and other kinds of play; prompt children's explanations for numerical inferences; probe their understanding; and relate mathematical ideas to everyday experience ( NRC, 2009 ). Unfortunately, the quality of mathematical instruction is highly variable in preschool and early primary grades (discussed further in Chapter 6 ).

Taken together, these studies suggest the diverse ways in which social experiences provide catalysts for children's developing language and number skills that are the focus of later academic work. In these domains, adult practices provide essential cognitive stimulants beginning in infancy. Similar practices—adapted to young children's developing skills—remain important as children proceed through the primary grades.

Relationships and Early Learning: Implications for Adults

The relationship of an adult to a child—the emotional quality of their interaction, the experiences they have shared, the adult's beliefs about the child's capabilities and characteristics—helps motivate young children's learning, inspire their self-confidence, and provide emotional support to engage them in new learning.

Commonplace interactions provide contexts for supporting the development of cognitive and learning skills and the emotional security in which early learning thrives. Applauding a toddler's physical skills or a second-grader's writing skills, counting together the leaves on the sidewalk or the ingredients of a recipe, interactively reading a book, talking about a sibling's temper tantrum or an episode of classroom peer conflict—these and other shared experiences contribute to young children's cognitive development and early learning.

Conclusion About Socioemotional Development Socioemotional development contributes to the growth of emotional security that enables young children to fully invest themselves in new learning and to the growth of cognitive skills and competencies that are important for learning. These capacities are essential because learning is inherently a social process. Young children's relationships—with parents, teachers, and peers—thus are central to the learning experiences that contribute to their later success.
  • PHYSICAL DEVELOPMENT AND HEALTH

Child development and early learning are closely intertwined with child health. Indeed, each is a foundation for outcomes in the other: health is a foundation for learning, while education is a determinant of health ( Zimmerman and Woolf, 2014 ). The Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University (2010) has described three foundational areas of child health and development that contribute to physical and mental well-being:

  • Stable and responsive relationships—Such relationships provide young children with consistent, nurturing, and protective interactions with adults that enhance their learning and help them develop adaptive capacities that promote well-regulated stress response systems.
  • Safe and supportive physical, chemical, and built environments—Such environments provide physical and emotional spaces that are free from toxins and fear, allow active exploration without significant risk of harm, and offer supports for families raising young children.
  • Sound and appropriate nutrition—Such nutrition includes health-promoting food intake as well as eating habits, beginning with the future mother's nutritional status even before conception.

This section examines interrelated topics of physical development, child health, nutrition, and physical activity and then touches on partnerships between the health and education sectors (also discussed in Chapter 5 ).

Physical Development

Physical development goes hand-in-hand with cognitive development in young children, and progress in one domain often relies on progress in the other. Similar to cognitive development, typical physical development follows a common trajectory among children but with individual differences in the rate of development. A child's physical development encompasses healthy physical growth; the development of sensory systems, including vision and hearing; and development of the ability to use the musculoskeletal system for gross motor skills that involve large body movements as well as fine motor skills that require precision and the controlled production of sound for speaking. Sensory and motor development are critical for both everyday and classroom activities that contribute to cognitive development, early learning, and eventually academic achievement.

Young children's growth in gross and fine motor skills develops throughout the birth through age 8 continuum—early on from holding their head up; rolling over; standing, crawling, and walking; to grasping cereal, picking up blocks, using a fork, tying shoelaces, and writing. A number of recent studies have focused on the relationships among the development of fine and gross motor skills in infants and young children, cognitive development, and school readiness. For example, one study found that students showing deficiencies in fine motor skills exhibited lower math and verbal scores ( Sandler et al., 1992 ), and more recent studies have also shown that fine motor skills were strongly linked to later achievement ( Grissmer et al., 2010a ; Pagani and Messier, 2012 ). Some of the same neural infrastructure in the brain that controls the learning process during motor development are also involved in the control of learning in cognitive development ( Grissmer et al., 2010a ). The evidence of the impact of motor skills on cognitive development and readiness for school calls for a shift in curricula to include activities that focus on fine motor skills, to include the arts, physical education, and play ( Grissmer et al., 2010b ).

Child Health

Health has an important influence on early learning and academic achievement. Hair and colleagues (2006) found that poor health can be as important in contributing to struggles with academic performance in first grade as language and cognitive skills, along with lack of social skills. Not only are healthy children better prepared to learn, but participation in high-quality early childhood programs leads to improved health in adulthood, setting the stage for intergenerational well-being. Data from Head Start and from the Carolina Abecedarian Project indicate that high-quality, intensive interventions can prevent, or at least delay, the onset of physical and emotional problems from adolescence into adulthood ( Campbell et al., 2014 ; Carneiro and Ginja, 2012 ). Data from a national longitudinal survey show that involvement in Head Start was associated with fewer behavior problems and serious health problems, such as 29 percent less obesity in males at 12 and 13 years of age. In addition, Head Start participants had less depression and obesity as adolescents and 31 percent less involvement in criminal activity as young adults. Similarly, long-term follow-up of adults who were enrolled in the Carolina Abecedarian Project revealed that males in their mid-30s in the project had lower rates of hypertension, obesity, and metabolic syndrome than controls. None of the males in the project had metabolic syndrome, compared with 25 percent of the control group. Further analysis of growth parameters indicated that those who were obese in their mid-30s were on that trajectory by 5 years of age, indicating the need for emphasis on healthy nutrition and regular physical activity beginning in early childhood. These studies suggest that the impact of early care and education programs on physical and emotional health is long term.

Sufficient, high-quality dietary intake is necessary for children's health, development, and learning. Support for providing healthy nutrition for children and their families, including pregnant and expectant mothers, is vital. Adequate protein, calories, and nutrients are needed for brain development and function. While the rapid brain growth and development that occurs in infants and toddlers may make children in this age group particularly vulnerable to dietary deficiencies, nutrition remains important as certain brain regions continue to develop through childhood into adolescence.

Nutrients, Cognitive Development, and Academic Performance

Deficiencies in protein, energy, and micronutrients such as iron, zinc, selenium, iodine, and omega-3 fatty acids have been linked to adverse effects on cognitive and emotional functioning ( Bryan et al., 2004 ). Research has shown that iron-deficiency anemia (IDA) is associated with lower cognitive and academic performance ( Bryan et al., 2004 ; Nyaradi et al., 2013 ; Taras, 2005 ). Children at an early school age who had IDA as an infant were found to have lower test scores than those who did not have IDA. Effects of severe IDA in infancy have been seen in adolescence. These effects include lower scores in motor functioning; written expression; arithmetic achievement; and some specific cognitive processes, such as spatial memory and selective recall ( NRC and IOM, 2000 ). However, it is not clear whether children with iron deficiency but no anemia have similar outcomes ( Taras, 2005 ). A review of daily iron supplementation in children aged 5-12 years studied in randomized and quasi-randomized controlled trials showed improvement in measures of attention and concentration, global cognitive scores, and, for children with anemia, intelligence quotient (IQ) scores ( Low et al., 2013 ).

IDA in infancy also has been associated with impaired inhibitory control and executive functioning. Altered socioemotional behavior and affect have been seen in infants with iron deficiency regardless of whether anemia is present ( Lozoff, 2011 ). One study found an association between iron supplementation in infancy and increased adaptive behavior at age 10 years, especially in the areas of affect and response to reward, which may have beneficial effects on school performance, mental health, and personal relationships ( Lozoff et al., 2014 ).

Folate and iodine also have been shown to be important for brain development and cognitive performance ( Bougma et al., 2013 ; Bryan et al., 2004 ; Nyaradi et al., 2013 ), although iodine deficiency is rare in the United States. While there is some evidence that zinc, vitamin B 12 , and omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids also may be important for cognitive development, the research on these associations is inconclusive ( Bougma et al., 2013 ; Bryan et al., 2004 ; Taras, 2005 ).

Food Insecurity, Diet Quality, and Healthful Eating

Food insecurity and diet quality in children have both been linked to impaired academic performance and cognitive and socioemotional development. Food insecurity refers to circumstances in which households do not have adequate food to eat, encompassing both inadequate quantity and nutritional quality of food ( ERS, 2014 ). Food insecurity affects development not only by compromising nutrition but also by contributing to a factor in family stress ( Cook and Frank, 2008 ). In 2012, 48 million Americans were food insecure, a fivefold increase from the 1960s and a 57 percent increase from the late 1990s. One in six Americans reported being short of food at least once per year. More than half of affected households were white, and more than half lived outside cities. Indeed, hunger in the suburbs has more than doubled since 2007. Two-thirds of food-insecure households with children have at least one working adult, typically in a full-time job ( McMillan, 2014 ).

A recent review indicates that food insecurity is a “prevalent risk to the growth, health, cognitive, and behavioral potential of low-income children” ( Cook and Frank, 2008 , p. 202). Studies found that children in food-insufficient families were more likely than those in households with adequate food to have fair/poor health; iron deficiency; and behavioral, emotional, and academic problems. Infants and toddlers are at particular risk from food insecurity even at its least severe levels ( Cook and Frank, 2008 ). Cross-sectional studies of children from developing countries have shown an association among general undernutrition and stunting, IQ scores, and academic performance ( Bryan et al., 2004 ). Alaimo and colleagues (2001) found that food insecurity was linked to poorer academic and psychosocial outcomes in children ages 6 to 11 years. Similarly, Florence and colleagues (2008) observed that students with lower overall diet quality were significantly more likely to fail a literacy assessment. Subsequent research has shown that while food insecurity experienced earlier in childhood was associated with emotional problems that appeared in adolescence, cognitive and behavioral problems could be accounted for by differences in the home environments, such as family income and the household's sensitivity to children's needs ( Belsky et al., 2010 ).

Eating breakfast, which can be related to food insecurity, diet quality, and healthful eating habits, has been associated with improved cognitive function, academic performance, and school attendance ( Basch, 2011 ; Hoyland et al., 2009 ; Mahoney et al., 2005 ; Nyaradi et al., 2013 ; Rampersaud et al., 2005 ). According to two reviews of the effect of consuming breakfast in children and adolescents, the evidence suggests that children who consume breakfast—particularly those children whose nutritional status is compromised—may have improved cognitive function, test grades, and school attendance. The positive effects of school breakfast programs may be explained in part by their effect of increasing school attendance ( Hoyland et al., 2009 ; Rampersaud et al., 2005 ). The composition of the breakfast meal may also be important to cognitive performance; a breakfast meal with a low glycemic index, such as oatmeal, has been shown to improve cognitive function ( Cooper et al., 2012 ; Mahoney et al., 2005 ).

In 2011, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) published a report documenting the relationship between healthy eating and increased life expectancy; improved quality of life; and fewer chronic diseases, including cardiovascular disease, obesity, metabolic syndrome, diabetes, and inadequate bone health ( CDC, 2011 ). The report documents the high rate of iron deficiency among obese children and emphasizes the link between dental caries and unhealthy diet. Children are unlikely to follow recommendations for the number of servings of various food groups and they consume higher-than-recommended amounts of saturated fats, sodium, and foods with added sugar. Children's eating behavior and food choices are influenced not only by taste preferences but also by the home environment and parental influences, including household eating rules, family meal patterns, and parents' lifestyles. The school environment influences children's eating behavior as well. The availability of unhealthy options in schools leads to poor choices by children, whereas research has shown that efforts to reduce the availability of sugar-sweetened beverages in the schools can have a positive impact on children's choices ( AAP Committee on School Health, 2004 ). There are also rising concerns about food insecurity in association with obesity; inexpensive foods tend not to be nutritious, and contribute to increasing rates of obesity ( IOM, 2011 ; McMillan, 2014 ).

Physical Activity

A recent Institute of Medicine (IOM) study linked increasing physical activity and enhancing physical fitness to improved academic performance, and found that this can be facilitated by physical activity built into children's days through physical education, recess, and physical classroom activity ( IOM, 2013 ). Likewise, the American Academy of Pediatrics recently highlighted the crucial role of recess as a complement to physical education, suggesting that recess offers cognitive, social, emotional, and physical benefits and is a necessary component of a child's development ( AAP Council on School Health, 2013 ). However, fewer than half of youth meet the current recommendation of at least 60 minutes of vigorous- or moderate-intensity physical activity per day, and recent years have seen a significant downward trend in the offering of daily physical education in schools at all levels ( CDC, 2012 ; GAO, 2012 ). Positive support from friends and family encourages children to engage in physical activity, as do physical environments that are conducive to activity. However, the school environment plays an especially important role. The IOM report recommends that schools provide access to a minimum of 60 minutes of vigorous- or moderate-intensity physical activity per day, including an average of 30 minutes per day in physical education class for students in elementary schools ( IOM, 2013 ).

Partnerships Between Health and Education

Each of the domains of child development and early learning discussed in this chapter can be supported through interventions that involve both the health and education sectors (see also the discussion of continuity among sectors in Chapter 5 ). Specific activities include coordinating vision, hearing, developmental, and behavioral screening to facilitate early identification of children with special needs; completing daily health checks; making appropriate referrals and collaborating with the child's medical home and dental health services; ensuring that immunizations for the entire family and for the early care and education workforce are up to date; modifying and adapting services to meet the individual needs of the child; and providing support to the early care and education workforce to promote more inclusive practices for children with special needs. In addition, teaching and modeling skills in sanitation and personal hygiene will contribute to preventing illness. Furthermore, pediatric health care professionals can make an important contribution by promoting literacy. Extensive research documents the positive impact on early language and literacy development when a pediatric professional gives advice to parents about reading developmentally appropriate books with children as early as 6 months of age ( AAP Council on Early Childhood et al., 2014 ).

There is evidence that coordinated efforts between educational settings and health care services lead to improved health. Head Start, the Infant Health and Development Program, and the Carolina Abecedarian Project are examples of early care and education programs that have integrated health care services into the intervention design, leading to positive health outcomes. Schools also can partner with pediatric health care professionals in their communities to identify opportunities to enhance physical activity in the school setting ( AAP Committee on Sports Medicine and Fitness and AAP Committee on School Health, 2000 ). CDC (2011) has offered recommendations for promoting healthful eating and physical activity that include the following and, if placed in an appropriate developmental context, can be applied to care and education settings for children aged 0-8:

  • Use a coordinated approach to develop, implement, and evaluate healthful eating and physical activity policies and practices.
  • Establish school environments that support healthy eating and activity.
  • Provide a quality school meal program and ensure that students have only appealing, healthy food and beverage choices offered outside of the school meal program.
  • Implement a comprehensive physical activity program with quality physical education as the cornerstone.
  • Implement health education that provides students with the knowledge, attitudes, skills, and experiences needed for healthy eating and physical activity.
  • Provide students with health, mental health, and social services to address healthy eating, physical activity, and related chronic disease prevention.
  • Partner with families and community members in the development and implementation of healthy eating and physical activity policies, practices, and programs.
  • Provide a school employee wellness program that includes healthy eating and physical activity services for all school staff members.
  • Employ qualified persons and provide them with professional development opportunities in staffing physical education; health education; nutrition services; health, mental health, and social services; and supervision of recess, cafeteria time, and out-of-school-time programs.

School-based health centers are another approach to partnering between health and education. They have been associated with improved immunization rates, better adherence to scheduled preventive examinations, and more treatment for illnesses and injuries, as well as fewer emergency room visits. For example, King and colleagues (2006) found that a school-based vaccination program significantly reduced influenza symptoms in the entire school. School-based mental health services also have been shown to be effective in addressing a wide range of emotional and behavioral issues ( Rones and Hoagwood, 2000 ). School-based health centers have been shown to reduce nonfinancial barriers to health care ( Keyl et al., 1996 ), and families also report more satisfaction with their care than in community or hospital settings ( Kaplan et al., 1999 ).

Conclusion About Health, Nutrition, and Early Learning Safe physical and built environments, health, and nutrition are essential to early learning and academic achievement. Food security and adequate nutrition are important to support cognitive development and participation in education, and food insecurity and poor nutrition can contribute to early learning difficulties. Care and education settings provide an opportunity to promote healthful eating and physical activity in learning environments. Providing appropriate health and developmental screenings and follow-up care and services also is important in supporting development and early learning.

Health and Early Learning: Implications for Adults

Healthy children supported by healthy adults are better prepared to learn. Child health begins prior to conception and extends through pregnancy and throughout childhood. Therefore, the early care and education workforce must be prepared to work across generations to provide education, support, and community linkages to ensure that children grow up poised for success. Ongoing federal support for evidence-based home visiting programs for high-risk families that begin early in pregnancy and continue through early childhood is essential. Professionals working in family childcare, early childhood education centers, preschools, and early elementary schools need to have working knowledge of the relationship between health and children's learning and development. Guidance related to nutrition, physical activity, oral health, immunizations, and preventive health care is essential across all early care and education settings. These professionals also need to be provided with supports and opportunities for close collaboration with health care services and their potential integration into or strengthened linkages with the early care and education setting.

  • EFFECTS OF CHRONIC STRESS AND ADVERSITY

As detailed in Chapter 3 , one of the most important advances in developmental science in recent years has been the recognition that the brain incorporates experience into its development. Although experience is important at any age, early experiences are especially formative in the development of the brain's structure and function. Human development is the result of the continuous interaction of genetics and experience. This interplay is true not just of brain development but of other aspects of human development as well. Research in this area encourages developmental scientists as well as parents and practitioners to consider how positive early experiences and enrichment, in formal and informal ways, may have a beneficial influence on the developing brain and in turn on the growth of thinking and learning. The brain's openness to experience is, however, a double-edged sword—adverse early experiences can have potentially significant negative consequences for brain development and early learning.

As discussed in Chapter 3 , evidence indicates that experiences of stress and adversity are biologically embedded and that individual differences exist in the health and developmental consequences of stress. A substantial body of evidence now shows that adversity and stress in early life are associated with higher rates of childhood mental and physical morbidities, more frequent disturbances in developmental trajectories and educational achievement, and lifelong risks of chronic disorders that compromise health and well- being ( Boyce et al., 2012 ; Hertzman and Boyce, 2010 ; Shonkoff et al., 2009 ). Children respond to stress differently. Many exhibit withdrawal, anger and irritability, difficulty paying attention and concentrating, disturbed sleep, repeated and intrusive thoughts, and extreme distress triggered by things that remind them of their traumatic experiences. Some develop psychiatric conditions such as depression, anxiety, posttraumatic stress disorder, and a variety of behavioral disorders ( NCTSN, 2005 ).

What are the circumstances that contribute to chronic adversity and stress for children? All children can experience forms of chronic stress and adversity, but exposures to stress and adversity are socioeconomically layered. Poverty, discussed in more detail below, has been the best studied and is a highly prevalent source of early chronic stress ( Blair and Raver, 2012 ; Evans and Kim, 2013 ; Jiang et al., 2014 ). Young children in the United States also suffer high levels of victimization through child abuse and exposure to domestic violence. The U.S. Department of Health and Human Services reported for the year 2012 that of all child abuse victims, approximately 60 percent were age 8 or younger ( Children's Bureau, 2013 ). The highest rates of child abuse and neglect, including fatalities related to child abuse, were reported for children in the first year of life. Comparable biological and behavioral effects of chronic stress have been studied in children in foster care, in those who experience significant or prolonged family conflict, in those who have a depressed parent, and in those who are abused or neglected (see Thompson, 2014 , for a review).

It is noteworthy that these circumstances include not only those that most people would regard as sources of extreme stress for children (e.g., child abuse), but also those that an adult might regard as less significant because they may be less severe although persistent (e.g., parents' chronic marital conflict, poverty). This broader range of circumstances that children experience as stressful is consistent with the view that, in addition to situations that are manifestly threatening and dangerous, children are stressed by the denial or withdrawal of supportive care, especially when they are young.

Culture also is closely interrelated with stress and adversity. Culture affects the meaning that a child or a family attributes to specific types of traumatic events as well as the ways in which they respond. Because culture also influences expectations regarding the self, others, and social institutions, it can also influence how children and families experience and express distress, grieve or mourn losses, provide support to each other, seek help, and disclose personal information to others. Historical or multigenerational trauma also can influence cultural differences in responses to trauma and loss ( NCTSN Core Curriculum on Childhood Trauma Task Force, 2012 ).

Building on the discussion in Chapter 3 of the biology of chronic stress and adversity, the following sections describe more broadly some of the contributing circumstances and consequences for young children, including the stressors associated with economic adversity; social buffering of stress; and the relationships among stress, learning, and mental health.

The Stressors of Economic Adversity

Children in any economic circumstances can experience stress and adversity, but considerable research on the effects of chronic stress on children's development has focused on children living in families in poverty or with low incomes. The number of children in these conditions of economic adversity is considerable. In 2012, nearly half the children under age 6 lived in poverty or low-income families (defined as up to 200 percent of the federal poverty level, 2 which remains a meagre subsistence) ( Jiang et al., 2014 ). During that same year, more than half the children living with their families in homeless shelters were under the age of 6 ( Child Trends, 2015 ).

The research is clear that poverty as a form of early chronic adversity is a risk factor to long-term physical and mental health, and that for children it can be a significant threat to their capacities to cope with stress, socialize constructively with others, and benefit from the cognitive stimulating opportunities of an early childhood classroom. Socioeconomic disparities in children's experiences of socioemotional adversity and challenging physical environments are well documented (see, e.g., Evans et al., 2012 ). Factors other than economic status itself contribute to the challenges and stresses for children living in low-income families ( Fernald et al., 2013 ). Poverty often is accompanied by the confluence of multiple sources of chronic stress, such as food insufficiency, housing instability (and sometimes homelessness), exposure to violence, environmental noise and toxins, dangerous neighborhoods, poor childcare and schools, family chaos, parents with limited capacity (e.g., resources, education, knowledge/information, time, physical or mental energy) to be supportive and nurturing, parents who are anxious or depressed, parents who are harsh or abusive caregivers, impoverished parent–child communication, and home environments lacking cognitively stimulating activities ( Evans et al., 2012 ; Fernald et al., 2013 ).

As discussed in detail in Chapter 3 , the perturbed biological processes that often accompany economic adversity include changes in the structure and function of children's brain circuitry and dysregulation of their central stress response systems. For these children, therefore, the effects of the chronic stresses associated with economic adversity are likely to contribute to academic, social, and behavioral problems. These problems affect not only early learning and the development of cognitive skills (with impacts on the development of language being best documented) but also the development of learning skills associated with self-regulation and persistence, as well as coping ability, health, and emotional well-being ( Blair and Raver, 2012 ; Evans and Kim, 2013 ).

In addition, developmental consequences related to socioeconomic status are not seen exclusively in children from severely impoverished families. Rather, evidence shows a graded effect of deprivation and adversity across the entire spectrum of socioeconomic status, with even those children from the second-highest social class showing poorer health and development compared with those from families of the very highest socioeconomic status ( Adler et al., 1994 ; Hertzman and Boyce, 2010 ). Moreover, as discussed in Chapter 3 , children are not equally affected by early adverse experiences. Genetic and epigenetic influences may have a role in whether some children are more resilient to early adversity than others ( Rutter, 2012 ).

Detrimental prenatal influences may also be important ( Farah et al., 2008 ; Hackman et al., 2010 ). Although this report focuses on children beginning at birth, child development and early learning also are affected by what a child is exposed to before birth, including influences of family disadvantage. Box 4-6 highlights major research findings on the relationships among family disadvantage, fetal health, and child development.

Family Disadvantage, Fetal Health, and Child Development. Children from different family backgrounds—affected by systemic inequities and disadvantage—start life with starkly different health endowments. As but one example, having a low-birth-weight (more...)

Social Buffering of Stress

The neuroscience of stress has yielded greater understanding of how the effects of stress may be buffered through social support. In behavioral and neurobiological studies of humans and animals, researchers have shown how individuals in adversity show diminished behavioral reactivity and better-regulated cortisol response, among other effects, in the company of people who provide them with emotional support. For children, these individuals often are attachment figures in the family or outside the home.

In health psychology, the benefits of social support for the development and maintenance of healthy practices and the control of disease pathology and healing have been studied since the 1970s (e.g., Cassel, 1976 ; Cobb, 1976 ). Social support also has been recognized as a contributor to psychological well-being for children and youth in difficult circumstances ( Thompson and Goodvin, in press) . In recent years, research on the neurobiology of the social buffering of stress has contributed to a better understanding of why social support has these benefits ( Hostinar et al., 2014 ). In human and animal studies, social companionship in the context of adversity appears to have effects on the biological regulators of hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) activity, contributing to greater regulation of stress reactivity through cortical and limbic influences. Social support also appears to stimulate the down-regulation of the proinflammatory tendencies induced by chronic stress, as well as processes driven by neurohormones, including oxytocin, that have other positive benefits ( Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2010 ). Stated differently, social support not only counters the negative effects of chronic stress reactivity but also stimulates constructive influences that contribute independently to greater self-regulation and well-being ( Hostinar et al., 2014 ). This research is still at an early stage, and establishing reliable associations between brain and behavioral functioning in this area is a work in progress, but research findings are providing increasing support for these processes. In one study, for example, greater maternal support measured when children were preschoolers predicted children's larger hippocampus volume at school age ( Luby et al., 2012 ).

The potential benefits of social support as a buffer of chronic stress reactivity underscore the plasticity of developing behavioral and biological systems. Children in adversity need not suffer long-term harms arising from the effects of chronic stress exposure. In a study of families living in rural poverty, for example, toddlers' chronic exposure to domestic violence was associated with elevated cortisol reactivity. However, this effect was buffered when mothers were observed to respond sensitively to their children ( Hibel et al., 2011 ). Experimental interventions designed to change stressful circumstances and promote positive relationships have yielded similar findings. For example a program aimed at easing young children's transition to new foster care placements and promoting warm, responsive, and consistent relationships with new foster parents provided individualized sessions with child therapists, weekly playgroup sessions, and support for foster parents. This program resulted in a normalization of the children's HPA hyporesponsiveness (an effect of stress discussed in Chapter 3 ) ( Fisher et al., 2007 , 2011 ). Another example comes from an intervention based on attachment theory, which trained caregivers to better interpret and respond affectionately to infants and toddlers in foster care and similarly resulted in a normalization of HPA activity and lower cortisol reactivity ( Dozier et al., 2006 , 2008 ). There may be limits to these potential ameliorative effects, depending on the severity and duration of the exposure to adversity. Children who lived for an extended period in profoundly depriving Romanian orphanages, for example, did not show recovery of dysregulated cortisol reactivity, even after a prolonged period of supportive adoptive care ( Gunnar et al., 2001 ).

Because interventions that can help children recover from the effects of chronic adversity can be expensive and time-consuming, however, it appears sensible to try to prevent these effects from occurring. This can be accomplished by reducing exposure to influences that cause significant stress for children, and by strengthening supportive relationships that can buffer its effects. The development of warm, secure attachments between parents and children illustrates the latter approach. As discussed earlier in this chapter, attachment theorists argue that the reliable support provided by a secure attachment relationship enables infants and children to explore and learn from their experiences confidently with the assurance that a trusted adult is available to assist if difficulty ensues. In this view, secure attachments buffer stress and significantly reduce the child's need to be vigilant for threat or danger. As noted previously, attachment research documents a range of benefits associated with secure parent–child relationships in childhood, including greater language skill, academic achievement, and social competence (see Thompson, 2008 , for a review; West et al., 2013 ). The view that these accomplishments are explained, at least in part, by how secure attachments buffer stress for children is supported by studies documenting the better-regulated cortisol reactivity of young children with secure attachments in challenging situations (see Gunnar and Donzella, 2002 , for a review; Nachmias et al., 1996 ).

Viewed in this light, it appears that the contributions adults make to children's learning extend significantly beyond their reading, conversing, counting, and providing other direct forms of cognitive stimulation. An essential contribution is the safety and security they provide that not only buffers children against significant stress when this occurs, but also enables children to invest themselves in learning opportunities with confidence that an adult will assist them when needed. Such confidence not only enables children to learn more from the opportunities afforded them in the family and outside the home but also fosters their developing self-confidence, curiosity, and other learning skills that emerge in the context of secure relationships ( Thompson, 2008 ). This is a benefit of secure, warm adult–child relationships for all children, not just those in adverse circumstances. This phenomenon is perhaps analogous to that seen in studies in which rat pups with nurturant mothers show enhanced learning and memory in low-stress contexts, whereas pups with nonnurturant mothers show greater proficiency in fear conditioning ( Champagne et al., 2008 ).

One problem, however, is that children in adverse circumstances usually have parents and other caregivers who are affected by the same conditions of adversity. Thus, their parents may not be able to provide them with the support they need. This realization has led to the growth of two-generation interventions that are designed to assist children by providing support to their parents in difficult circumstances ( Chase-Lansdale and Brooks-Gunn, 2014 ).

Stress, Learning, and Mental Health

Children learn readily in contexts of social support and emotional well-being, which derive from positive relationships with those who care for and educate them in the family and outside the home. In these contexts, adults can support and encourage developing competencies, convey positive values about learning and school, and help instill curiosity and self-confidence in children. By contrast, learning and cognitive achievement are hindered when children are troubled. This is the case for children from infancy through adolescence who are living in homes with significant marital conflict, when mothers are chronically depressed, when parents are hostile and coercive, or in other circumstances of family turmoil (e.g., Bascoe et al., 2009 ; Brennan et al., 2013 ; Canadian Paediatric Society, 2004 ; Davies et al., 2008 ).

Socioemotional hindrances to learning and cognitive achievement are apparent very early, before children have begun school, and continue to be important as children move into the primary grades. In educational settings, the emotional effects of problems in educator–child relationships can undermine children's performance and their academic success ( Hamre and Pianta, 2004 ; Jeon et al., 2014 ; Pianta, 1999 ; Pianta and Stuhlman, 2004b ; Skinner and Belmont, 1993 ). As discussed in Chapter 3 , when children are in circumstances of chronic or overwhelming stress, stress hormones affect multiple brain regions, including those relevant to learning, attention, memory, and self-regulation ( McEwen, 2012 ; Ulrich-Lai and Herman, 2009 ). Over time and with continued exposure to stressful circumstances, these neurocognitive processes become altered as a result of the progressive wear and tear of stress hormones on biological systems as they adapt to this chronic stress. As a consequence, immunologic capacities become weakened (contributing to more frequent acute and chronic illness), self-regulation is impaired (contributing to poorer emotion regulation and impulse control), and cognitive and attentional capabilities are blunted ( Danese and McEwen, 2012 ; Lupien et al., 2009 ; Miller et al., 2011 ). For children, these effects can help account for problems in following instructions, paying attention, managing impulsivity, focusing thinking, and controlling emotions in social encounters—each of which can impair classroom performance and academic achievement.

Young children's vulnerability to stress and their reliance on the support of adults are two central considerations in understanding the foundations for childhood mental health ( IOM and NRC, 2009 ). This relationship among stress, early development, and mental health is relevant to understanding the influences that can threaten the socioemotional well-being of younger children—and to understanding why behavior problems can undermine learning and cognitive growth. One illustration of these effects is the high rates of preschool and prekindergarten children being expelled from their classrooms because of disruptive behavior problems—by one report at a rate more than three times the rate of children in the K-12 grades ( Gilliam, 2005 ; see also U.S. Department of Education Office for Civil Rights, 2014 ). In this study, the likelihood of expulsion decreased significantly when educators were provided access to early childhood mental health consultants who could assist them in managing behavior problems.

Another illustration is reports by kindergarten teachers that social, emotional, and self-regulatory problems are a common impediment to children's readiness to achieve in their classrooms ( Lewit and Baker, 1995 ; Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2000 ). Other studies have shown that children's conduct problems and internalizing (anxious, depressed) behavior in the classroom can undermine the development of constructive educator–child relationships and foreshadow later social and academic difficulties ( Berry and O'Connor, 2010 ; Koles et al., 2009 ; Ladd and Burgess, 2001 ).

Consistent with the research concerning the biological and behavioral effects of chronic stress, there is increasing evidence that even very young children show clear evidence of traumatization and posttraumatic stress, anxious and depressive symptomatology, behavioral and conduct problems, and other serious psychological problems ( Egger and Angold, 2006 ; Lieberman et al., 2011 ; Luby, 2006 ; Zeanah, 2009 ). Sometimes these symptom patterns overlap, such as in the comorbidity in which depressive symptomatology appears along with oppositional behavior in preschoolers ( Egger and Angold, 2006 ). The origins of these problems are multifaceted, but certainly include interaction of environmental stresses with genetic factors that heighten or reduce children's vulnerability to these stresses. Often these environmental stresses undermine the social support that would otherwise buffer the effects of stress on children. Diagnosing these disorders in young children is a challenge because the behaviors associated with early mental health problems in young children can be different from those observed in adults and adolescents ( Egger and Emde, 2011 ). But progress has been made in developing reliable diagnostic criteria for preschoolers (e.g., Egger and Angold, 2006 ; Keenan et al., 1997 ; Lavigne et al., 2009 ) and even infants and toddlers ( Zero to Three, 2005 ). This work provides a foundation for further study of the developmental origins of early mental health challenges and therapeutic interventions that might help these children.

Connecting the Socioemotional Health of Children and Adults

The preceding discussion makes clear that children's socioemotional health is linked to the socioemotional well-being of the adults in their lives. Consistent with the research on the social buffering of stress discussed earlier, when parents and other caregivers are managing well, they can help children cope more competently with the ordinary stresses that inevitably occur. When caregivers are stressed, by contrast, they cannot provide this buffering and are instead more often a source of stress for children. When parents are depressed, for example, they can be unpredictably sad, hostile, critical, and/or disengaged ( NRC and IOM, 2009 ). This constellation of behaviors constitutes a difficult combination of threat and withdrawal of support for children. Young children with a depressed mother are more likely, therefore, to exhibit heightened stress reactivity to moderate challenges; to have an insecure attachment to the parent; to show lower levels of cognitive performance and, later, poorer academic achievement; and to be at greater risk of becoming depressed themselves.

The adult's emotional well-being is important in the classroom as well. Using data from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing study, Jeon and colleagues (2014) measured the depressive symptomatology of 761 home- and center-based care providers, as well as overall observed classroom quality, and obtained independent measures of the behavior problems of the 3-year-olds in their classrooms. They found that educator depression was linked to higher levels of behavior problems in children, attributable to the poorer quality of the classroom environment. Notably, this study was conducted with a sample of families in economic stress, with the educators often sharing the same financial difficulties. Nevertheless, the association of educator depression with child behavior problems remained even when family influences, including maternal depression and family poverty status, were controlled for. Similar associations of educator well-being with the quality of the classroom environment and children's learning have been found in studies of children in the early primary grades (e.g., Pianta, 1999 ; Pianta and Stuhlman, 2004b ).

Conclusions About Chronic Stress and Adversity Chronic stress and adversity constitute fundamental risks to learning and academic success as well as to emotional well-being for many young children. The biological and behavioral effects of stress and adversity can disrupt brain circuitry and stress response systems, affect fundamental cognitive skills, undermine focused thinking and attention, diminish self-regulation, and imperil mental and physical health. Trauma, adversity, and chronic stress can arise from many sources, such as poverty, family conflict, parental depression, abuse, neglect, or exposure to violence in the community. Supportive and stable relationships with adults can help develop children's adaptive capacities and provide them with a significant stress buffer. It is important for adults who work with children to recognize and appreciate the effects of adversity and to have the capacity to employ strategies for preventing or mitigating them, as well as for promoting cognitive, social, and emotional strengths for coping with adverse and stressful experiences. Given the importance of stable and responsive relationships that provide consistent and nurturing interactions, the well-being of the adults who care for young children contributes to their healthy development and early learning. The stresses of economic disadvantage are manifested not only in differences in children's early experiences in the family and the community but also in the quality and stability of the out-of-home care and education families can access and afford and the quality of the schools children later attend. Socioeconomic differences in the quality of early learning opportunities place large numbers of children at a learning disadvantage and undermine their potential for academic success. These differences begin early and have a cumulative effect over time. Strengthening early learning and developing competencies requires serious and sustained attention to these socioeconomic disparities in opportunity.
  • AAP (American Academy of Pediattics) Committee on School Health. Soft drinks in schools. Pediatrics. 2004; 113 (1 Pt. 1):152–154. [ PubMed : 14702469 ]
  • AAP Committee on Sports Medicine and Fitness and AAP Committee on School Health. Physical fitness and activity in schools. Pediatrics. 2000; 105 (5):1156–1157. [ PubMed : 10790480 ]
  • AAP Council on Early Childhood. High PC, Klass P. Literacy promotion: An essential component of primary care pediatric practice. Pediatrics. 2014; 134 (2):404–409. [ PubMed : 24962987 ]
  • AAP Council on School Health. The crucial role of recess in school. Pediatrics. 2013; 131 (1):183–188. [ PubMed : 23277311 ]
  • Abu-Saad K, Fraser D. Maternal nutrition and birth outcomes. Epidemiologic Reviews. 2010; 32 (1):5–25. [ PubMed : 20237078 ]
  • Adams MJ. Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1990.
  • Adler NE, Boyce T, Chesney MA, Cohen S, Folkman S, Kahn RL, Syme SL. Socioeconomic status and health. The challenge of the gradient. American Psychologist. 1994; 49 (1):15–24. [ PubMed : 8122813 ]
  • Ahnert L, Pinquart M, Lamb ME. Security of children's relationships with nonparental care providers: A meta-analysis. Child Development. 2006; 77 (3):664–679. [ PubMed : 16686794 ]
  • Aizer A. Poverty, violence, and health: The impact of domestic violence during pregnancy on newborn health. Journal of Human Resources. 2011; 46 (3):518–538. [ PMC free article : PMC4019993 ] [ PubMed : 24839303 ]
  • Aizer A, Currie J. The intergenerational transmission of inequality: Maternal disadvantage and health at birth. Science. 2014; 344 (6186):856–861. [ PMC free article : PMC4578153 ] [ PubMed : 24855261 ]
  • Alaimo K, Olson CM, Frongillo EA Jr. Food insufficiency and American school-aged children's cognitive, academic, and psychosocial development. Pediatrics. 2001; 108 (1):44–53. [ PubMed : 11433053 ]
  • Alexander KL, Entwisle DR, Dauber SL. First-grade classroom behavior: Its short- and long-term consequences for school performance. Child Development. 1993; 64 (3):801–814. [ PubMed : 8339696 ]
  • Almond D, Currie J. Killing me softly: The fetal origins hypothesis. Journal of Economic Perspectives. 2011; 25 (3):153–172. [ PMC free article : PMC4140221 ] [ PubMed : 25152565 ]
  • Almond D, Mazumder B. Health capital and the prenatal environment: The effect of Ramadan observance during pregnancy. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 2011; 3 (4):56–85.
  • Aloise-Young PA. The development of self-presentation: Self-promotion in 6- to 10-year-old children. Social Cognition. 1993; 11 (2):201–222.
  • Anghel D. Executive function in preschool children: Working memory as a predictor of mathematical ability at school age. Revista Romaneasca pentru Educatie Multidimensionala. 2010; 2 (4):5–16.
  • Ansari D, Garcia N, Lucas E, Hamon K, Dhital B. Neural correlates of symbolic number processing in children and adults. Neuroreport. 2005; 16 :1769–1775. [ PubMed : 16237324 ]
  • Antell SE, Keating DP. Perception of numerical invariance in neonates. Child Development. 1983; 54 :695–701. [ PubMed : 6851716 ]
  • Anthony JL, Lonigan CJ, Driscoll K, Phillips BM, Burgess SR. Phonological sensitivity: A quasi-parallel progression of word structure units and cognitive operations. Reading Research Quarterly. 2003; 38 (4):470–487.
  • Ashcraft MH. Math performance, working memory, and math anxiety; some possible directions for neural functioning work; Paper read at The Neural Basis of Mathematical Development; November; Nashville, TN. 2006.
  • Ashkenazi S, Mark-Zigdon N, Henik A. Do subitizing deficits in developmental dyscalculia involve pattern recognition weakness? Developmental Science. 2013; 16 (1):35–46. [ PubMed : 23278925 ]
  • Au TK, Chan CK, Chan TK, Cheung MW, Ho JY, Ip GW. Folkbiology meets microbiology: A study of conceptual and behavioral change. Cognitive Psychology. 2008; 57 (1):1–19. [ PubMed : 18457822 ]
  • Aukrust VG. Young children acquiring second language vocabulary in preschool group-time: Does amount, diversity, and discourse complexity of teacher talk matter? Journal of Research in Childhood Education. 2007; 22 (1):17–37.
  • Aunola K, Leskinen E, Lerkkanen M-K, Nurmi J-E. Developmental dynamics of math performance from pre-school to grade 2. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2004; 96 :699–713.
  • Baillargeon R, Wu D, Yuan S, Li J, Luo Y. Young infants' expectations about self-propelled objects. In: Hood BM, Santos L, editors. The origins of object knowledge. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2009.
  • Baillargeon R, Scott RM, He Z. False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2010; 14 (3):110–118. [ PMC free article : PMC2930901 ] [ PubMed : 20106714 ]
  • Baker D, Knipe H, Collins J, Leon J, Cummings E, Blair C, Gramson D. One hundred years of elementary school mathematics in the United States: A content analysis and cognitive assessment of textbooks from 1900 to 2000. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 2010; 41 (4):383–423.
  • Baldwin DA. Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development. 1991; 62 (5):875–890. [ PubMed : 1756664 ]
  • Baldwin DA, Moses LJ. Links between social understanding and early word learning: Challenges to current accounts. Social Development. 2001; 10 (3):309–329.
  • Baldwin DA, Tomasello M. Word learning: A window on early pragmatic understanding. Clark EV, editor. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; The Proceedings of the Twenty-ninth Annual Child Language Research Forum. 1998; 29 :3–24.
  • Ball EW, Blachman BA. Does phoneme awareness training in kindergarten make a difference in early word recognition and developmental spelling? Reading Research Quarterly. 1991; 26 (1):49–66.
  • Barata MC. Executive functions in Chilean preschool children: Investigating the associations of early executive functions with emergent mathematics and literacy skills. Harvard Graduate School of Education; 2010. (PhD diss.).
  • Barkley RA. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, self-regulation, and time: Toward a more comprehensive theory. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 1997; 18 (4):271–279. [ PubMed : 9276836 ]
  • Baroody AE, Dobbs-Oates J. Child and parent characteristics, parental expectations, and child behaviours related to preschool children's interest in literacy. Early Child Development and Care. 2009; 181 (3):345–359.
  • Baroody AJ. The developmental bases for early childhood number and operations standards. In: Clements DH, Sarama J, DiBiase A-M, editors. Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2004. pp. 173–219.
  • Baroody AJ, Lai M-L, Mix KS. Changing views of young children's numerical and arithmetic competencies; Paper read at National Association for the Education of Young Children; December; Washington, DC. 2005.
  • Baroody AJ, Lai M-L, Mix KS. The development of young children's number and operation sense and its implications for early childhood education. In: Spodek B, Saracho ON, editors. Handbook of research on the education of young children. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2006. pp. 187–221.
  • Baroody AJ, Li X, Lai M-l. Toddlers' spontaneous attention to number. Mathematical Thinking and Learning. 2008; 10 :240–270.
  • Bartsch K, Wellman HM. Children talk about the mind. New York: Oxford University Press; 1995.
  • Basch CE. Breakfast and the achievement gap among urban minority youth. Journal of School Health. 2011; 81 (10):635–640. [ PubMed : 21923876 ]
  • Bascoe SM, Davies PT, Sturge-Apple ML, Cummings EM. Children's representations of family relationships, peer information processing, and school adjustment. Developmental Psychology. 2009; 45 (6):1740–1751. [ PMC free article : PMC2912155 ] [ PubMed : 19899928 ]
  • Bassett HH, Denham S, Wyatt TM, Warren-Khot HK. Refining the preschool self-regulation assessment for use in preschool classrooms. Infant and Child Development. 2012; 21 (6):596–616.
  • Bassok D, Rorem A. Is kindergarten the new first grade? The changing nature of kindergarten in the age of accountability. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia; 2014.
  • Beilock SL. Learning and performing math: Self-concept, self-doubt, and self-fulfilling prophesy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2001; 130 :224–237. [ PubMed : 11409101 ]
  • Belsky DW, Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Melchior M, Caspi A. Context and sequelae of food insecurity in children's development. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2010; 172 (7):809–818. [ PMC free article : PMC2984258 ] [ PubMed : 20716700 ]
  • Bennett N, Desforges C, Cockburn A, Wilkinson B. The quality of pupil learning experiences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1984.
  • Berch DB, Mazzocco MMM, editors. Why is math so hard for some children? The nature and origins of mathematical learning difficulties and disabilities. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.; 2007.
  • Berry D, O'Connor E. Behavioral risk, teacher–child relationships, and social skill development across middle childhood: A child-by-environment analysis of change. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2010; 31 (1):1–14.
  • Best JR, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences. 2011; 21 (4):327–336. [ PMC free article : PMC3155246 ] [ PubMed : 21845021 ]
  • Bharadwaj P, Johnsen JV, Løken KV. Smoking bans, maternal smoking and birth outcomes. 2012. (IZA Institute for the Study of Labor Discussion Paper No. 7006:72-93).
  • Bharadwaj P, Eberhard J, Neilson C. Health at birth, parental investments and academic outcomes. San Diego: University of California, San Diego; 2013. (unpublished)
  • Bialystok E. Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2011; 65 (4):229–235. [ PMC free article : PMC4341987 ] [ PubMed : 21910523 ]
  • Bialystok E, Craik FIM. Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2010; 19 (1):19–23.
  • Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Green DW, Gollan TH. Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 2009; 10 (3):89–129. [ PubMed : 26168404 ]
  • Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Doyle AE, Seidman LJ, Wilens TE, Ferrero F, Morgan CL, Faraone SV. Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004; 72 (5):757–766. [ PubMed : 15482034 ]
  • Bielaczyc K, Pirolli PL, Brown AL. Training in self-explanation and self-regulation strategies: Investigating the effects of knowledge acquisition activities on problem solving. Cognition and Instruction. 1995; 13 :221–252.
  • Bierman KL, Nix RL, Greenberg MT, Blair C, Domitrovich CE. Executive functions and school readiness intervention: Impact, moderation, and mediation in the Head Start REDI program. Development and Psychopathology. 2008a; 20 (3):821–843. [ PMC free article : PMC3205459 ] [ PubMed : 18606033 ]
  • Bierman KL, Domitrovich CE, Nix RL, Gest SD, Welsh JA, Greenberg MT, Blair C, Nelson KE, Gill S. Promoting academic and social-emotional school readiness: The Head Start REDI program. Child Development. 2008b; 79 (6):1802–1817. [ PMC free article : PMC3549580 ] [ PubMed : 19037951 ]
  • Birch SH, Ladd GW. The teacher–child relationship and children's early school adjustment. Journal of School Psychology. 1997; 35 (1):61–79.
  • Birch SH, Ladd GW. Children's interpersonal behaviors and the teacher–child relationship. Developmental Psychology. 1998; 34 (5):934–946. [ PubMed : 9779740 ]
  • Blair C. School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry. American Psychologist. 2002; 57 (2):111–127. [ PubMed : 11899554 ]
  • Blair C, Diamond A. Biological processes in prevention and intervention: The promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Development and Psychopathology. 2008; 20 (3):899–911. [ PMC free article : PMC2593474 ] [ PubMed : 18606037 ]
  • Blair C, Raver CC. Child development in the context of adversity: Experiential canalization of brain and behavior. American Psychologist. 2012; 67 (4):309–318. [ PMC free article : PMC5264526 ] [ PubMed : 22390355 ]
  • Blair C, Razza RP. Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development. 2007; 78 :647–663. [ PubMed : 17381795 ]
  • Blair C, Protzko J, Ursache A. Self-regulation and early literacy. Neuman SB, Dickinson DK, editors. New York: Guilford Press; Handbook of early literacy research. 2010; 3 :20–35.
  • Blevins-Knabe B, Musun-Miller L. Number use at home by children and their parents and its relationship to early mathematical performance. Early Development and Parenting. 1996; 5 :35–45.
  • Bloom P. Just babies: The origins of good and evil. New York: Crown Publishers; 2013.
  • Bodovski K, Farkas G. Mathematics growth in early elementary school: The roles of beginning knowledge, student engagement, and instruction. Elementary School Journal. 2007; 108 (2):115–130.
  • Bodrova E, Leong DJ. Scaffolding self-regulated learning in young children: Lessons from tools of the mind. In: Pianta RC, Barnett WS, Justice LM, Sheridan SM, editors. Handbook of early childhood education. New York: Guilford Press; 2012. pp. 352–369.
  • Bougma K, Aboud FE, Harding KB, Marquis GS. Iodine and mental development of children 5 years old and under: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutrients. 2013; 5 (4):1384–1416. [ PMC free article : PMC3705354 ] [ PubMed : 23609774 ]
  • Bowers EP, Vasilyeva M. The relation between teacher input and lexical growth of preschoolers. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2011; 32 (1):221–241.
  • Boyce WT, Sokolowski MB, Robinson GE. Toward a new biology of social adversity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012; 109 (Suppl. 2):17143–17148. [ PMC free article : PMC3477390 ] [ PubMed : 23045689 ]
  • Bradley L, Bryant PE. Categorizing sounds and learning to read—a causal connection. Nature. 1983; 301 (5899):419–421.
  • Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002; 53 :371–399. [ PubMed : 11752490 ]
  • Brennan LM, Shelleby EC, Shaw DS, Gardner F, Dishion TJ, Wilson M. Indirect effects of the family check-up on school-age academic achievement through improvements in parenting in early childhood. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2013; 105 (3) [ PMC free article : PMC3850059 ] [ PubMed : 24319295 ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Broidy LM, Nagin DS, Tremblay RE, Brame B, Dodge KA, Fergusson D, Horwood J, Loeber R, Laird R, Lyname D, Moffit TF, Bates JE, Pettit GS, Vitaro F. Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: A six-site, cross-national study. Developmental Psychology. 2003; 30 (2):222–245. [ PMC free article : PMC2753823 ] [ PubMed : 12661883 ]
  • Brosnan M, Demetre J, Hamill S, Robson K, Shepherd H, Cody G. Executive functioning in adults and children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia. 2002; 40 (12):2144–2155. [ PubMed : 12208010 ]
  • Bruner J. The role of dialogue in language acquisition. In: Sinclair A, Jarvella R, Levelt WJM, editors. The child's conception of language. New York: Springer; 1978. pp. 241–256.
  • Bryan J, Osendarp S, Hughes D, Calvaresi E, Baghurst K, van Klinken JW. Nutrients for cognitive development in school-aged children. Nutrition Reviews. 2004; 62 (8):295–306. [ PubMed : 15478684 ]
  • Bühler E, Bachmann C, Goyert H, Heinzel-Gutenbrunner M, Kamp-Becker I. Differential diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder by means of inhibitory control and “theory of mind.” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2011; 41 :1718–1726. [ PubMed : 21373957 ]
  • Buhs ES, Ladd GW. Peer rejection as an antecedent of young children's school adjustment: An examination of mediating processes. Developmental Psychology. 2001; 37 (4):550–560. [ PubMed : 11444490 ]
  • Bull R, Scerif G. Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2001; 19 (3):273–293. [ PubMed : 11758669 ]
  • Bull R, Johnston RS, Roy JA. Exploring the roles of the visual-spatial sketch pad and central executive in children's arithmetical skills: Views from cognition and developmental neuropsychology. Developmental Neuropsychology. 1999; 15 (3):421–442.
  • Bull R, Espy KA, Wiebe SA. Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2008; 33 :205–228. [ PMC free article : PMC2729141 ] [ PubMed : 18473197 ]
  • Busch-Rossnagel NA. Mastery motivation, preschool and early childhood. In: Fisher C, Lerner R, editors. Encyclopedia of applied developmental science. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.; 2005. pp. 679–681.
  • Butler LP, Markman EM. Finding the cause: Verbal framing helps children extract causal evidence embedded in a complex scene. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2012a; 13 (1):38–66.
  • Butler LP, Markman EM. Preschoolers use intentional and pedagogical cues to guide inductive inferences and exploration. Child Development. 2012b; 83 (4):1416–1428. [ PubMed : 22540939 ]
  • Butler LP, Markman EM. Preschoolers use pedagogical cues to guide radical reorganization of category knowledge. Cognition. 2014; 130 (1):116–127. [ PubMed : 24211439 ]
  • Butterworth B. The development of arithmetical abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005; 46 :3–18. [ PubMed : 15660640 ]
  • Butterworth B. Foundational numerical capacities and the origins of dyscalculia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2010; 14 :534–541. [ PubMed : 20971676 ]
  • Butterworth B, Varma S, Laurillard D. Dyscalculia: From brain to education. Science. 2011; 332 :1049–1053. [ PubMed : 21617068 ]
  • Byrne B, Fielding-Barnsley R. Phonemic awareness and letter knowledge in the child's acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1989; 81 (3):313–321.
  • Byrne B, Fielding-Barnsley R. Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children: A 2- and 3-year follow-up and a new preschool trial. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1995; 87 :488–503.
  • Byrnes JP, Wasik BA. Factors predictive of mathematics achievement in kindergarten, first and third grades: An opportunity–propensity analysis. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 2009; 34 :167–183.
  • Cameron CE, Brock LL, Murrah WM, Bell LH, Worzalla SL, Grissmer D, Morrison FJ. Fine motor skills and executive function both contribute to kindergarten achievement. Child Development. 2012; 83 (4):1229–1244. [ PMC free article : PMC3399936 ] [ PubMed : 22537276 ]
  • Campbell F, Conti G, Heckman JJ, Moon SH, Pinto R, Pungello E, Pan Y. Early childhood investments substantially boost adult health. Science. 2014; 343 (6178):1478–1485. [ PMC free article : PMC4028126 ] [ PubMed : 24675955 ]
  • Canadian Paediatric Society. Maternal depression and child development. Paediatrics & Child Health. 2004; 9 (8):575–583. [ PMC free article : PMC2724169 ] [ PubMed : 19680490 ]
  • Cantlon JF, Brannon EM, Carter EJ, Pelphrey KA. Functional imaging of numerical processing in adults and 4-y-old children. PLoS Biology. 2006; 4 (5):e125. [ PMC free article : PMC1431577 ] [ PubMed : 16594732 ]
  • Carey S. The origin of concepts. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press; 2009.
  • Carneiro P, Ginja R. Long term impacts of compensatory preschool on health and behavior: Evidence from Head Start. Bonn, Germany: Institute for the Study of Labor; 2012. (IZA Discussion Paper No. 6315).
  • Cartwright KB. Cognitive development and reading: The relation of reading-specific multiple classification skill to reading comprehension in elementary school children. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2002; 94 (1):56–63.
  • Cartwright KB. Cognitive flexibility and reading comprehension: Relevance to the future. In: Block CC, Parris SR, editors. Comprehension instruction: Research-based best practices. 2nd. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 50–64.
  • Cartwright KB, Marshall TR, Dandy KL, Isaac MC. The development of graphophonological-semantic cognitive flexibility and its contribution to reading comprehension in beginning readers. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2010; 11 (1):61–85.
  • Cassel J. The contribution of the social environment to host resistance: The fourth Wade Hampton Frost lecture. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1976; 104 (2):107–123. [ PubMed : 782233 ]
  • Catts HW, Kamhi AG. Language and reading disabilities. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon; 1999.
  • Catts HW, Fey ME, Zhang X, Tomblin JB. Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading. 1999; 3 (4):331–361.
  • CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention). School health guidelines to promote healthy eating and physical activity. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2011; 60 (RR05):1–71. [ PubMed : 21918496 ]
  • CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention). Youth risk behavior surveillance—United States, 2011. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report: Surveillance Summaries. 2012; 61 (4):1–162. [ PubMed : 22673000 ]
  • CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention). Middle childhood (6-8 years of age): Developmental milestones. 2014. [September 15, 2014]. http://www ​.cdc.gov/ncbddd ​/childdevelopment ​/positiveparenting/middle.html .
  • Celedón-Pattichis S, Musanti SI, Marshall ME. Bilingual elementary teachers' reflections on using students' native language and culture to teach mathematics. In: Foote MQ, editor. Mathematics teaching & learning in K-12: Equity and professional development. New York: Palgrave Macmillan; 2010. pp. 7–24.
  • Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University. The foundations of lifelong health are built in early childhood. 2010. [January 22, 2015]. http://www ​.developingchild.harvard.edu .
  • Champagne DL, Bagot RC, van Hasselt F, Ramakers G, Meaney MJ, de Kloet ER, Joels M, Krugers H. Maternal care and hippocampal plasticity: Evidence for experience-dependent structural plasticity, altered synaptic functioning, and differential responsiveness to glucocorticoids and stress. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008; 28 (23):6037–6045. [ PMC free article : PMC6670331 ] [ PubMed : 18524909 ]
  • Chase-Lansdale L, Brooks-Gunn J. Two-generation programs in the twenty-first century. Future of Children. 2014; 24 (1):13–39. [ PubMed : 25518701 ]
  • Chi MTH, Klahr D. Span and rate of apprehension in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1975; 19 :434–439. [ PubMed : 1236928 ]
  • Child Trends. Homeless children and youth. 2015. [January 27, 2015]. http://www ​.childtrends ​.org/?indicators=homeless-children-and-youth .
  • Children's Bureau. Child maltreatment 2012. 2013. [January 27, 2015]. http://www ​.acf.hhs.gov ​/programs/cb/research-data-technology ​/statistics-research ​/child-maltreatment .
  • Chu FW, Vanmarle K, Geary DC. Quantitative deficits of preschool children at risk for mathematical learning disability. Frontiers in Psychology. 2013; 4 :195. [ PMC free article : PMC3655274 ] [ PubMed : 23720643 ]
  • Cimpian A. The impact of generic language about ability on children's achievement motivation. Developmental Psychology. 2010; 46 (5):1333–1340. [ PubMed : 20822242 ]
  • Cimpian A, Markman EM. Information learned from generic language becomes central to children's biological concepts: Evidence from their open-ended explanations. Cognition. 2009; 113 (1):14–25. [ PubMed : 19674739 ]
  • Cimpian A, Markman EM. The generic/nongeneric distinction influences how children interpret new information about social others. Child Development. 2011; 82 (2):471–492. [ PubMed : 21410911 ]
  • Cimpian A, Arce HC, Markman EM, Dweck CS. Subtle linguistic cues affect children's motivation. Psychological Science. 2007; 18 (4):314–316. [ PubMed : 17470255 ]
  • Claessens A, Engel M, Curran FC. Academic content, student learning, and the persistence of preschool effects. American Educational Research Journal. 2014; 51 (2):403–434.
  • Clarke BA, Clarke DM, Cheeseman J. The mathematical knowledge and understanding young children bring to school. Mathematics Education Research Journal. 2006; 18 (1):81–107.
  • Clements DH. Subitizing: What is it? Why teach it? Teaching Children Mathematics. 1999; 5 :400–405.
  • Clements DH, Sarama J. Learning trajectories in mathematics education. Mathematical Thinking & Learning. 2004; 6 (2):81–89.
  • Clements DH, Sarama J. Experimental evaluation of the effects of a research-based preschool mathematics curriculum. American Educational Research Journal. 2008; 45 :443–494.
  • Clements DH, Sarama J. Learning and teaching early math: The learning trajectories approach. New York: Routledge; 2009.
  • Clements DH, Sarama J. Learning and teaching early and elementary mathematics. In: Carlson JS, Levin JR, editors. Instructional strategies for improving students' learning: Focus on early reading and mathematics. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing; 2012.
  • Clements DH, Sarama J. Learning and teaching early math: The learning trajectories approach. 2nd. New York: Routledge; 2014.
  • Clements DH, Swaminathan S, Hannibal MAZ, Sarama J. Young children's concepts of shape. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 1999; 30 :192–212.
  • Clements DH, Sarama J, Spitler ME, Lange AA, Wolfe CB. Mathematics learned by young children in an intervention based on learning trajectories: A large-scale cluster randomized trial. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 2011; 42 (2):127–166.
  • Clements DH, Baroody AJ, Sarama J. Background research on early mathematics. National Governor's Association (NGA) Center Project on Early Mathematics; 2013a. (unpublished)
  • Clements DH, Sarama J, Wolfe CB, Spitler ME. Longitudinal evaluation of a scale-up model for teaching mathematics with trajectories and technologies: Persistence of effects in the third year. American Educational Research Journal. 2013b; 50 (4):812–850.
  • Cobb S. Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1976; 38 (5):300–314. [ PubMed : 981490 ]
  • Colman S, Nichols-Barrer IP, Redline JE, Devaney BL, Ansell SV, Joyce T. Effects of the Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC): A review of recent research (summary). Alexandria, VA: U.S. Department of Agriculture, Food and Nutrition Service, Office of Research and Analysis; 2012.
  • Connor CM, Morrison FJ, Slominski L. Preschool instruction and children's emergent literacy growth. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2006; 98 (4):665–689.
  • Connor CM, Morrison FJ, Underwood PS. A second chance in second grade: The independent and cumulative impact of first- and second-grade reading instruction and students' letter-word reading skill growth. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2007; 11 (3):199–233.
  • Connor CM, Alberto PA, Compton DL, O'Connor RE. Improving reading outcomes for students with or at risk for reading disabilities: A synthesis of the contributions from the Institute of Education Sciences Research Centers. Washington, DC: National Center for Special Education Research; 2014. (NCSER 2014-3000).
  • Conway ARA, Kane MJ, Engle RW. Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003; 7 (12):547–552. [ PubMed : 14643371 ]
  • Cook JT, Frank DA. Food security, poverty, and human development in the United States. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2008; 1136 (1):193–209. [ PubMed : 17954670 ]
  • Cooper DH, Roth FP, Speece DL, Schatschneider C. The contribution of oral language skills to the development of phonological awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2002; 23 :399–416.
  • Cooper SB, Bandelow S, Nute ML, Morris JG, Nevill ME. Breakfast glycaemic index and cognitive function in adolescent school children. British Journal of Nutrition. 2012; 107 (12):1823–1832. [ PubMed : 22017815 ]
  • Copple C, Bredekamp S, Koralek DG, Charner K. Developmentally appropriate practice. Focus on preschoolers. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children; 2013.
  • Coyne MD, McCoach DB, Loftus S, Zipoli R Jr., Kapp S. Direct vocabulary instruction in kindergarten: Teaching for breadth versus depth. Elementary School Journal. 2009; 110 (1):1–18.
  • Csibra G. Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language. 2010; 25 (2):141–168.
  • Csibra G, Gergely G. Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009; 13 (4):148–153. [ PubMed : 19285912 ]
  • Cunha F, Heckman JJ. Investing in our young people. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2010. (Working paper 1620).
  • Cunningham AE, Zibulsky J. Book smart: How to develop and support successful, motivated readers. New York: Oxford University Press; 2014.
  • Currie J. Inequality at birth: Some causes and consequences. American Economic Review. 2011; 101 (3):1–22.
  • Currie J, Rossin-Slater M. Weathering the storm: Hurricanes and birth outcomes. Journal of Health Economics. 2013; 32 (3):487–503. [ PMC free article : PMC3649867 ] [ PubMed : 23500506 ]
  • Currie J, Walker WR. Traffic congestion and infant health: Evidence from E-ZPass. 2009. (National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper Series No. 15413).
  • Currie J, Neidell M, Schmieder JF. Air pollution and infant health: Lessons from New Jersey. Journal of Health Economics. 2009; 28 (3):688–703. [ PMC free article : PMC2727943 ] [ PubMed : 19328569 ]
  • Currie J, Graff Zivin J, Meckel K, Neidell M, Schlenker W. Something in the water: Contaminated drinking water and infant health. Canadian Journal of Economics. 2013; 46 (3):791–810. [ PMC free article : PMC4849482 ] [ PubMed : 27134285 ]
  • Danese A, McEwen BS. Adverse childhood experiences, allostasis, allostatic load, and age-related disease. Physiology & Behavior. 2012; 106 (1):29–39. [ PubMed : 21888923 ]
  • Davies PT, Woitach MJ, Winter MA, Cummings EM. Children's insecure representations of the interparental relationship and their school adjustment: The mediating role of attention difficulties. Child Development. 2008; 79 (5):1570–1582. [ PubMed : 18826543 ]
  • de Bruin A, Treccani B, Della Sala S. Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychological Science. 2015; 26 (1):99–107. [ PubMed : 25475825 ]
  • de Ruiter C, van IJzendoorn MH. International Journal of Educational Research. 19. 1993. Attachment and cognition: A review of the literature; pp. 525–540.
  • Dearing E, McCartney K, Taylor BA. Change in family income-to-needs matters more for children with less. Child Development. 2001; 72 (6):1779–1793. [ PubMed : 11768145 ]
  • Denham SA, Brown C. “Plays nice with others”: Social–emotional learning and academic success. Early Education and Development. 2010; 21 (5):652–680.
  • Denton K, West J. Children's reading and mathematics achievement in kindergarten and first grade. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics; 2002.
  • Derryberry D, Reed M. Regulatory processes and the development of cognitive representations. Development and Psychopathology. 1996; 8 (1):215–234.
  • Deschenes O, Greenstone M, Guryan J. Climate change and birth weight. American Economic Review. 2009; 99 (2):211–217. [ PubMed : 29505213 ]
  • DeYoung CG. Intelligence and personality. In: Sternberg RJ, Kaufman SB, editors. The Cambridge handbook of intelligence. Cambridge, MA, and New York: Cambridge University Press; 2011. pp. 711–737.
  • Diamond A. Neuropsychological insights into the meaning of object concept development. In: Carey SE, Gelman R, editors. The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1991. pp. 67–110.
  • Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011; 333 (6045):959–964. [ PMC free article : PMC3159917 ] [ PubMed : 21852486 ]
  • Diamond KE, Justice LM, Siegler RS, Snyder PA. Synthesis of IES research on early intervention and early childhood education. Washington, DC: National Center for Special Education Research, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education; 2013.
  • Dickinson DK. Why we must improve teacher–child conversations in preschools and the promise of professional development. In: Girolametto L, Weitzman E, editors. Enhancing caregiver language facilitation in childcare settings. Toronto: The Hanen Institute; 2003. pp. 41–48.
  • Dickinson DK, Freiberg J. Environmental factors affecting language acquisition from birth–five: Implications for literacy development and intervention efforts; Paper presented at Workshop on the Role of Language in School Learning: Implications for Closing the Achievement Gap; Menlo Park, CA. 2009.
  • Dickinson DK, Porche MV. Relation between language experiences in preschool classrooms and children's kindergarten and fourth-grade language and reading abilities. Child Development. 2011; 82 (3):870–886. [ PubMed : 21413936 ]
  • Dickinson DK, Smith MW. Preschool talk: Patterns of teacher–child interaction in early childhood classrooms. Journal of Research in Childhood Education. 1991; 6 (1):20–29.
  • Dickinson DK, Tabors PO. Beginning literacy with language: Young children learning at home and school. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.; 2001.
  • Dickinson DK, Darrow CL, Tinubu TA. Patterns of teacher–child conversations in Head Start classrooms: Implications for an empirically grounded approach to professional development. Early Education and Development. 2008; 19 (3):396–429.
  • Doig B, McCrae B, Rowe K. A good start to numeracy: Effective numeracy strategies from research and practice in early childhood. Canberra ACT, Australia: Australian Council for Educational Research; 2003.
  • Dombek JL, Connor CM. Preventing retention: First grade classroom instruction and student characteristics. Psychology in the Schools. 2012; 49 (6):568–588.
  • Domitrovich CE, Moore JE, Thompson RA. the CASEL Preschool to Elementary School Social and Emotional Learning Assessment Workgroup. Interventions that promote social-emotional learning in young children. In: Pianta RC, Barnett WS, Justice LM, Sheridan SM, editors. Handbook of early childhood education. New York: Guilford Press; 2012. pp. 393–415.
  • Dozier M, Peloso E, Lindhiem O, Gordon MK, Manni M, Sepulveda S, Ackerman J, Bernier A, Levine S. Developing evidence-based interventions for foster children: An example of a randomized clinical trial with infants and toddlers. Journal of Social Issues. 2006; 62 (4):767–785.
  • Dozier M, Peloso E, Lewis E, Laurenceau JP, Levine S. Effects of an attachment-based intervention on the cortisol production of infants and toddlers in foster care. Development and Psychopathology. 2008; 20 (3):845–859. [ PMC free article : PMC3258505 ] [ PubMed : 18606034 ]
  • Drake K, Belsky J, Fearon RM. From early attachment to engagement with learning in school: The role of self-regulation and persistence. Developmental Psychology. 2014; 50 (5):1350–1361. [ PubMed : 23647414 ]
  • Duke NK, Block MK. Improving reading in the primary grades. The Future of Children. 2012; 22 (2):55–72. [ PubMed : 23057131 ]
  • Duncan GJ, Magnuson K. The nature and impact of early achievement skills, attention skills, and behavior problems. In: Duncan GJ, Murnane R, editors. Whither opportunity? Rising inequality and the uncertain life chances of low-income children. New York: Russell Sage Press; 2011. pp. 47–70.
  • Duncan GJ, Claessens A, Engel M. The contributions of hard skills and socioemotional behavior to school readiness. 2005. [January 20, 2015]. http://www ​.ipr.northwestern ​.edu/publications ​/docs/workingpapers ​/2005/IPR-WP-05-01.pdf .
  • Duncan GJ, Dowsett CJ, Claessens A, Magnuson K, Huston AC, Klebanov P, Pagani L, Feinstein L, Engel M, Brooks-Gunn J, Sexton H, Duckworth K, Japel C. School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology. 2007; 43 (6):1428–1446. [ PubMed : 18020822 ]
  • Dunlap G, Strain PS, Fox L, Carta JJ, Conroy M, Smith BJ, Kern L, Hemmeter ML, Timm MA, McCart A, Sailor W, Markey U, Markey DJ, Lardieri S, Sowell C. Prevention and intervention with young children's challenging behavior: Perspectives regarding current knowledge. Behavioral Disorders. 2006; 32 (1):29–45.
  • Dunn J. Mindreading, emotion understanding, and relationships. In: Hartup WW, Silbereisen RK, editors. Growing points in developmental science: An introduction. Hove, NY: Psychology Press; 2002. pp. 167–176.
  • Edens KM, Potter EF. An exploratory look at the relationships among math skills, motivational factors and activity choice. Early Childhood Education Journal. 2013; 41 (3):235–243.
  • Egger HL, Angold A. Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: Presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2006; 47 (3-4):313–337. [ PubMed : 16492262 ]
  • Egger HL, Emde RN. Developmentally sensitive diagnostic criteria for mental health disorders in early childhood: The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV, the research diagnostic criteria-preschool age, and the diagnostic classification of mental health and developmental disorders of infancy and early childhood-revised. American Psychologist. 2011; 66 (2):95–106. [ PMC free article : PMC3064438 ] [ PubMed : 21142337 ]
  • Ehri LC. Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2005; 9 (2):167–188.
  • Eimeren LV, MacMillan KD, Ansari D. The role of subitizing in children's development of verbal counting; Paper read at Society for Research in Child Development; April; Boston, MA. 2007.
  • Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Eggum ND. Self-regulation and school readiness. Early Education and Development. 2010; 21 (5):681–698. [ PMC free article : PMC3018834 ] [ PubMed : 21234283 ]
  • Ellis R. The study of second language acquisition. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press; 2008.
  • Englund MM, Luckner AE, Whaley GJL, Egeland B. Children's achievement in early elementary school: Longitudinal effects of parental involvement, expectations, and quality of assistance. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2004; 96 (4):723–730.
  • Ericsson KA, Krampe RT, Tesch-Römer C. The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review. 1993; 100 :363–406.
  • ERS (Economic Research Service). Definitions of food security. 2014. [March 23, 2015]. http://www ​.ers.usda.gov ​/topics/food-nutrition-assistance ​/food-security-in-the-us ​/definitions-of-food-security.aspx .
  • Espada JP. The native language in teaching kindergarten mathematics. Journal of International Education Research. 2012; 8 (4):359–366.
  • Espinosa LM. Curriculum and assessment considerations for young children from culturally, linguistically, and economically diverse backgrounds. Psychology in the Schools. 2005; 42 (8):837–853.
  • Evans GW, Kim P. Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self-regulation, and coping. Child Development Perspectives. 2013; 7 (1):43–48.
  • Evans GW, Chen E, Miller G, Seeman T. How poverty gets under the skin: A life-course perspective. In: Maholmes V, King RB, editors. The Oxford handbook of poverty and child development. New York: Oxford University Press; 2012. pp. 13–36.
  • Fahie CM, Symons DK. Executive functioning and theory of mind in children clinically referred for attention and behavior problems. Applied Developmental Psychology. 2003; 24 :51–73.
  • Farah MJ, Betancourt L, Shera DM, Savage JH, Giannetta JM, Brodsky NL, Malmud EK, Hurt H. Environmental stimulation, parental nurturance and cognitive development in humans. Developmental Science. 2008; 11 (5):793–801. [ PubMed : 18810850 ]
  • Farran DC, Aydogan C, Kang SJ, Lipsey M. Preschool classroom environments and the quantity and quality of children's literacy and language behaviors. In: Dickinson D, Neuman S, editors. Handbook of early literacy research. New York: Guilford Press; 2005. pp. 257–268.
  • Farran DC, Lipsey MW, Watson B, Hurley S. Balance of content emphasis and child content engagement in an early reading first program; Paper presented at American Educational Research Association; April; Chicago, IL. 2007.
  • Feigenson L, Libertus ME, Halberda J. Links between the intuitive sense of number and formal mathematics ability. Child Development Perspectives. 2013; 7 (2):74–79. [ PMC free article : PMC3891767 ] [ PubMed : 24443651 ]
  • Fernald A, Marchman VA, Weisleder A. SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science. 2013; 16 (2):234–248. [ PMC free article : PMC3582035 ] [ PubMed : 23432833 ]
  • Field E, Robles O, Torero M. Iodine deficiency and schooling attainment in Tanzania. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 2009; 1 (4):140–169.
  • Figlio D, Guryan J, Karbownik K, Roth J. The effects of poor neonatal health on children's cognitive development? American Economic Review. 2014; 104 (12):4205–4230. [ PubMed : 29533575 ]
  • Fisher PA, Stoolmiller M, Gunnar MR, Burraston BO. Effects of a therapeutic intervention for foster preschoolers on diurnal cortisol activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007; 32 (8-10):892–905. [ PMC free article : PMC2174427 ] [ PubMed : 17656028 ]
  • Fisher PA, Van Ryzin MJ, Gunnar MR. Mitigating HPA axis dysregulation associated with placement changes in foster care. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011; 36 (4):531–539. [ PMC free article : PMC3610565 ] [ PubMed : 20888698 ]
  • Fivush R, Haden CA, Reese E. Elaborating on elaborations: Role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Development. 2006; 77 (6):1568–1588. [ PubMed : 17107447 ]
  • Florence MD, Asbridge M, Veugelers PJ. Diet quality and academic performance. (quiz 239-241). Journal of School Health. 2008; 78 (4):209–215. [ PubMed : 18336680 ]
  • Furrow D, Nelson K, Benedict H. Mothers' speech to children and syntactic development: Some simple relationships. Journal of Child Language. 1979; 6 (03):423–442. [ PubMed : 536408 ]
  • Fuson KC. Children's counting and concepts of number. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1988.
  • Fuson KC. Research on learning and teaching addition and subtraction of whole numbers. In: Leinhardt G, Putman R, Hattrup RA, editors. Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1992. pp. 53–187.
  • Fuson KC. Pre-K to grade 2 goals and standards: Achieving 21st century mastery for all. In: Clements DH, Sarama J, DiBiase A-M, editors. Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2004. pp. 105–148.
  • Fuson KC, Kwon Y. Korean childen's understanding of multidigit addition and subtraction. Child Development. 1992; 63 :491–506. [ PubMed : 1611949 ]
  • Gámez PB, Lesaux NK. The relation between exposure to sophisticated and complex language and early-adolescent English-only and language minority learners' vocabulary. Child Development. 2012; 83 (4):1316–1331. [ PubMed : 22591162 ]
  • Gámez PB, Levine SC. Oral language skills of spanish-speaking English language learners: The impact of high-quality native language exposure. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2013; 34 (4):673–696.
  • GAO (U.S. Government Accountability Office). K-12 education school-based physical education and sports programs: Report to congressional requesters. Washington, DC: GAO; 2012.
  • Geary DC. A componential analysis of an early learning deficit in mathematics. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1990; 49 :363–383. [ PubMed : 2348157 ]
  • Geary DC. Mathematical disabilities: Cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. Psychological Bulletin. 1993; 114 :345–362. [ PubMed : 8416036 ]
  • Geary DC. Children's mathematical development: Research and practical applications. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1994.
  • Geary DC. Mathematics and learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2004; 37 :4–15. [ PubMed : 15493463 ]
  • Geary DC. Cognitive addition: A short longitudinal study of strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in normal and mathematically disabled children. Developmental Psychology. 2011; 47 :1539–1552.
  • Geary DC, Bow-Thomas CC, Yao Y. Counting knowledge and skill in cognitive addition: A comparison of normal and mathematically disabled children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1992; 54 :372–391. [ PubMed : 1453139 ]
  • Geary DC, Hoard MK, Byrd-Craven J, Nugent L, Numtee C. Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning disability. Child Development. 2007; 78 :1343–1359. [ PMC free article : PMC4439199 ] [ PubMed : 17650142 ]
  • Geary DC, Hoard MK, Nugent L. Independent contributions of the central executive, intelligence, and in-class attentive behavior to developmental change in the strategies used to solve addition problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2012; 113 (1):49–65. [ PMC free article : PMC3392437 ] [ PubMed : 22698947 ]
  • Gelman SA. The essential child origins of essentialism in everyday thought. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003.
  • Gelman SA, Markman EM. Young children's inductions from natural kinds: The role of categories and appearances. Child Development. 1987; 58 (6):1532–1541. [ PubMed : 3691200 ]
  • Gergely G, Bekkering H, Kiraly I. Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature. 2002; 415 (6873):755. [ PubMed : 11845198 ]
  • Gerson SA, Woodward AL. Learning from their own actions: The unique effect of producing actions on infants' action understanding. Child Development. 2014; 85 (1):264–277. [ PMC free article : PMC3740060 ] [ PubMed : 23647241 ]
  • Gilliam WS. Prekindergarteners left behind: Expulsion rates in state prekindergarten systems. New York: Foundation for Child Development; 2005.
  • Girolametto L, Weitzman E. Responsiveness of child care providers in interactions with toddlers and preschoolers. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 2002; 33 (4):268–281. [ PubMed : 27764500 ]
  • Glaser R. The maturing of the relationship between the science of learning of learning and cognition and educational practice. Learning and Instruction. 1991; 1 :129–144.
  • Glasersfeld EV. Sensory experience, abstraction, and teaching. In: Steffe LP, Gale J, editors. Constructivism in education. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1995. pp. 369–383.
  • Gopnik A, Wellman HM. Reconstructing constructivism: Causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms, and the theory theory. Psychological Bulletin. 2012; 138 (6):1085–1108. [ PMC free article : PMC3422420 ] [ PubMed : 22582739 ]
  • Gopnik A, Sobel DM, Schulz LE, Glymour C. Causal learning mechanisms in very young children: Two-, three-, and four-year-olds infer causal relations from patterns of variation and covariation. Developmental Psychology. 2001; 37 (5):620–629. [ PubMed : 11552758 ]
  • Gough PB, Tunmer WE. Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education (RASE). 1986; 7 (1):6–10.
  • Gough PB, Hoover WA, Peterson CL. Some observations on a simple view of reading. In: Cornoldi C, Oakhill J, editors. Reading comprehension difficulties. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1996. pp. 1–13.
  • Graham SA, Kilbreath CS, Welder AN. Thirteen-month-olds rely on shared labels and shape similarity for inductive inferences. Child Development. 2004; 75 (2):409–427. [ PubMed : 15056196 ]
  • Graziano PA, Reavis RD, Keane SP, Calkins SD. The role of emotion regulation and children's early academic success. Journal of School Psychology. 2007; 45 (1):3–19. [ PMC free article : PMC3004175 ] [ PubMed : 21179384 ]
  • Greenwood C, Buzhardt J, Walker D, Howard W, Anderson R. Program-level influences on the measurement of early communication for infants and toddlers in early Head Start. Journal of Early Intervention. 2011; 33 (2):110–134.
  • Gripshover SJ, Markman EM. Teaching young children a theory of nutrition: Conceptual change and the potential for increased vegetable consumption. Psychological Science. 2013; 24 (8):1541–1553. [ PubMed : 23804961 ]
  • Grissmer D, Grimm KJ, Aiyer SM, Murrah WM, Steele JS. Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: Two new school readiness indicators. Developmental Psychology. 2010a; 46 (5):1008–1017. [ PubMed : 20822219 ]
  • Grissmer D, Grimm KJ, Aiyer SM, Murrah WM, Steele JS. New school readiness indicators. Charlottesville: University of Virginia, Center for Advanced Study of Teaching and Learning; 2010b. (Research brief).
  • Gunderson EA, Levine SC. Some types of parent number talk count more than others: Relations between parents' input and children's cardinal-number knowledge. Developmental Science. 2011; 14 (5):1021–1032. [ PMC free article : PMC3177161 ] [ PubMed : 21884318 ]
  • Gunderson EA, Gripshover SJ, Romero C, Dweck CS, Goldin-Meadow S, Levine SC. Parent praise to 1- to 3-year-olds predicts children's motivational frameworks 5 years later. Child Development. 2013; 84 (5):1526–1541. [ PMC free article : PMC3655123 ] [ PubMed : 23397904 ]
  • Gunnar MR, Donzella B. Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early human development. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002; 27 (1-2):199–220. [ PubMed : 11750779 ]
  • Gunnar MR, Morison SJ, Chisholm K, Schuder M. Salivary cortisol levels in children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Development and Psychopathology. 2001; 13 (3):611–628. [ PubMed : 11523851 ]
  • Gweon H, Schulz L. 16-month-olds rationally infer causes of failed actions. Science. 2011; 332 (6037):1524. [ PubMed : 21700866 ]
  • Hackman DA, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009; 13 (2):65–73. [ PMC free article : PMC3575682 ] [ PubMed : 19135405 ]
  • Hackman DA, Farah MJ, Meaney MJ. Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience. 2010; 11 (9):651–659. [ PMC free article : PMC2950073 ] [ PubMed : 20725096 ]
  • Hair E, Halle T, Terry-Humen E, Lavelle B, Calkins J. Children's school readiness in the ECLS-K: Predictions to academic, health, and social outcomes in first grade. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2006; 21 (4):431–454.
  • Hamayan EV, Marler B, Sánchez López C, Damico J. Special education considerations for English language learners: Delivering a continuum of services. 2nd. Philadelphia, PA: Caslon Publishing; 2013.
  • Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P. Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature. 2007; 450 (7169):557–559. [ PubMed : 18033298 ]
  • Hamre BK. Teachers' daily interactions with children: An essential ingredient in effective early childhood programs. Child Development Perspectives. 2014; 8 (4):223–230.
  • Hamre BK, Pianta RC. Early teacher–child relationships and the trajectory of children's school outcomes through eighth grade. Child Development. 2001; 72 :625–638. [ PubMed : 11333089 ]
  • Hamre BK, Pianta RC. Self-reported depression in nonfamilial caregivers: Prevalence and associations with caregiver behavior in child-care settings. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2004; 19 (2):297–318.
  • Hamre BK, Pianta RC. Can instructional and emotional support in the first-grade classroom make a difference for children at risk of school failure? Child Development. 2005; 76 (5):949–967. [ PubMed : 16149994 ]
  • Hannula MM. Spontaneous focusing on numerosity in the development of early mathematical skills. Turku, Finland: University of Turku; 2005.
  • Hannula MM, Räsänen P, Lehtinen E. Development of counting skills: Role of spontaneous focusing on numerosity and subitizing-based enumeration. Mathematical Thinking and Learning. 2007; 9 :51–57.
  • Harris KR, Friedlander BD, Saddler B, Frizzelle R, Graham S. Self-monitoring of attention versus self-monitoring of academic performance: Effects among students with ADHD in the general education classroom. Journal of Special Education. 2005; 39 (3):145–156.
  • Harris PL. Trusting what you're told: How children learn from others. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press; 2012.
  • Hart B, Risley TR. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.; 1995.
  • Hawthorne K, Gerken L. From pauses to clauses: Prosody facilitates learning of syntactic constituency. Cognition. 2014; 133 (2):420–428. [ PMC free article : PMC4163511 ] [ PubMed : 25151251 ]
  • Hecht SA, Torgesen JK, Wagner RK, Raschotte CA. The relations between phonological processing abilities and emerging individual differences in mathematical computation skills: A longitudinal study from second to fifth grades. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2001; 79 :192–227. [ PubMed : 11343408 ]
  • Heckman JJ. The economics, technology, and neuroscience of human capability formation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007; 104 (33):13250–13255. [ PMC free article : PMC1948899 ] [ PubMed : 17686985 ]
  • Heckman JJ, Pinto R, Savelyev P. Understanding the mechanisms through which an influential early childhood program boosted adult outcomes. American Economic Review. 2013; 103 (6):2052–2086. [ PMC free article : PMC3951747 ] [ PubMed : 24634518 ]
  • Henry GT, Rickman DK. Do peers influence children's skill development in preschool? Economics of Education Review. 2007; 26 (1):100–112.
  • Hertzman C, Boyce WT. How experience gets under the skin to create gradients in developmental health. Annual Review of Public Health. 2010; 31 :329–347. [ PubMed : 20070189 ]
  • Heyman GD, Dweck CS. Achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: Their relation and their role in adaptive motivation. Motivation and Emotion. 1992; 16 (3):231–247.
  • Heyman GD, Gelman SA. The use of trait labels in making psychological inferences. Child Development. 1999; 70 (3):604–619. [ PubMed : 10368912 ]
  • Hibel LC, Granger DA, Blair C, Cox MJ. Maternal sensitivity buffers the adrenocortical implications of intimate partner violence exposure during early childhood. Development and Psychopathology. 2011; 23 (2):689–701. [ PubMed : 23786704 ]
  • Hill EL. Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004; 8 (1):26–32. [ PubMed : 14697400 ]
  • Hindman AH, Connor CM, Jewkes AM, Morrison FJ. Untangling the effects of shared book reading: Multiple factors and their associations with preschool literacy outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2008; 23 (3):330–350.
  • Hinnant JB, O'Brien M, Ghazarian SR. The longitudinal relations of teacher expectations to achievement in the early school years. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2009; 101 (3):662–670. [ PMC free article : PMC2860190 ] [ PubMed : 20428465 ]
  • Hoff E. How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review. 2006; 26 (1):55–88.
  • Hoff E, Naigles L. How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development. 2002; 73 (2):418–433. [ PubMed : 11949900 ]
  • Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, Baddeley AD. Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012; 16 (3):174–180. [ PubMed : 22336729 ]
  • Hongwanishkul D, Happaney KR, Lee WSC, Zelazo PD. Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005; 28 (2):617–644. [ PubMed : 16144430 ]
  • Hoover WA, Gough PB. The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. 1990; 2 (2):127–160.
  • Hostinar CE, Sullivan RM, Gunnar MR. Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis: A review of animal models and human studies across development. Psychological Bulletin. 2014; 140 (1):256–282. [ PMC free article : PMC3844011 ] [ PubMed : 23607429 ]
  • Howes C, Hamilton CE. Children's relationships with child care teachers: Stability and concordance with parental attachments. Child Development. 1992; 63 (4):867–878. [ PubMed : 1505245 ]
  • Howes C, Hamilton CE, Phillipsen LC. Stability and continuity of child-caregiver and child-peer relationships. Child Development. 1998; 69 (2):418–426. [ PubMed : 9586216 ]
  • Howse RB, Lange G, Farran DC, Boyles CD. Motivation and self-regulation as predictors of achievement in economically disadvantaged young children. Journal of Experimental Education. 2003a; 71 (2):151–174.
  • Howse RB, Calkins SD, Anastopoulos AD, Keane SP, Shelton TL. Regulatory contributors to children's kindergarten achievement. Early Education and Development. 2003b; 14 (1):101–120.
  • Hoyland A, Dye L, Lawton CL. A systematic review of the effect of breakfast on the cognitive performance of children and adolescents. Nutrition Research Reviews. 2009; 22 (2):220–243. [ PubMed : 19930787 ]
  • Hoynes H, Page M, Stevens AH. Can targeted transfers improve birth outcomes?: Evidence from the introduction of the WIC program. Journal of Public Economics. 2011; 95 (7-8):813–827.
  • Huang-Pollock CL, Karalunas SL, Tam H, Moore AN. Evaluating vigilance deficits in ADHD: A meta-analysis of CPT performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2012; 121 (2):360–371. [ PMC free article : PMC3664643 ] [ PubMed : 22428793 ]
  • Huffman LC, Mehlinger SL, Kerivan AS. Risk factors for academic and behavioral problems in the beginning of school. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina, Frank Porter Graham Child Development Center; 2000.
  • Hughes C, Ensor R. Individual differences in growth in executive function across the transition to school predict externalizing and internalizing behaviors and self-perceived academic success at 6 years of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011; 108 :663–676. [ PubMed : 20673580 ]
  • Huntley-Fenner G, Carey S, Solimando A. Objects are individuals but stuff doesn't count: Perceived rigidity and cohesiveness influence infants' representations of small groups of discrete entities. Cognition. 2002; 85 :203–221. [ PubMed : 12169409 ]
  • Huttenlocher J. Language input and language growth. Preventive Medicine. 1998; 27 (2):195–199. [ PubMed : 9578994 ]
  • Huttenlocher J, Jordan NC, Levine SC. A mental model for early arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1994; 123 :284–296. [ PubMed : 7931093 ]
  • Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M, Cymerman E, Levine S. Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology. 2002; 45 (3):337–374. [ PubMed : 12480478 ]
  • Huttenlocher J, Waterfall H, Vasilyeva M, Vevea J, Hedges LV. Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology. 2010; 61 (4):343–365. [ PMC free article : PMC2981670 ] [ PubMed : 20832781 ]
  • Hyde DC, Spelke ES. Neural signatures of number processing in human infants: Evidence for two core systems underlying numerical cognition. Developmental Science. 2011; 14 (2):360–371. [ PMC free article : PMC3050652 ] [ PubMed : 21399717 ]
  • Inagaki K, Hatano G. Vitalistic causality in young children's naive biology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004; 8 (8):356–362. [ PubMed : 15335462 ]
  • IOM (Institute of Medicine). Hunger and obesity: Understanding a food insecurity paradigm: Workshop summary. Troy LM, Miller EA, Olson S, editors. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2011. [ PubMed : 24983070 ]
  • IOM (Institute of Medicine). Educating the student body: Taking physical activity and physical education to school. Kohl HW III, Cook HD, editors. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2013. [ PubMed : 24851299 ]
  • IOM and NRC (National Research Council). Preventing mental, emotional, and behavioral disorders among young people: Progress and possibilities. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2009. [ PubMed : 20662125 ]
  • Isen A, Rossin-Slater M, Walker WR. Every breath you take—every dollar you'll make: The long-term consequences of the Clean Air Act of 1970. 2014. (National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper Series No. 19858).
  • Jacoby JW, Lesaux NK. Support for extended discourse in teacher talk with linguistically diverse preschoolers. Early Education and Development. 2014; 25 (8):1162–1179.
  • Jamison KR, Cabell SQ, LoCasale-Crouch J, Hamre BK, Pianta RC. Class–infant: An observational measure for assessing teacher–infant interactions in center-based child care. Early Education & Development. 2014; 25 (4):553–572.
  • Janzen J. Teaching English language learners. Review of Educational Research. 2008; 78 :1010–1038.
  • Jaswal VK. Believing what you're told: Young children's trust in unexpected testimony about the physical world. Cognitive Psychology. 2010; 61 (3):248–272. [ PMC free article : PMC2930108 ] [ PubMed : 20650449 ]
  • Jenks KM, van Lieshout EC, de Moor JM. Cognitive correlates of mathematical achievement in children with cerebral palsy and typically developing children. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 2012; 82 (1):120–135. [ PubMed : 22429061 ]
  • Jeon L, Buettner CK, Snyder AR. Pathways from teacher depression and childcare quality to child behavioral problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2014; 82 (2):225–235. [ PubMed : 24447005 ]
  • Jiang Y, Ekono M, Skinner C. Basic facts about low-income children: Children under 6 years, 2012. 2014. [January 26, 2015]. http://nccp ​.org/publications/pub_1088 ​.html .
  • Johnson-Pynn JS, Ready C, Beran M. Estimation mediates preschoolers: Numerical reasoning: Evidence against precise calculation abilities; Paper read at Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development; April; Atlanta, GA. 2005.
  • Johnston T, Kirby J. The contribution of naming speed to the simple view of reading. Reading and Writing. 2006; 19 (4):339–361.
  • Jordan NC, Hanich LB, Uberti HZ. Mathematical thinking and learning difficulties. In: Baroody AJ, Dowker A, editors. The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2003. pp. 359–383.
  • Joshi RM, Aaron PG. The component model of reading: Simple view of reading made a little more complex. Reading Psychology. 2000; 21 (2):85–97.
  • Justice LM, Meier J, Walpole S. Learning new words from storybooks: An efficacy study with at-risk kindergartners. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 2005; 36 (1):17–32. [ PubMed : 15801505 ]
  • Justice LM, McGinty AS, Zucker T, Cabell SQ, Piasta SB. Bi-directional dynamics underlie the complexity of talk in teacher–child play-based conversations in classrooms serving at-risk pupils. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2013; 28 (3):496–508.
  • Kamins ML, Dweck CS. Person versus process praise and criticism: Implications for contingent self-worth and coping. Developmental Psychology. 1999; 35 (3):835–847. [ PubMed : 10380873 ]
  • Kaplan DW, Brindis CD, Phibbs SL, Melinkovich P, Naylor K, Ahlstrand K. A comparison study of an elementary school-based health center: Effects on health care access and use. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 1999; 153 (3):235–243. [ PubMed : 10086399 ]
  • Karmiloff K, Karmiloff-Smith A. Pathways to language: From fetus to adolescent. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2001.
  • Kaufman EL, Lord MW, Reese TW, Volkmann J. The discrimination of visual number. American Journal of Psychology. 1949; 62 :498–525. [ PubMed : 15392567 ]
  • Keenan K, Shaw DS, Walsh B, Delliquadri E, Giovannelli J. DSM-III-R disorders in preschool children from low-income families. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997; 36 (5):620–627. [ PubMed : 9136496 ]
  • Kelley SA, Brownell CA, Campbell SB. Mastery motivation and self-evaluative affect in toddlers: Longitudinal relations with maternal behavior. Child Development. 2000; 71 (4):1061–1071. [ PubMed : 11016566 ]
  • Kerr A, Zelazo PD. Development of “hot” executive function: The children's gambling task. Brain and Cognition. 2004; 55 (1):148–157. [ PubMed : 15134849 ]
  • Keyl PM, Hurtado MP, Barber MM, Borton J. School-based health centers. Students' access, knowledge, and use of services. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 1996; 150 (2):175–180. [ PubMed : 8556122 ]
  • Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Gouin JP, Hantsoo L. Close relationships, inflammation, and health. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010; 35 (1):33–38. [ PMC free article : PMC2891342 ] [ PubMed : 19751761 ]
  • King JC Jr., Stoddard JJ, Gaglani MJ, Moore KA, Magder L, McClure E, Rubin JD, Englund JA, Neuzil K. Effectiveness of school-based influenza vaccination. New England Journal of Medicine. 2006; 355 (24):2523–2532. [ PubMed : 17167135 ]
  • Kipping P, Gard A, Gilman L, Gorman J. Speech and language development chart. 3rd. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.; 2012.
  • Kirkham NZ, Slemmer JA, Johnson SP. Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence for a domain general learning mechanism. Cognition. 2002; 83 (2):B35–B42. [ PubMed : 11869728 ]
  • Kishiyama MM, Boyce WT, Jimenez AM, Perry LM, Knight RT. Socioeconomic disparities affect prefrontal function in children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009; 21 (6):1106–1115. [ PubMed : 18752394 ]
  • Klibanoff RS, Levine SC, Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M, Hedges LV. Preschool children's mathematical knowledge: The effect of teacher “math talk.” Developmental Psychology. 2006; 42 :59–69. [ PubMed : 16420118 ]
  • Koenig MA, Doebel S. Children's understanding of unreliability: Evidence for a negativity bias. In: Banaji MR, Gelman SA, editors. Navigating the social world. New York: Oxford University Press; 2013. pp. 235–240.
  • Koles B, O'Connor E, McCartney K. Teacher–child relationships in prekindergarten: The influences of child and teacher characteristics. Journal of Early Childhood Teacher Education. 2009; 30 (1):3–21.
  • Koponen T, Salmi P, Eklund K, Aro T. Counting and ran: Predictors of arithmetic calculation and reading fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2013; 105 (1):162–175.
  • La Paro KM, Pianta RC. Predicting children's competence in the early school years: A meta-analytic review. Review of Educational Research. 2000; 70 (4):443–484.
  • Ladd GW, Burgess KB. Do relational risks and protective factors moderate the linkages between childhood aggression and early psychological and school adjustment? Child Development. 2001; 72 (5):1579–1601. [ PubMed : 11699688 ]
  • Ladd GW, Kochenderfer BJ, Coleman CC. Friendship quality as a predictor of young children's early school adjustment. Child Development. 1996; 67 (3):1103–1118. [ PubMed : 8706512 ]
  • Ladd GW, Kochenderfer BJ, Coleman CC. Classroom peer acceptance, friendship, and victimization: Distinct relational systems that contribute uniquely to children's school adjustment? Child Development. 1997; 68 (6):1181–1197. [ PubMed : 9418233 ]
  • Ladd GW, Birch S, Buhs E. Children's social and scholastic lives in kindergarten: Related spheres of influence? Child Development. 1999; 70 :1373–1400. [ PubMed : 10621962 ]
  • Lalonde CE, Chandler MJ. Children's understanding of interpretation. New Ideas in Psychology. 2002; 20 (2-3):163–198.
  • Lan X, Legare CH, Ponitz CC, Li S, Morrison FJ. Investigating the links between the subcomponents of executive function and academic achievement: A cross-cultural analysis of chinese and American preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011; 108 :677–692. [ PubMed : 21238977 ]
  • Lane K, Barton-Arwood S, Nelson JR, Wehby J. Academic performance of students with emotional and behavioral disorders served in a self-contained setting. Journal of Behavioral Education. 2008; 17 (1):43–62.
  • Laski EV, Siegler RS. Learning from number board games: You learn what you encode. Developmental Psychology. 2014; 50 (3):853–864. [ PubMed : 24099546 ]
  • Lauderdale DS. Birth outcomes for Arabic-named women in California before and after September 11. Demography. 2006; 43 (1):185–201. [ PubMed : 16579214 ]
  • Lavigne JV, Lebailly SA, Hopkins J, Gouze KR, Binns HJ. The prevalence of ADHD, odd, depression, and anxiety in a community sample of 4-year-olds. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2009; 38 (3):315–328. [ PubMed : 19437293 ]
  • Lawrence J, Snow C. Oral discourse and reading. In: Kamil ML, Pearson PD, Moje EB, Afflerbach P, editors. Handbook of reading research. New York: Routledge; 2011.
  • Le Corre M, Van de Walle GA, Brannon EM, Carey S. Re-visiting the competence/performance debate in the acquisition of counting as a representation of the positive integers. Cognitive Psychology. 2006; 52 (2):130–169. [ PubMed : 16364281 ]
  • Leckman JF, March JS. Editorial: Developmental neuroscience comes of age. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011; 52 (4):333–338. [ PubMed : 21410471 ]
  • LeDoux J. The emotional brain: Development and psychopathology. New York: Simon & Schuster; 1996.
  • Lee JS. Size matters: Early vocabulary as a predictor of language and literacy competence. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2011; 32 (1):69–92.
  • Lee JS, Ginsburg HP. Preschool teachers' beliefs about appropriate early literacy and mathematics education for low- and middle-socioeconomic status children. Early Education & Development. 2007; 18 (1):111–143.
  • Lee K. Little liars: Development of verbal deception in children. Child Development Perspectives. 2013; 7 (2):91–96. [ PMC free article : PMC3653594 ] [ PubMed : 23687515 ]
  • Leerkes EM, Paradise MJ, O'Brien M, Calkins SD, Lange G. Emotion and cognition processes in preschool children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2008; 54 (1):102–124.
  • LeFevre J-A, Berrigan L, Vendetti C, Kamawar D, Bisanz J, Skwarchuk S-L, Smith-Chant BL. The role of executive attention in the acquisition of mathematical skills for children in grades 2 through 4. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2013; 114 (2):243–261. [ PubMed : 23168083 ]
  • Lerkkanen M-K, Rasku-Puttonen H, Aunola K, Nurmi J-E. Mathematical performance predicts progress in reading comprehension among 7-year-olds. European Journal of Psychology of Education. 2005; 20 (2):121–137.
  • Levine SC, Suriyakham LW, Rowe ML, Huttenlocher J, Gunderson EA. What counts in the development of young children's number knowledge? Developmental Psychology. 2010; 46 (5):1309–1319. [ PMC free article : PMC2998540 ] [ PubMed : 20822240 ]
  • Lewit EM, Baker LS. School readiness. The Future of Children/Center for the Future of Children, the David and Lucile Packard Foundation. 1995; 5 (2):128–139. [ PubMed : 8528685 ]
  • Libertus ME, Brannon EM. Stable individual differences in number discrimination in infancy. Developmental Science. 2010; 13 (6):900–906. [ PMC free article : PMC2966022 ] [ PubMed : 20977560 ]
  • Lieberman AF, Chu A, Van Horn P, Harris WW. Trauma in early childhood: Empirical evidence and clinical implications. Development and Psychopathology. 2011; 23 (2):397–410. [ PubMed : 23786685 ]
  • Lien DS, Evans WN. Estimating the impact of large cigarette tax hikes: The case of maternal smoking and infant birth weight. Journal of Human Resources. 2005; 40 (2):373–392.
  • Low M, Farrell A, Biggs BA, Pasricha SR. Effects of daily iron supplementation in primary-school-aged children: Systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Canadian Medical Association Journal. 2013; 185 (17):E791–E802. [ PMC free article : PMC3832580 ] [ PubMed : 24130243 ]
  • Lozoff B. Early iron deficiency has brain and behavior effects consistent with dopaminergic dysfunction. Journal of Nutrition. 2011; 141 (4):740s–746s. [ PMC free article : PMC3056585 ] [ PubMed : 21346104 ]
  • Lozoff B, Castillo M, Clark KM, Smith JB, Sturza J. Iron supplementation in infancy contributes to more adaptive behavior at 10 years of age. Journal of Nutrition. 2014; 144 (6):838–845. [ PMC free article : PMC4018948 ] [ PubMed : 24717366 ]
  • Luby JL. Handbook of preschool mental health: Development, disorders, and treatment. New York: Guilford Press; 2006.
  • Luby JL, Barch DM, Belden A, Gaffrey MS, Tillman R, Babb C, Nishino T, Suzuki H, Botteron KN. Maternal support in early childhood predicts larger hippocampal volumes at school age. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012; 109 (8):2854–2859. [ PMC free article : PMC3286943 ] [ PubMed : 22308421 ]
  • Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009; 10 (6):434–445. [ PubMed : 19401723 ]
  • Lynam D, Moffitt T, Stouthamer-Loeber M. Explaining the relation between IQ and delinquency: Class, race, test motivation, school failure, or self-control? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1993; 102 (2):187–196. [ PubMed : 8315131 ]
  • Lyon GR, Krasnegor NA. Attention, memory, and executive function. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.; 1996.
  • Maclellan E. Number sense: The underpinning understanding for early quantitative literacy. Numeracy. 2012; 5 (2):1–19.
  • Mages WK. Does creative drama promote language development in early childhood?: A review of the methods and measures employed in the empirical literature. Review of Educational Research. 2008; 78 (1):124–152.
  • Mahoney CR, Taylor HA, Kanarek RB, Samuel P. Effect of breakfast composition on cognitive processes in elementary school children. Physiology and Behavior. 2005; 85 (5):635–645. [ PubMed : 16085130 ]
  • Maier MF, Greenfield DB. The differential role of initiative and persistence in early childhood; Paper presented at Institute of Education Science 2007 Research Conference; Washington, DC. 2008.
  • Mandler JM. The foundations of mind origins of conceptual thought. 2004. [January 2, 2015]. http://site ​.ebrary.com/id/10103678 .
  • Mantzicopoulos P. The relationship of family variables to Head Start children's preacademic competence. Early Education and Development. 1997; 8 (4):357–375.
  • Marcovitch S, Zelazo PD. A hierarchical competing systems model of the emergence and early development of executive function. Developmental Science. 2009; 12 (1):1–25. [ PMC free article : PMC2842568 ] [ PubMed : 19120405 ]
  • Markman EM. Realizing that you don't understand: A preliminary investigation. Child Development. 1977; 48 :986–999.
  • Markman EM. Comprehension monitoring. In: Dickson WP, editor. Children's oral communication skills. New York: Academic Press; 1981. pp. 61–84.
  • Marsh HW, Craven R, Debus R. Structure, stability, and development of young children's self-concepts: A multicohort-multioccasion study. Child Development. 1998; 69 (4):1030–1053. [ PubMed : 9768485 ]
  • Marsh HW, Ellis LA, Craven RG. How do preschool children feel about themselves? Unraveling measurement and multidimensional self-concept structure. Developmental Psychology. 2002; 38 (3):376–393. [ PubMed : 12005381 ]
  • Masataka N, Ohnishi T, Imabayashi E, Hirakata M, Matsuda H. Neural correlates for numerical processing in the manual mode. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 2006; 11 (2):144–152. [ PubMed : 16319374 ]
  • Mashburn AJ, Pianta RC, Hamre BK, Downer JT, Barbarin OA, Bryant D, Burchinal M, Early DM, Howes C. Measures of classroom quality in prekindergarten and children's development of academic, language, and social skills. Child Development. 2008; 79 (3):732–749. [ PubMed : 18489424 ]
  • Mashburn AJ, Justice LM, Downer JT, Pianta RC. Peer effects on children's language achievement during pre-kindergarten. Child Development. 2009; 80 (3):686–702. [ PubMed : 19489897 ]
  • Masten AS, Herbers JE, Desjardins CD, Cutuli JJ, McCormick CM, Sapienza JK, Long JD, Zelazo PD. Executive function skills and school success in young children experiencing homelessness. Educational Researcher. 2012; 41 (9):375–384.
  • Master A, Markman EM, Dweck CS. Thinking in categories or along a continuum: Consequences for children's social judgments. Child Development. 2012; 83 (4):1145–1163. [ PubMed : 22540868 ]
  • Mazzocco MMM, Hanich LB. Math achievement, numerical processing, and executive functions in girls with Turner syndrome: Do girls with Turner syndrome have math learning disability? Learning and Individual Differences. 2010; 20 :70–81.
  • Mazzocco MMM, Myers GF. Complexities in identifying and defining mathematics learning disability in the primary school-age years. Annuals of Dyslexia. 2003; 53 :218–253. [ PMC free article : PMC2742419 ] [ PubMed : 19750132 ]
  • Mazzocco MMM, Thompson RE. Kindergarten predictors of math learning disability. Learning Disability Quarterly Research and Practice. 2005; 20 :142–155. [ PMC free article : PMC2806680 ] [ PubMed : 20084182 ]
  • McClelland MM, Cameron CE, Connor CM, Farris CL, Jewkes AM, Morrison FJ. Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers' literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental Psychology. 2007; 43 (4):947–959. [ PubMed : 17605527 ]
  • McCormick MC, Brooks-Gunn J, Buka SL, Goldman J, Yu J, Salganik M, Scott DT, Bennett FC, Kay LL, Bernbaum JC, Bauer CR, Martin C, Woods ER, Martin A, Casey PH. Early intervention in low birth weight premature infants: Results at 18 years of age for the infant health and development program. Pediatrics. 2006; 117 (3):771–780. [ PubMed : 16510657 ]
  • McEwen BS. Brain on stress: How the social environment gets under the skin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012; 109 (Suppl. 2):17180–17185. [ PMC free article : PMC3477378 ] [ PubMed : 23045648 ]
  • McGillicuddy-De Lisi AV. The relationship between parents' beliefs about development and family constellation, socioeconomic status, and parents' teaching strategies. In: Laosa LM, Sigel IE, editors. Families as learning environments for children. New York: Plenum Press; 1982. pp. 261–299.
  • McIntyre LL, Blacher J, Baker BL. The transition to school: Adaptation in young children with and without intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2006; 50 (Pt. 5):349–361. [ PubMed : 16629928 ]
  • McLean JF, Hitch GJ. Working memory impairments in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1999; 74 :240–260. [ PubMed : 10527556 ]
  • McLeod DB, Adams VM, editors. Affect and mathematical problem solving. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1989.
  • McMillan T. National Geographic Magazine. Aug, 2014. 2014. The new face of hunger; pp. 66–68. 70, 72-74, 77-80, 83-89.
  • Meltzoff AN. Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology. 1995; 31 (5):838–850. [ PMC free article : PMC4137788 ] [ PubMed : 25147406 ]
  • Metsala JL, Walley AC. Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In: Metsala JL, Ehri LC, editors. Word recognition in beginning literacy. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1998. pp. 89–120.
  • Middleton JA, Spanias P. Motivation for achievement in mathematics: Findings, generalizations, and criticisms of the research. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 1999; 30 :65–88.
  • Miller AL, Seifer R, Stroud L, Sheinkopf SJ, Dickstein S. Biobehavioral indices of emotion regulation relate to school attitudes, motivation, and behavior problems in a low-income preschool sample. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2006; 1094 :325–329. [ PubMed : 17347370 ]
  • Miller GE, Chen E, Parker KJ. Psychological stress in childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: Moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin. 2011; 137 (6):959–997. [ PMC free article : PMC3202072 ] [ PubMed : 21787044 ]
  • Mills CM. Knowing when to doubt: Developing a critical stance when learning from others. Developmental Psychology. 2013; 49 (3):404–418. [ PMC free article : PMC3810952 ] [ PubMed : 22889395 ]
  • Mischel W, Shoda Y, Peake PK. The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988; 54 (4):687–696. [ PubMed : 3367285 ]
  • Mitchell-Copeland J, Denham SA, DeMulder EK. Q-sort assessment of child–teacher attachment relationships and social competence in the preschool. Early Education and Development. 1997; 8 (1):27–39.
  • Mix KS, Huttenlocher J, Levine SC. Quantitative development in infancy and early childhood. New York: Oxford University Press; 2002.
  • Mix KS, Sandhofer CM, Baroody AJ. Number words and number concepts: The interplay of verbal and nonverbal processes in early quantitative development. Kail R, editor. New York: Academic Press; Advances in child development and behavior. 2005; 33 :305–345. [ PubMed : 16101121 ]
  • Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology. 2000; 41 (1):49–100. [ PubMed : 10945922 ]
  • Mol SE, Bus AG, de Jong MT. Interactive book reading in early education: A tool to stimulate print knowledge as well as oral language. Review of Educational Research. 2009; 79 (2):979–1007.
  • Moll LC, Amanti C, Neff D, Gonzalez N. Funds of knowledge for teaching: Using a qualitative approach to connect homes and classrooms. Theory into Practice. 1992; 31 (2):132–141.
  • Mooji T. Design and implementation of ICT-supported education for highly able pupils; Paper read at European Conference on Educational Research; Helsinki, Finland. 2010.
  • Morgan GA, Harmon RJ, Maslin-Cole CA. Mastery motivation: Definition and measurement. Early Education and Development. 1990; 1 (5):318–339.
  • Morgan PL, Farkas G, Tufis PA, Sperling RA. Are reading and behavior problems risk factors for each other? Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2008; 41 (5):417–436. [ PMC free article : PMC4422059 ] [ PubMed : 18768774 ]
  • Morrow LM, Schickedanz JA. The relationships between sociodramatic play and literacy development. Dickinson DK, Neuman SB, editors. New York and London: Guilford Press; Handbook of early literacy research. 2006; 2
  • Müller U, Lieberman D, Frye D, Zelazo PD. Executive function, school readiness, and school achievement. In: Fiorello C, Thurman K, editors. Cognitive development in K-3 classroom learning: Research applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2008. pp. 41–84.
  • Mullis IVS, Martin MO, Foy P, Arora A. TIMSS 2011 international results in mathematics. Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College, Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study and Progress in International Reading Literacy Study International Study Center; 2012.
  • Munn P. Mathematics in early childhood—the early years math curriculum in the UK and children's numerical development. International Journal of Early Childhood. 2006; 38 (1):99–112.
  • Nachmias M, Gunnar M, Mangelsdorf S, Parritz RH, Buss K. Behavioral inhibition and stress reactivity: The moderating role of attachment security. Child Development. 1996; 67 (2):508–522. [ PubMed : 8625725 ]
  • Nader-Grosbois N, Lefèvre N. Self-regulation and performance in problem-solving using physical materials or computers in children with intellectual disability. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2011; 32 :1492–1505. [ PubMed : 21367576 ]
  • Nagy W, Berninger VW, Abbott RD. Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2006; 98 (1):134–147.
  • Nation K, Snowling MJ. Developmental differences in sensitivity to semantic relations among good and poor comprehenders: Evidence from semantic priming. Cognition. 1999; 70 (1):1. [ PubMed : 10193058 ]
  • National Early Literacy Panel. Developing early literacy: A report of the National Early Literacy Panel. 2008. [December 15, 2014]. http://purl ​.access.gpo.gov/GPO/LPS108121 .
  • National Mathematics Advisory Panel. Foundations for success: The final report of the National Mathematics Advisory Panel. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Office of Planning, Evaluation and Policy Development; 2008.
  • Nayfeld I, Fuccillo J, Greenfield DB. Executive functions in early learning: Extending the relationship between executive functions and school readiness to science. Learning and Individual Differences. 2013; 26 :81–88.
  • NCTSN (National Child Traumatic Stress Network). Understanding child traumatic stress. 2005. [March 23, 2015]. http://www ​.nctsnet.org ​/sites/default/files ​/assets/pdfs/understanding ​_child_traumatic ​_stress_brochure_9-29-05.pdf .
  • NCTSN Core Curriculum on Childhood Trauma Task Force. The 12 core concepts: Concepts for understanding traumatic stress responses in children and families. Core curriculum on childhood trauma. Los Angeles, CA, and Durham, NC: UCLA-Duke University National Center for Child Traumatic Stress; 2012.
  • Neece CL, Baker BL, Blacher J, Crnic KA. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among children with and without intellectual disability: An examination across time. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2011; 55 (7):623–635. [ PubMed : 21492290 ]
  • Nelson JR, Benner GJ, Lane K, Smith BW. Academic achievement of K-12 students with emotional and behavioral disorders. Exceptional Children. 2004; 71 (1):59–73.
  • Nes FTv. Young children's spatial structuring ability and emerging number sense. de Universtiteit Utrecht; Utrecht, The Netherlands: 2009. (PhD diss.).
  • Neuenschwander R, Röthlisberger M, Cimeli P, Roebers CM. How do different aspects of self-regulation predict successful adaptation to school? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2012; 113 (3):353–371. [ PubMed : 22920433 ]
  • NICHD (Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development) Early Child Care Research Network. Social functioning in first grade: Associations with earlier home and child care predictors and with current classroom experiences. Child Development. 2003; 74 (6):1639–1662. [ PubMed : 14669887 ]
  • NICHD (Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development) Early Child Care Research Network. A day in third grade: A large-scale study of classroom quality and teacher and student behavior. Elementary School Journal. 2005; 105 (3):305–323.
  • NRC (National Research Council). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1998.
  • NRC (National Research Council). Eager to learn: Educating our preschoolers. Bowman BT, Donovan MS, Burns MS, editors. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2001.
  • NRC (National Research Council). Mathematics in early childhood: Learning paths toward excellence and equity. Cross CT, Woods TA, Schweingruber H, editors. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2009.
  • NRC and IOM. From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Shonkoff JP, Phillips DA, editors. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2000. [ PubMed : 25077268 ]
  • NRC and IOM. Depression in parents, parenting, and children: Opportunities to improve identification, treatment, and prevention. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2009. [ PubMed : 25009931 ]
  • Nunes T, Bryant P, Barros R. The development of word recognition and its significance for comprehension and fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2012; 104 (4):959–973.
  • Nyaradi A, Li J, Hickling S, Foster J, Oddy WH. The role of nutrition in children's neurocognitive development, from pregnancy through childhood. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013; 7 :97. [ PMC free article : PMC3607807 ] [ PubMed : 23532379 ]
  • O'Connor E, McCartney K. Examining teacher–child relationships and achievement as part of an ecological model of development. American Educational Research Journal. 2007; 44 (2):340–369.
  • O'Connor RE. Teaching word recognition effective strategies for students with learning difficulties. New York: Guilford Press; 2014.
  • O'Connor RE, Jenkins JR. Improving the generalization of sound-symbol knowledge: Teaching spelling to kindergarten children with disabilities. Journal of Special Education. 1995; 29 (3):255–275.
  • O'Connor RE, Jenkins JR, Slocum TA. Transfer among phonological tasks in kindergarten: Essential instructional content. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1995; 2 :202–217.
  • Olson SL, Hoza B. Preschool developmental antecedents of conduct problems in children beginning school. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1993; 22 (1):60.
  • O'Neill DK, Pearce MJ, Pick JL. Preschool children's narratives and performance on the peabody individualized achievement test—revised: Evidence of a relation between early narrative and later mathematical ability. First Language. 2004; 24 (2):149–183.
  • Oreopoulos P, Stabile M, Walld R, Roos LL. Short-, medium-, and long-term consequences of poor infant health: An analysis using siblings and twins. Journal of Human Resources. 2008; 43 (1):88–138.
  • Ostad SA. Subtraction strategies in developmental perspective: A comparison of mathematically normal and mathematically disabled children. Olivier A, Newstead K, editors. Stellenbosch, South Africa: University of Stellenbosch; Proceedings of the 22nd Conference for the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education. 1998; 3 :311–318.
  • Pagani L, Messier S. Links between motor skills and indicators of school readiness at kindergarten entry in urban disadvantaged children. Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology. 2012; 2 (1):95.
  • Palmer A, Baroody AJ. Blake's development of the number words “one,” “two,” and “three.” Cognition and Instruction. 2011; 29 :265–296.
  • Parish-Morris J, Mahajan N, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM, Collins MF. Once upon a time: Parent–child dialogue and storybook reading in the electronic era. Mind, Brain, and Education. 2013; 7 (3):200–211.
  • Passolunghi MC, Vercelloni B, Schadee H. The precursors of mathematics learning: Working memory, phonological ability and numerical competence. Cognitive Development. 2007; 22 (2):165–184.
  • Pearson PD, Hiebert EH. National reports in literacy: Building a scientific base for policy and practice. Educational Researcher. 2010; 39 :286–294.
  • Perfetti CA. Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press; 1985.
  • Perfetti CA, Hart L. The lexical quality hypothesis. Verhoeven LT, Elbro C, Reitsma P, editors. Amsterdam and Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing Company; Precursors of functional literacy. 2002; 11 :67–86.
  • Piaget J, Szeminska A. The child's conception of number. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul; 1952.
  • Pianta RC. Enhancing relationships between children and teachers. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1999.
  • Pianta RC, Stuhlman MW. Conceptualizing risk in relational terms: Associations among the quality of child–adult relationships prior to school entry and children's developmental outcomes in first grade. Educational and Child Psychology. 2004a; 21 (1):32–45.
  • Pianta RC, Stuhlman MW. Teacher–child relationships and children's success in the first years of school. School Psychology Review. 2004b; 33 (3):444–458.
  • Pianta RC, Steinberg MS, Rollins KB. The first two years of school: Teacher–child relationships and deflections in children's classroom adjustment. Development and Psychopathology. 1995; 7 (02):295–312.
  • Piazza M, Izard V, Pinel P, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S. Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. Neuron. 2004; 44 :547–555. [ PubMed : 15504333 ]
  • Pinhas M, Donohue SE, Woldorff MG, Brannon EM. Electrophysiological evidence for the involvement of the approximate number system in preschoolers' processing of spoken number words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2014; 26 (9):1891–1904. [ PMC free article : PMC4228473 ] [ PubMed : 24702455 ]
  • Ponitz CC, McClelland MM, Matthews JS, Morrison FJ. A structured observation of behavioral self-regulation and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. Developmental Psychology. 2009; 45 (3):605–619. [ PubMed : 19413419 ]
  • Praet M, Titeca D, Ceulemans A, Desoete A. Language in the prediction of arithmetics in kindergarten and grade 1. Learning and Individual Differences. 2013; 27 :90–96.
  • Purpura DJ, Hume LE, Sims DM, Lonigan CJ. Early literacy and early numeracy: The value of including early literacy skills in the prediction of numeracy development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011; 110 :647–658. [ PubMed : 21831396 ]
  • Raches D, Mazzocco MMM. Emergence and nature of mathematical difficulties in young children with Barth syndrome. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2012; 33 (4):328–335. [ PubMed : 22566029 ]
  • Rachlin H. The science of self-control. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2000.
  • Raikes H, Pan BA, Luze G, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Brooks-Gunn J, Constantine J, Tarullo LB, Raikes HA, Rodriguez ET. Mother-child bookreading in low-income families: Correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Development. 2006; 77 (4):924–953. [ PubMed : 16942498 ]
  • Ramirez G, Gunderson EA, Levine SC, Beilock SL. Math anxiety, working memory, and math achievement in early elementary school. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2013; 14 (2):187–202.
  • Rampersaud GC, Pereira MA, Girard BL, Adams J, Metzl JD. Breakfast habits, nutritional status, body weight, and academic performance in children and adolescents. (quiz 761-742). Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 2005; 105 (5):743–760. [ PubMed : 15883552 ]
  • Raver CC. Targeting self-regulation through intervention: Lessons from RCTS; Paper presented at Society for Research on Educational Effectiveness (SREE); September 27; Washington, DC. 2013.
  • Raver CC, Knitzer J. Ready to enter: What research tells policymakers about strategies to promote social and emotional school readiness among three- and four-year-old children. New York: Columbia University, National Center for Children in Poverty, Mailman School of Public Health; 2002.
  • Raver CC, Jones SM, Li-Grining C, Zhai F, Metzger MW, Solomon B. Targeting children's behavior problems in preschool classrooms: A cluster-randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2009; 77 (2):302–316. [ PubMed : 19309189 ]
  • Raver CC, Jones SM, Li-Grining C, Zhai F, Bub K, Pressler E. CSRP's impact on low-income preschoolers' preacademic skills: Self-regulation as a mediating mechanism. Child Development. 2011; 82 (1):362–378. [ PMC free article : PMC3682645 ] [ PubMed : 21291447 ]
  • Reigosa-Crespo V, González-Alemañy E, León T, Torres R, Mosquera R, Valdés-Sosa M. Numerical capacities as domain-specific predictors beyond early mathematics learning: A longitudinal study. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8 (11):e79711. [ PMC free article : PMC3821842 ] [ PubMed : 24255710 ]
  • Rimm-Kaufman SE, Pianta RC, Cox MJ. Teachers' judgments of problems in the transition to kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2000; 15 (2):147–166.
  • Roberts TA. Home storybook reading in primary or second language with preschool children: Evidence of equal effectiveness for second-language vocabulary acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly. 2008; 43 (2):103–130.
  • Roebers CM, Cimeli P, Röthlisberger M, Neuenschwander R. Executive functioning, metacognition, and self-perceived competence in elementary school children: An explorative study on their interrelations and their role for school achievement. Metacognition Learning. 2012; 7 (3):151–173.
  • Rogoff B. Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press; 1991. [ PubMed : 25813118 ]
  • Rogoff B, Angelillo C. Investigating the coordinated functioning of multifaceted cultural practices in human development. Human Development. 2002; 45 (4):211–225.
  • Romine CB, Reynolds CR. A model of the development of frontal lobe functioning: Findings from a meta-analysis. Applied Neuropsychology. 2005; 12 (4):190–201. [ PubMed : 16422660 ]
  • Romine CB, Lee D, Wolfe ME, Homack S, George C, Riccio CA. Wisconsin card sorting test with children: A meta-analytic study of sensitivity and specificity. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2004; 19 (8):1027–1041. [ PubMed : 15533695 ]
  • Rones M, Hoagwood K. School-based mental health services: A research review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2000; 3 (4):223–241. [ PubMed : 11225738 ]
  • Roseberry S, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Development. 2014; 85 (3):956–970. [ PMC free article : PMC3962808 ] [ PubMed : 24112079 ]
  • Rossin-Slater M. WIC in your neighborhood: New evidence on the impacts of geographic access to clinics. Journal of Public Economics. 2013; 102 :51–69. [ PMC free article : PMC3772681 ] [ PubMed : 24043906 ]
  • Rothbart MK, Rueda MR. The development of effortful control. In: Mayr U, Awh E, Keele S, editors. Developing individuality in the human brain: A tribute to Michael I. Posner. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2005. pp. 167–188.
  • Rouse C, Brooks-Gunn J, McLanahan S. Introducing the issue. The Future of Children. 2005; 15 :5–14.
  • Rowe ML. A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development. 2012; 83 (5):1762–1774. [ PMC free article : PMC3440540 ] [ PubMed : 22716950 ]
  • Royer H. Separated at girth: US twin estimates of the effects of birth weight. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 2009; 1 (1):49–85.
  • Rutter M. Achievements and challenges in the biology of environmental effects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012; 109 (Suppl. 2):17149–17153. [ PMC free article : PMC3477381 ] [ PubMed : 23045650 ]
  • Saarni CD, Mumme D, Campos JJ. Emotional development: Action, communication, and understanding. In: Damon W, editor. Handbook of child psychology. 5th. New York: Wiley; 1998. pp. 237–309.
  • Sadler PM, Tai RH. The two high-school pillars supporting college science. Science. 2007; 317 :457–458. [ PubMed : 17656706 ]
  • Saffran JR. Statistical language learning: Mechanisms and constraints. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2003; 12 (4):110–114.
  • Saffran JR, Aslin RN, Newport EL. Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science. 1996; 274 (5294):1926–1928. [ PubMed : 8943209 ]
  • Saffran JR, Johnson EK, Aslin RN, Newport EL. Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition. 1999; 70 (1):27–52. [ PubMed : 10193055 ]
  • Samarapungavan A, Patrick H, Mantzicopoulos P. What kindergarten students learn in inquiry-based science classrooms. Cognition and Instruction. 2011; 29 (4):416–470.
  • Sanders NJ. What doesn't kill you makes you weaker: Prenatal pollution exposure and educational outcomes. Journal of Human Resources. 2012; 47 (3):826–850.
  • Sandhofer CM, Smith LB. Learning color words involves learning a system of mappings. Developmental Psychology. 1999; 35 :668–679. [ PubMed : 10380858 ]
  • Sandler AD, Watson TE, Footo M, Levine MD, Coleman WL, Hooper SR. Neurodevelopmental study of writing disorders in middle childhood. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 1992; 13 (1):17–23. [ PubMed : 1556195 ]
  • Sandman CA, Davis EP, Buss C, Glynn LM. Exposure to prenatal psychobiological stress exerts programming influences on the mother and her fetus. Neuroendocrinology. 2012; 95 (1):8–21. [ PMC free article : PMC7068789 ] [ PubMed : 21494029 ]
  • Sarama J, Clements DH. Early childhood mathematics education research: Learning trajectories for young children. New York: Routledge; 2009.
  • Sarama J, Lange A, Clements DH, Wolfe CB. The impacts of an early mathematics curriculum on emerging literacy and language. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2012; 27 :489–502.
  • Saxe GB, Guberman SR, Gearhart M. Social processes in early number development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1987; 52 (2, Serial #216)
  • Saxe R. The new puzzle of theory of mind development. In: Banaji MR, Gelman SA, editors. Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us. New York: Oxford University Press; 2013.
  • Scarborough HS. Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In: Neuman SB, Dickinson DK, editors. Handbook of early literacy research. New York: Guilford Press; 2001. pp. 97–110.
  • Schleppegrell MJ. Grammar for writing: Academic language and the ELD Standards. Santa Barbara, CA: University of California Linguistic Minority Research Institute; 2003.
  • Schneidman LA, Arroyo ME, Levine SC, Goldin-Meadow S. What counts as effective input for word learning? Journal of Child Language. 2013; 40 (3):672–686. [ PMC free article : PMC3445663 ] [ PubMed : 22575125 ]
  • Schoemaker K, Bunte T, Espy KA, Dekovic M, Matthys W. Executive functions in preschool children with ADHD and DBD: An 18-month longitudinal study. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2014; 39 (4):302–315. [ PubMed : 24854774 ]
  • Schore J, Schore A. Modern attachment theory: The central role of affect regulation in development and treatment. Clinical Social Work Journal. 2008; 36 (1):9–20.
  • Schulz LE, Bonawitz EB. Serious fun: Preschoolers engage in more exploratory play when evidence is confounded. Developmental Psychology. 2007; 43 (4):1045–1050. [ PubMed : 17605535 ]
  • Secada WG. Race, ethnicity, social class, language, and achievement in mathematics. In: Grouws DA, editor. Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning. Toronto and New York: Macmillan, Maxwell Macmillan Canada, Maxwell Macmillan International; 1992. pp. 623–660.
  • Semrud-Clikeman M. The role of inattention on academics, fluid reasoning, and visual-spatial functioning in two subtypes of ADHD. Applied Neuropsychology. Child. 2012; 1 (1):18–29. [ PubMed : 23428274 ]
  • Sénéchal M, Ouellette G, Rodney D. The misunderstood giant: On the predictive role of early vocabulary in future reading. Dickinson DK, Neuman SB, editors. New York: Guilford Press; Handbook of early literacy research. 2006; 2 :173–184.
  • Seo K-H, Ginsburg HP. What is developmentally appropriate in early childhood mathematics education? In: Clements DH, Sarama J, DiBiase A-M, editors. Engaging young children in mathematics: Standards for early childhood mathematics education. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2004. pp. 91–104.
  • Shayer M, Adhami M. Realizing the cognitive potential of children 5-7 with a mathematics focus: Post-test and long-term effects of a 2-year intervention. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 2010; 80 (3):363–379. [ PubMed : 20070920 ]
  • Shonkoff JP, Boyce WT, McEwen BS. Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities: Building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2009; 301 (21):2252–2259. [ PubMed : 19491187 ]
  • Shuai L, Chan RC, Wang Y. Executive function profile of Chinese boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Different subtypes and comorbidity. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2011; 26 (2):120–132. [ PubMed : 21177762 ]
  • Siegel LS, Mazabel S. Basic cognitive processes and reading disabilities. In: Swanson HL, Harris KR, Graham S, editors. Handbook of learning disabilities. New York: Guilford Press; 2013. pp. 186–213.
  • Silver RB, Measelle JR, Armstrong JM, Essex MJ. Trajectories of classroom externalizing behavior: Contributions of child characteristics, family characteristics, and the teacher–child relationship during the school transition. Journal of School Psychology. 2005; 43 (1):39–60.
  • Simmons FR, Willis C, Adams A-M. Different components of working memory have different relationships with different mathematical skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2012; 111 (2):139–155. [ PubMed : 22018889 ]
  • Skinner EA, Belmont MJ. Motivation in the classroom: Reciprocal effects of teacher behavior and student engagement across the school year. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1993; 85 (4):571–581.
  • Slaughter V, Itakura S, Kutsuki A, Siegal M. Learning to count begins in infancy: Evidence from 18 month olds' visual preferences. Proceedings: Biological Sciences. 2011; 278 (1720):2979–2984. [ PMC free article : PMC3151703 ] [ PubMed : 21325331 ]
  • Smiley PA, Dweck CS. Individual differences in achievement goals among young children. Child Development. 1994; 65 (6):1723–1743. [ PubMed : 7859551 ]
  • Smith MW, Dickinson DK. Describing oral language opportunities and environments in Head Start and other preschool classrooms. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 1994; 9 (3-4):345–366.
  • Snow CE, Uccelli P. The challenge of academic language. In: Olson DR, Torrance N, editors. In Cambridge handbook of literacy. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press; 2009. pp. 112–133.
  • Snow CE, Tabors PO, Dickinson DK. Language development in the preschool years. In: Dickinson DK, Tabors PO, editors. Beginning literacy with language: Young children learning at home and school. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.; 2001. pp. 1–25.
  • Spelke ES, Kinzler KD. Core knowledge. Developmental Science. 2007; 10 (1):89–96. [ PubMed : 17181705 ]
  • Stanovich KE, Siegel LS. Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1994; 86 :24–53.
  • Starr A, Libertus ME, Brannon EM. Infancy. 2013. Infants show ratio-dependent number discrimination regardless of set size; pp. 1–15. [ PMC free article : PMC3864890 ] [ PubMed : 24353478 ]
  • Steacy LM, Kirby JR, Parrila R, Compton DL. Classification of double deficit groups across time: An analysis of group stability from kindergarten to second grade. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2014; 18 (4):255–273.
  • Steffe LP, Tzur R. Interaction and children's mathematics. Journal of Research in Childhood Education. 1994; 8 (2):99–116.
  • Sternberg R. Beyond IQ. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press; 1985.
  • Stevenson HW, Newman RS. Long-term prediction of achievement and attitudes in mathematics and reading. Child Development. 1986; 57 :646–659. [ PubMed : 3720396 ]
  • Stipek D, Recchia S, McClintic S. Self-evaluation in young children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1992; 57 (1):1–98. [ PubMed : 1560797 ]
  • Strouse GA, O'Doherty K, Troseth GL. Effective coviewing: Preschoolers' learning from video after a dialogic questioning intervention. Developmental Psychology. 2013; 49 (12):2368–2382. [ PubMed : 23544859 ]
  • Szűcs D, Devine A, Soltesz F, Nobes A, Gabriel F. Cognitive components of a mathematical processing network in 9-year-old children. Developmental Science. 2014; 17 (4):506–524. [ PMC free article : PMC4253132 ] [ PubMed : 25089322 ]
  • Taras H. Nutrition and student performance at school. Journal of School Health. 2005; 75 (6):199–213. [ PubMed : 16014126 ]
  • Thayer ZM, Kuzawa CW. Early origins of health disparities: Material deprivation predicts maternal evening cortisol in pregnancy and offspring cortisol reactivity in the first few weeks of life. American Journal of Human Biology. 2014; 26 (6):723–730. [ PubMed : 24599586 ]
  • Thompson RA. The development of the person: Social understanding, relationships, self, conscience. Damon W, Lerner RM, editors. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; Handbook of child psychology. (6th) 2006; 3 :24–98.
  • Thompson RA. Early attachment and later development: Familiar questions, new answers. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. 2nd. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 348–365.
  • Thompson RA. Whither the preconventional child? Toward a life-span moral development theory. Child Development Perspectives. 2012; 6 (4):423–429.
  • Thompson RA. Attachment theory and research: Precis and prospect. Zelazo P, editor. New York: Oxford University Press; Oxford handbook of developmental psychology. 2013; 2 :191–216.
  • Thompson RA. Stress and child development. The Future of Children. 2014; 24 (1):41–59. [ PubMed : 25518702 ]
  • Thompson RA. The development of virtue: A perspective from developmental psychology. In: Snow NE, editor. Cultivating virtue: Perspectives from philosophy, theology, and psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2015.
  • Thompson RA. Early attachment and later development: Reframing the questions. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment. 3rd. New York: Guilford Press; in press.
  • Thompson RA, Goodvin R. Social support and developmental psychopathology. In: Cicchetti D, editor. Developmental psychopathology. 3rd. New York: John Wiley & Sons; in press.
  • Thompson RA, Laible DJ, Ontai LL. Early understandings of emotion, morality, and self: Developing a working model. Advances in Child Development and Behavior. 2003; 31 :137–171. [ PubMed : 14528661 ]
  • Thomson S, Rowe K, Underwood C, Peck R. Numeracy in the early years: Project good start. Camberwell, Victoria, Australia: Australian Council for Educational Research; 2005.
  • Toll SWM, Van der Ven S, Kroesbergen E, Van Luit JEH. Executive functions as predictors of math learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2010; 20 (10):1–12.
  • Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. (discussion 691-735). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2005; 28 (5):675–691. [ PubMed : 16262930 ]
  • Tomlinson HB. An overview of development in the primary grades. In: Copple C, Bredekamp S, Koralek DG, Charner K, editors. Developmentally appropriate practice. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children; 2014. pp. 9–38.
  • Trentacosta CJ, Izard CE. Kindergarten children's emotion competence as a predictor of their academic competence in first grade. Emotion. 2007; 7 (1):77–88. [ PubMed : 17352565 ]
  • Trick LM, Pylyshyn ZW. Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently?: A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. Psychological Review. 1994; 101 :80–102. [ PubMed : 8121961 ]
  • Troseth GL, Saylor MM, Archer AH. Young children's use of video as a source of socially relevant information. Child Development. 2006; 77 (3):786–799. [ PubMed : 16686801 ]
  • Tseng V, Seidman E. A systems framework for understanding social settings. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2007; 39 (3-4):217–228. [ PubMed : 17436080 ]
  • Tunmer W, Hoover W. Components of variance models of language-related factors in reading disability: A conceptual overview. In: Joshi RJ, Leong CK, editors. Reading disabilities: Diagnosis and component processes. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer; 1993. pp. 135–173.
  • Tymms P, Merrell C. ADHD and academic attainment: Is there an advantage in impulsivity? Learning and Individual Differences. 2011; 21 (6):753–758.
  • Uccelli P, Hemphill L, Pan BA, Snow C. Conversing with toddlers about the nonpresent: Precursors to narrative development in two genres. In: Balter L, Tamis-LeMonda CS, editors. Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues. 2nd. New York: Taylor & Francis; 2006. pp. 215–237.
  • Ulrich-Lai YM, Herman JP. Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience. 2009; 10 (6):397–409. [ PMC free article : PMC4240627 ] [ PubMed : 19469025 ]
  • U.S. Department of Education Office for Civil Rights. Civil rights data collection: Data snapshot (school discipline). 2014. [February 9, 2015]. (Issue brief no. 1). http://www2 ​.ed.gov/about ​/offices/list/ocr ​/docs/crdc-discipline-snapshot.pdf .
  • Valiente C, Eisenberg N, Haugen R, Spinrad TL, Hofer C, Liew J, Kupfer AS. Children's effortful control and academic achievement: Mediation through social functioning. Early Education & Development. 2011; 22 (3):411–433. [ PMC free article : PMC3346258 ] [ PubMed : 22573931 ]
  • Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen M. Assessment and realistic mathematics education. Utrecht, The Netherlands: Utrecht University, Freudenthal Institute; 1996.
  • Van der Ven SHG, Kroesbergen EH, Boom J, Leseman PPM. The development of executive functions and early mathematics: A dynamic relationship. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 2012; 82 (1):100–119. [ PubMed : 22429060 ]
  • van Ijzendoorn MH, Dijkstra J, Bus AG. Attachment, intelligence, and language: A meta-analysis. Social Development. 1995; 4 (2):115–128.
  • van Kleek A. Fostering preliteracy development via storybook-sharing interactions: The cultural context of mainstream family practices. In: Stone CA, Silliman ER, Ehren B, Apel K, editors. Handbook of language and literacy: Development and disorders. New York: Guilford Press; 2004. pp. 175–208.
  • Van Luit JEH, Van de Rijt BAM. Effectiveness of the additional early mathematics program for teaching children early mathematics. Instructional Science. 1998; 26 :337–358.
  • Vasilyeva M, Waterfall H. Variability in language development: Relation to socioeconomic status and environmental input. Neuman SB, Dickinson DK, editors. New York: Guilford Press; Handbook of early literacy research. 2011; 3 :36–48.
  • Vasilyeva M, Huttenlocher J, Waterfall H. Effects of language intervention on syntactic skill levels in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology. 2006; 42 (1):164–174. [ PubMed : 16420126 ]
  • Vellutino FR, Tunmer WE, Jaccard JJ, Chen R. Components of reading ability: Multivariate evidence for a convergent skills model of reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2007; 11 (1):3–32.
  • Vest JR, Catlin TK, Chen JJ, Brownson RC. Multistate analysis of factors associated with intimate partner violence. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2002; 22 (3):156–164. [ PubMed : 11897459 ]
  • Vieillevoye S, Nader-Grosbois N. Self-regulation during pretend play in children with intellectual disability and in normally developing children. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2008; 29 (3):256–272. [ PubMed : 17576048 ]
  • Vitiello VE, Greenfield DB, Munis P, George JL. Cognitive flexibility, approaches to learning, and academic school readiness in head start preschool children. Early Education & Development. 2011; 22 (3):388–410.
  • von Suchodoletz A, Gunzenhauser C. Behavior regulation and early math and vocabulary knowledge in German preschool children. Early Education & Development. 2013; 24 (3):310–331.
  • Vukovic RK. Mathematics difficulty with and without reading difficulty: Findings and implications from a four-year longitudinal study. Exceptional Children. 2012; 78 :280–300.
  • Vukovic RK, Lesaux NK. The language of mathematics: Investigating the ways language counts for children's mathematical development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2013; 115 (2):227–244. [ PubMed : 23563157 ]
  • Vygotsky LS. Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cole M, John-Steiner V, Scribner S, Souberman E, editors. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1978.
  • Vygotsky LS. Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1986.
  • Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Laughon P, Simmons K, Rashotte CA. Development of young readers' phonological processing abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1993; 85 :83–103.
  • Wagner SW, Walters J. A longitudinal analysis of early number concepts: From numbers to number. In: Forman GE, editor. Action and thought. New York: Academic Press; 1982. pp. 137–161.
  • Wang J, Barrett KC. Mastery motivation and self-regulation during early childhood. In: Barrett KC, Fox NA, Morgan GA, Fidler DJ, Daunhauer LA, editors. Handbook of self-regulatory processes in development new directions and international perspectives. Boca Raton, FL: Taylor & Francis Press; 2013. pp. 337–380.
  • Wanless SB, McClelland MM, Tominey SL, Acock AC. The influence of demographic risk factors on children's behavioral regulation in prekindergarten and kindergarten. Early Education & Development. 2011; 22 (3):461–488.
  • Warneken F, Tomasello M. Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science. 2006; 311 (5765):1301–1303. [ PubMed : 16513986 ]
  • Warneken F, Tomasello M. Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age. Infancy. 2007; 11 (3):271–294. [ PubMed : 33412734 ]
  • Weiner B. An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1986.
  • Weisleder A, Fernald A. Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science. 2013; 24 (11):2143–2152. [ PMC free article : PMC5510534 ] [ PubMed : 24022649 ]
  • Weizman ZO, Snow CE. Lexical input as related to children's vocabulary acquisition: Effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. Developmental Psychology. 2001; 37 (2):265–279. [ PubMed : 11269394 ]
  • Wellman HM. Developing a theory of mind. In: Goswami UC, editor. The handbook of childhood cognitive development. 2nd. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell; 2011. pp. 258–284.
  • Wellman HM, Woolley JD. From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition. 1990; 35 (3):245–275. [ PubMed : 2364653 ]
  • Welsh JA, Nix RL, Blair C, Bierman KL, Nelson KE. The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2010; 102 (1):43–53. [ PMC free article : PMC2856933 ] [ PubMed : 20411025 ]
  • West KK, M. BL, A KK. Mother–child attachment and cognitive performance in middle childhood: An examination of mediating mechanisms. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2013; 28 (2):259–270.
  • Whitehurst GJ, Falco FL, Lonigan CJ, F. JE, DeBarshe BD, Valdex-Menchaca MC, Caulfield M. Accelerating language development through picture book reading. Developmental Psychology. 1988; 24 (4):552–559.
  • Wigfield A, Eccles JS, Schiefele U, Roeser RW, Davis-Kean P. Development of achievement motivation. Damon W, Lerner RM, editors. New York: Wiley; Handbook of child psychology. (6th) 2006; 3 :933–1002.
  • Willoughby MT, Kupersmidt J, Voegler-Lee M, Bryant D. Contributions of hot and cool self-regulation to preschool disruptive behavior and academic achievement. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2011; 36 (2):161–180. [ PMC free article : PMC5555639 ] [ PubMed : 21347919 ]
  • Wright RJ. A study of the numerical development of 5-year-olds and 6-year-olds. Educational Studies in Mathematics. 1994; 26 :25–44.
  • Wulfert E, Block JA, Santa Ana E, Rodriguez ML, Colsman M. Delay of gratification: Impulsive choices and problem behaviors in early and late adolescence. Journal of Personality. 2002; 70 (4):533–552. [ PubMed : 12095190 ]
  • Wynn K. Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature. 1992a; 358 (6389):749–750. [ PubMed : 1508269 ]
  • Wynn K. Children's acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cognitive Psychology. 1992b; 24 :220–251.
  • Wynn K, Bloom P, Chiang W-C. Enumeration of collective entities by 5-month-old infants. Cognition. 2002; 83 :B55–B62. [ PubMed : 11934407 ]
  • Xu F, Denison S. Statistical inference and sensitivity to sampling in 11-month-old infants. Cognition. 2009; 112 (1):97–104. [ PubMed : 19435629 ]
  • Yoon JM, Johnson MH, Csibra G. Communication-induced memory biases in preverbal infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008; 105 (36):13690–13695. [ PMC free article : PMC2533251 ] [ PubMed : 18757762 ]
  • Young CB, Wu SS, Menon V. The neuro-developmental basis of math anxiety. Psychological Science Online First. 2012; 23 (5) [ PMC free article : PMC3462591 ] [ PubMed : 22434239 ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Yow WQ, Markman EM. Bilingual children's use of paralinguistic cues to determine emotion in speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2011a; 14 (4):562–569.
  • Yow WQ, Markman EM. Young bilingual children's heightened sensitivity to referential cues. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2011b; 12 (1):12–31.
  • Yuill N. A funny thing happened on the way to the classroom: Jokes, riddles, and metalinguistic awareness in understanding and improving poor comprehension in children. In: Cornoldi C, Oakhill J, editors. Reading comprehension difficulties: Processes and intervention. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1996. pp. 193–220.
  • Zeanah CH. Handbook of infant mental health. New York and London: Guilford Press; 2009.
  • Zelazo PD, Carlson SM. Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives. 2012; 6 (4):354–360.
  • Zelazo PD, Lyons KE. The potential benefits of mindfulness training in early childhood: A developmental social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Child Development Perspectives. 2012; 6 (2):154–160.
  • Zelazo PD, Jacques S, Burack JA, Frye D. The relation between theory of mind and rule use: Evidence from persons with autism-spectrum disorders. Infant and Child Development. 2002; 11 :171–195.
  • Zelazo PD, Müller U, Frye D, Marcovitch S, Argitis G, Boseovski J, Chiang JK, Hongwanishkul D, Schuster BV, Sutherland A, Carlson SM. The development of executive function in early childhood. (Serial No. 274). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2003; 68 (3) [ PubMed : 14723273 ]
  • Zentall SR, Morris BJ. “Good job, you're so smart”: The effects of inconsistency of praise type on young children's motivation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2010; 107 (2):155–163. [ PubMed : 20570281 ]
  • Zero to Three. Diagnostic classification of mental health and developmental disorders of infancy and early childhood. Washington, DC: Zero to Three Press; 2005.
  • Ziegler JC, Goswami U. Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychonomic Bulletin. 2005; 131 :3–29. [ PubMed : 15631549 ]
  • Zimmerman BJ. Achieving academic excellence: A self-regulatory perspective. In: Ferrar M, editor. The pursuit of excellence through education. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2002. pp. 85–110.
  • Zimmerman E, Woolf SH. Understanding the relationship between education and health. 2014. [March 23, 2015]. (Discussion paper). http://www ​.iom.edu/understandingtherelationship .
  • Zins JE, Bloodworth MR, Weissberg RP, Walberg HJ. The scientific base linking social and emotional learning to school success. Journal of Educational and Psychological Consultation. 2007; 17 (2-3):191–210.
  • Zipke M, Ehri LC, Cairns HS. Using semantic ambiguity instruction to improve third graders' metalinguistic awareness and reading comprehension: An experimental study. Reading Research Quarterly. 2009; 44 (3):300–321.
  • Zucker TA, Cabell SQ, Justice LM, Pentimonti JM, Kaderavek JN. The role of frequent, interactive prekindergarten shared reading in the longitudinal development of language and literacy skills. Developmental Psychology. 2013; 49 (8):1425–1439. [ PubMed : 23066674 ]

An ongoing study and forthcoming report of the Institute of Medicine and the National Research Council focuses on research, practice, and policy for young dual language learners. More information about this study can be found at www ​.iom.edu/English-DualLanguageLearners .

The 2012 federal poverty threshold was $23,364 for a family of four with two children, $18,480 for a family of three with one child, $15,825 for a family of two with one child.

  • Cite this Page Committee on the Science of Children Birth to Age 8: Deepening and Broadening the Foundation for Success; Board on Children, Youth, and Families; Institute of Medicine; National Research Council; Allen LR, Kelly BB, editors. Transforming the Workforce for Children Birth Through Age 8: A Unifying Foundation. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US); 2015 Jul 23. 4, Child Development and Early Learning.
  • PDF version of this title (11M)

In this Page

Related information.

  • PMC PubMed Central citations
  • PubMed Links to PubMed

Recent Activity

  • Child Development and Early Learning - Transforming the Workforce for Children B... Child Development and Early Learning - Transforming the Workforce for Children Birth Through Age 8

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

Connect with NLM

National Library of Medicine 8600 Rockville Pike Bethesda, MD 20894

Web Policies FOIA HHS Vulnerability Disclosure

Help Accessibility Careers

statistics

InBrief: The Science of Early Childhood Development

This brief is part of a series that summarizes essential scientific findings from Center publications.

Content in This Guide

Step 1: why is early childhood important.

  • : Brain Hero
  • : The Science of ECD (Video)
  • You Are Here: The Science of ECD (Text)

Step 2: How Does Early Child Development Happen?

  • : 3 Core Concepts in Early Development
  • : 8 Things to Remember about Child Development
  • : InBrief: The Science of Resilience

Step 3: What Can We Do to Support Child Development?

  • : From Best Practices to Breakthrough Impacts
  • : 3 Principles to Improve Outcomes

The science of early brain development can inform investments in early childhood. These basic concepts, established over decades of neuroscience and behavioral research, help illustrate why child development—particularly from birth to five years—is a foundation for a prosperous and sustainable society.

Brains are built over time, from the bottom up.

The basic architecture of the brain is constructed through an ongoing process that begins before birth and continues into adulthood. Early experiences affect the quality of that architecture by establishing either a sturdy or a fragile foundation for all of the learning, health and behavior that follow. In the first few years of life, more than 1 million new neural connections are formed every second . After this period of rapid proliferation, connections are reduced through a process called pruning, so that brain circuits become more efficient. Sensory pathways like those for basic vision and hearing are the first to develop, followed by early language skills and higher cognitive functions. Connections proliferate and prune in a prescribed order, with later, more complex brain circuits built upon earlier, simpler circuits.

In the proliferation and pruning process, simpler neural connections form first, followed by more complex circuits. The timing is genetic, but early experiences determine whether the circuits are strong or weak. Source: C.A. Nelson (2000). Credit: Center on the Developing Child

The interactive influences of genes and experience shape the developing brain.

Scientists now know a major ingredient in this developmental process is the “ serve and return ” relationship between children and their parents and other caregivers in the family or community. Young children naturally reach out for interaction through babbling, facial expressions, and gestures, and adults respond with the same kind of vocalizing and gesturing back at them. In the absence of such responses—or if the responses are unreliable or inappropriate—the brain’s architecture does not form as expected, which can lead to disparities in learning and behavior.

The brain’s capacity for change decreases with age.

The brain is most flexible, or “plastic,” early in life to accommodate a wide range of environments and interactions, but as the maturing brain becomes more specialized to assume more complex functions, it is less capable of reorganizing and adapting to new or unexpected challenges. For example, by the first year, the parts of the brain that differentiate sound are becoming specialized to the language the baby has been exposed to; at the same time, the brain is already starting to lose the ability to recognize different sounds found in other languages. Although the “windows” for language learning and other skills remain open, these brain circuits become increasingly difficult to alter over time. Early plasticity means it’s easier and more effective to influence a baby’s developing brain architecture than to rewire parts of its circuitry in the adult years.

Cognitive, emotional, and social capacities are inextricably intertwined throughout the life course.

The brain is a highly interrelated organ, and its multiple functions operate in a richly coordinated fashion. Emotional well-being and social competence provide a strong foundation for emerging cognitive abilities, and together they are the bricks and mortar that comprise the foundation of human development. The emotional and physical health, social skills, and cognitive-linguistic capacities that emerge in the early years are all important prerequisites for success in school and later in the workplace and community.

Toxic stress damages developing brain architecture, which can lead to lifelong problems in learning, behavior, and physical and mental health.

Scientists now know that chronic, unrelenting stress in early childhood, caused by extreme poverty, repeated abuse, or severe maternal depression, for example, can be toxic to the developing brain. While positive stress (moderate, short-lived physiological responses to uncomfortable experiences) is an important and necessary aspect of healthy development, toxic stress is the strong, unrelieved activation of the body’s stress management system. In the absence of the buffering protection of adult support, toxic stress becomes built into the body by processes that shape the architecture of the developing brain.

Brains subjected to toxic stress have underdeveloped neural connections in areas of the brain most important for successful learning and behavior in school and the workplace. Source: Radley et al (2004); Bock et al (2005). Credit: Center on the Developing Child.

Policy Implications

  • The basic principles of neuroscience indicate that early preventive intervention will be more efficient and produce more favorable outcomes than remediation later in life.
  • A balanced approach to emotional, social, cognitive, and language development will best prepare all children for success in school and later in the workplace and community.
  • Supportive relationships and positive learning experiences begin at home but can also be provided through a range of services with proven effectiveness factors. Babies’ brains require stable, caring, interactive relationships with adults — any way or any place they can be provided will benefit healthy brain development.
  • Science clearly demonstrates that, in situations where toxic stress is likely, intervening as early as possible is critical to achieving the best outcomes. For children experiencing toxic stress, specialized early interventions are needed to target the cause of the stress and protect the child from its consequences.

Suggested citation: Center on the Developing Child (2007). The Science of Early Childhood Development (InBrief). Retrieved from www.developingchild.harvard.edu .

Related Topics: toxic stress , brain architecture , serve and return

Explore related resources.

  • Reports & Working Papers
  • Tools & Guides
  • Presentations
  • Infographics

A mother responds to her baby's babbling

Videos : Serve & Return Interaction Shapes Brain Circuitry

A cover image from the Best Practices to Breakthrough Impacts paper, showing the title and an image of two parents kissing their baby

Reports & Working Papers : From Best Practices to Breakthrough Impacts

The Science of Neglect InBrief

Briefs : InBrief: The Science of Neglect

Black and white photo of a worried baby behind crib bars

Videos : InBrief: The Science of Neglect

Working Paper 12 cover

Reports & Working Papers : The Science of Neglect: The Persistent Absence of Responsive Care Disrupts the Developing Brain

Working Paper 1 cover

Reports & Working Papers : Young Children Develop in an Environment of Relationships

Daycare teacher works with baby who's playing.

Tools & Guides , Briefs : 5 Steps for Brain-Building Serve and Return

children with caregiver/teacher

Briefs : 8 Things to Remember about Child Development

Child riding bike with the words "Mini Parenting Master Class" on the image

Partner Resources : Building Babies’ Brains Through Play: Mini Parenting Master Class

Old-fashioned microphone in front of unfocused black-and-white background Photo by Matt Botsford on Unsplash

Podcasts : About The Brain Architects Podcast

research paper child development

Videos : FIND: Using Science to Coach Caregivers

Serve and return video cover

Videos : How-to: 5 Steps for Brain-Building Serve and Return

Two boys look out a window (Photo by Andrew Seaman on Unsplash)

Briefs : How to Support Children (and Yourself) During the COVID-19 Outbreak

The Science of ECD video still

Videos : InBrief: The Science of Early Childhood Development

The Best Start in Life MOOC logo

Partner Resources , Tools & Guides : MOOC: The Best Start in Life: Early Childhood Development for Sustainable Development

research paper child development

Presentations : Parenting for Brain Development and Prosperity

A child and caregiver wearing goggles and doing a science experiment at a children's museum

Videos : Play in Early Childhood: The Role of Play in Any Setting

research paper child development

Videos : Child Development Core Story

Animated people standing outside (a still from the Science by Design video)

Videos : Science X Design: Three Principles to Improve Outcomes for Children

Young girl wearing face mask receives a vaccination

Podcasts : The Brain Architects Podcast: COVID-19 Special Edition: Self-Care Isn’t Selfish

Gray concrete pillars supporting a structure (Photo by Mirko Blicke on Unsplash)

Podcasts : The Brain Architects Podcast: Serve and Return: Supporting the Foundation

Videos : Three Core Concepts in Early Development

research paper child development

Reports & Working Papers : Three Principles to Improve Outcomes for Children and Families

Photo of woman caregiver holding an baby talking to another caregiver

Partner Resources , Tools & Guides : Training Module: “Talk With Me Baby”

Detail of the first panel of the "What is COVID-19" infographic

Infographics : What Is COVID-19? And How Does It Relate to Child Development?

research paper child development

Partner Resources , Tools & Guides : Vroom

  • Developmental Psychology

Exploring Parenting Styles and Their Impact on Child Development in the Community

  • Jurnal Sosial Sains Terapan dan Riset (Sosateris) 10(2):105-119
  • 10(2):105-119
  • This person is not on ResearchGate, or hasn't claimed this research yet.

Discover the world's research

  • 25+ million members
  • 160+ million publication pages
  • 2.3+ billion citations

Subhadra Evans

  • Ramchandra Keshav Dhongade
  • Preeti Sagar Lad
  • Rahul Kulkarni

Maizura Yasin

  • Child Adolesc Psychiatr Ment Health
  • Norbert Skokauskas

Daniel Fung

  • Lois T. Flaherty
  • Anthony P. S. Guerrero

Zahra Vafaeenejad

  • Laurence Steinberg
  • CHILD YOUTH SERV REV

Miran Lavrič

  • Jenni Menon

Kendall Cotton Bronk

  • Recruit researchers
  • Join for free
  • Login Email Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google Welcome back! Please log in. Email · Hint Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google No account? Sign up
  • Open access
  • Published: 07 October 2016

Analyzing early child development, influential conditions, and future impacts: prospects of a German newborn cohort study

  • Sabine Weinert 1 ,
  • Anja Linberg 2 ,
  • Manja Attig 3 ,
  • Jan-David Freund 1 &
  • Tobias Linberg 3  

International Journal of Child Care and Education Policy volume  10 , Article number:  7 ( 2016 ) Cite this article

28k Accesses

23 Citations

1 Altmetric

Metrics details

The paper provides an overview of a German cohort study of newborns which includes a representative sample of about 3500 infants and their mothers. The aims, challenges, and solutions concerning the large-scale assessment of early child capacities and skills as well as the measurements of learning environments that impact early developmental progress are presented and discussed. First, a brief overview of the German regulations related to early child education and care (ECEC) and parental leave as well as the study design are outlined. Then, the assessments of domain-specific and domain-general cognitive and socio-emotional indicators of early child functioning and development are described and the assessments of structural, orientational, and process quality of the children’s learning environment at home and in child care are presented. Special attention is given to direct assessments and their reliability and validity; in addition, some selected results on social disparities are reported and the prospects of data analyses are discussed.

Early childhood and early child education are an important basis for later development, educational performance, and pathways as well as for lifelong learning and well-being. This important claim has been made repeatedly (Caspi et al. 2003 ; Noble et al. 2007 ), and even critical phases of development have been suggested (e.g., Mayberry et al. 2002 ). Nevertheless and despite the existence of quite a few longitudinal studies addressing this issue, empirical evidence concerning effective conditions, differential child progress, and how the early phases of life impact future development and prospects is still rare.

From an educational and political point of view, it is alarming that various studies have documented profound disparities in child development according to family background when children are merely 3 years of age (Brooks-Gunn and Duncan 1997 ; Dubowy et al. 2008 ; Hart and Risley 1995 , 1999 ; Weinert et al. 2010 ). Even in the first year of life, very early roots of social disparities have been demonstrated which increased substantially over the next few years (Halle et al. 2009 ). In addition, some studies show a high stability of interindividual differences and social disparities from age three onward across preschool (Weinert and Ebert 2013 ; Weinert et al. 2010 ) and school age (Law et al. 2014 ). Notably, the stability of individual differences in children’s test performance has been shown to be even more pronounced in educationally dependent domains of development, like language and factual knowledge, than in more domain-general and less culture-dependent facets of children’s cognitive functioning, as indicated by non-verbal intelligence test scores (Weinert et al. 2010 ).

Drawing on a bioecological model of development (Bronfenbrenner and Morris 2006 ), developmental progress and child education are influenced from early on by the interaction between (developing) child characteristics, skills, and competencies and the quality of structural and process characteristics of the learning environment at the child’s home (Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn 2011 ; Bradley and Corwyn 2002 ; Ebert et al. 2013 ; Weinert et al. 2012 ) as well as in child care (Anders et al. 2013 ). Longitudinal studies shed light on these interactions and how they impact later development and education, which is of great importance for gaining a better understanding of the underlying processes and influential conditions. It is important to note that the form and organization of the various learning environments are affected by state regulations, which differ between countries, resulting in different support systems, offers and regulations for parents from child birth until her/his formal school enrolment (Waldfogel 2001 ).

Regulations in Germany

Maternity leave regulations in Germany prescribe a period of 14 weeks for maternity leave which is divided into two phases: 6 weeks before and 8 weeks after birth. Mothers receive maternity pay from public funds in addition to their employer’s contribution which amounts to 100 % of their former income. After this period, parents are offered various options for taking parental leave until the child’s third birthday. Specifically, parents may interrupt their employment to provide child care and are legally protected from dismissal during this 3-year period; parents also receive parental pay during their parental leave (substitution of income) amounting to two-thirds of her/his prior salary (ranging from € 300.- up to € 1800.-) for a maximum period of 14 months.

Governments also support families through child care policies. The German early child education and care (ECEC) system covers institutional care and education before and alongside elementary and secondary school. Since 1993 children from age of three onward have had a legal right to institutional child care which is primarily organized by local communities and welfare organizations providing care to mainly age-mixed groups at centers with varying opening hours (Linberg et al. 2013 ). However, during the last decade, there has been growing demand for ECEC for children under the age of three that led to the enactment of laws on the demand-driven expansion of child care (“Tagesbetreuungsausbaugesetz TAG”) and the expansion of child care infrastructure for infants and children (“Kinderförderungsgesetz KiföG”) in 2005 and 2008, respectively. Additionally, the legal right to institutional ECEC was expanded in 2013 to include 1-year-old children and political leaders from local, state, and federal levels agreed to provide enough places for 35 % of the children.

Accordingly, the actual use of child care for young children under the age of three has rapidly changed during recent years: Within 8 years (2005–2013), the child care rates for the under 3-year olds increased from 7 to 23 % in the Western states of Germany and from 36 to 47 % in the Eastern states, which have their own distinct tradition and infrastructure concerning early care and education (Kreyenfeld and Krapf 2016 ). In 2015, the nation-wide care rate amounted to 32.9 % with mean values of 28.2 % for the Western and 51.9 % for the Eastern states (Statistisches Bundesamt 2016 ).

However, despite rising rates of early education, a child’s family still is the first and often only environment for developmental processes during the first years of life. Thus, there is a substantial need for analyzing the decision mechanisms as well as the effects of the various options available for early child care.

To summarize, longitudinal studies that provide a basis for analyzing the conditions which significantly contribute to early developmental progress are of great importance for the individual child as well as for society. These studies produce relevant knowledge on how children’s abilities, skills, and competencies develop based on individual resources and conditions; how learning opportunities influence their development in different contexts; how disparities emerge early in life; and how all this impacts educational careers, lifelong learning, well-being, and participation in society.

The German National Educational Panel Study

(NEPS) Footnote 1 has been set up to substantially contribute to these issues (Blossfeld et al. 2011 ). The idea of a multicohort panel study was brought up by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF). A nation-wide interdisciplinary scientific network of researchers was established to develop this idea further and to prepare a proposal for a longitudinal representative large-scale educational study to investigate, monitor, and compare competence development and educational processes in Germany. In light of the specific challenges associated with sampling and measurement of early child characteristics, a newborn cohort study was not initially included in the main NEPS program, but was planned to be conducted as an associated add-on project. However, the study was incorporated into the NEPS study design on behalf of the international evaluation committee organized by the German Research Foundation (DFG) for two main reasons: the growing research on the importance of early child development and education and the rapid changes taking place in early child care, including new social policies being implemented in Germany (see above).

The NEPS is carried out by a network of excellence. It features a longitudinal multicohort sequence design and comprises more than 60,000 target persons as well as 40,000 context persons. In particular, the NEPS design encompasses six longitudinal panel studies conducted simultaneously, which cover a wide range of ages and educational stages. NEPS data are disseminated in a user-friendly way to the scientific community. According to the sensitivity of data, the access is given by a web download, a remote access solution, or on-site in a secure environment. All data are documented in English and are available for use by national and international researchers. In addition to providing substantial analyses of the data themself, it can be used as a benchmark for intervention research, international comparison, and for evaluating issues such as the differences and changes in the use of institutional child care.

At the moment, more than 1100 researchers from more than 700 projects are drawing on the NEPS data already published. The data are used for research in a variety of scientific disciplines and also for educational monitoring—especially, the indicator-based National Report for Education. In order to facilitate access to results for a wide range of professions interested in education—including policy, administration, and practice—scientific papers with important conclusions and empirical evidence are currently summarized by the Leibniz Institute for Educational Trajectories (LIfBi) for public communication and information beyond science and are distributed via the NEPS webpage. Moreover, results are regularly fed back to these groups by presentations and newsletters.

The present paper provides an overview of the NEPS newborn cohort study and its analytic potential. First, the design of the study will be presented with a special emphasis on the aims, challenges, and solutions for the assessment of child characteristics and learning environments. We will then report a few selected results (a) concerning the validity and reliability of the measures used and (b) on early social disparities.

Design of the newborn cohort study of the NEPS: a brief overview

Like all other cohort studies of the NEPS, the cohort study of newborns addresses five research perspectives (Blossfeld et al. 2011 ). Drawing on a theoretical framework, various domain-specific as well as domain-general indicators of early child capacities, characteristics, and developments are assessed as well as measures of structural and process characteristics of their (different) learning environments and their social, occupational, and educational family background. In addition, there is a special focus on families with a migration background, on educational decisions (e.g., concerning child care), and—especially in the newborn cohort study—on patterns of coparenting and child care arrangements. By combining direct observational measures, interview data, and questionnaires, the newborn cohort study allows for in-depth analyses of developmental progress and influential conditions that affect the development of educationally relevant competencies and the stability or changes of interindividual differences. Therefore, it provides insight into the mechanisms through which social disparities emerge, change, and impact children’s future prospects and returns to education.

Sampling strategy

To ensure a representative sample, a two-stage procedure was implemented: 84 German municipalities were used as primary sampling units, explicitly stratified according to three strata of urbanization (via the number of inhabitants; see Aßmann et al. 2015 ). Within these municipalities, addresses were sampled and divided into two birth tranches (infants born between February and April 2012 and between May and June 2012) in order to guarantee a small age range for the infant sample. Starting from a gross sample of about 8500 families, a total of about 3500 families (response rate 41 %) took part in the first assessment wave. In the second wave, the realized sample still included about 2850 families (panel stability 83 %).

Assessment waves and data collection

During the very early phases of child development, three successive assessment waves were carried out when children were on average 7 months (wave 1), 17 months (wave 2), and 26 months of age (wave 3). In the first and third wave video-taped observations and computer-assisted personal interviews (CAPI) were conducted at the family’s home for the entire sample. In the second wave, families were surveyed by computer-assisted telephone interviews (CATI), while video-taped observational measures at the child’s home were only assessed in half of the sample (subsample approx. 1500) in accordance with the study’s design. After wave 3 (i.e., from age two onward) children and their context persons were and will be surveyed every year. Data are collected by trained interviewers. Mothers are the primary respondents, as they can provide valid information about conditions and feelings during and after their pregnancy. Each assessment wave is preceded by a longitudinal pilot study, which runs 1 year before the main study is conducted, to test all instruments and procedures.

Measuring early child characteristics: aims, challenges, and solutions

The assessment of a child’s capacities, characteristics, and early development is pivotal for analyzing the effects of environmental conditions and the impact of early child development and education on later development, educational achievement, career, and life satisfaction or other outcomes and returns. In particular, measuring child characteristics is essential to the modeling of intra-individual progress and changes in interindividual differences, including the emergence of social disparities in various domains of development across childhood. At the same time, it is crucial for analyzing the mechanisms of change, the effects of learning environments and opportunities, and their interactions with the individual capacities and characteristics of the children, while taking the risk or protecting factors of the individual child and his/her environment into account, as well as for controlling for basic interindividual differences if necessary.

However, measuring early child characteristics is a major challenge for longitudinal studies, especially large-scale studies. This is due to various issues and questions, such as which aspects and indicators of early child development should be assessed, how should they be measured, and how can the standardization and validity of measurements be ensured in large-scale assessments of very young children.

Early child development: domain-specific challenges for the child

Developmental psychology has convincingly documented for a long time that neither the development of children nor the development of infants is a homogeneous endeavor. Since the time of Piaget’s ( 1970 ) overarching stage theory of development, it has been empirically demonstrated that development is domain-specific, i.e., demands, prerequisites, effective environmental stimulations differ according to the developmental domain under study (e.g., the acquisition of language, of mathematical competencies, of competencies in natural science, or of an intuitive psychology) (Karmiloff-Smith 1999 ). Even in infancy domain-specific precursors of e.g., mathematical and psychological knowledge and competencies are observable (Goswami 2008 ). Determining how educationally relevant competencies emerge from the interplay of these domain-specific precursors and domain-general basic capacities of the child (like basic reasoning abilities, speed of information processing, or executive functions including cognitive flexibility, inhibition, working memory) on the one hand and of the environmental conditions in the family and in child care on the other is an important issue to be addressed by educational studies. It is important to note that (interindividual differences in) basic capacities also change with age and environmental conditions, although not to the same extent as culture- and education-dependent competencies, and that stimulation of and progress in one developmental domain may enhance, hinder, or compensate for those in other domains.

General NEPS framework for assessing competencies

Within the NEPS, a general framework for assessing educationally relevant abilities and competencies has been developed (Weinert et al. 2011 ). Specifically, the assessments include (a) domain-general cognitive abilities/capacities captured by the constructs of “fluid intelligence” (Cattell 1971 ) or “cognitive mechanics” (Baltes et al. 2006 ); these refer to performance differences in speed of basic cognitive processes, the capacity of working memory, and the ability to apply deductive or analogical thinking in new situations (Brunner et al. 2014 ); (b) domain-specific cognitive competencies, e.g., language competencies, mathematical competencies, and natural science competencies are to be assessed longitudinally and as coherently as possible; and not least (c) meta-competencies, including self-regulation (in the cognitive, behavioral, and emotional domain) and socio-emotional competencies are to be measured (see Weinert et al. 2011 for an elaborated rational of the assessments).

Selecting and measuring relevant and predictive indicators of early child development: a challenge for research

As already mentioned, even in infancy and early childhood, there is no overall indicator for children’s capacities and development. Considering the fact that there are thousands of studies into infant competencies, the indicators have to be carefully selected—not least because of the limited study time and other constraints associated with large-scale assessments, especially those concerning infants and young children who cannot be tested in group settings and whose attentional capacities are still limited. Within the NEPS, the selection draws on the general framework outlined above, including domain-general basic capacities, domain-specific precursors and early roots of language and mathematics as well as indicators of socio-emotional development and early self-regulation.

However, deciding on how to measure these early child characteristics and developments is a major challenge for theoretically sound educational large-scale assessments. Just relying on parents’ reports is problematic since the parents’ judgements might be affected, for example, by their (different) knowledge of child development, by possible restrictions/differences in how they observe the child, and by their particular cultural and individual beliefs and biases. In addition, major aspects of domain-general and domain-specific cognitive functioning and development are not easily observable and need sophisticated assessment methods developed in infancy research.

If newborn cohort studies took direct measures into consideration in addition to interviews and questionnaires, they often relied on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (Bayley 2006 ; Schlesiger et al. 2011 for a brief overview). However, the NEPS feasibility and pilot studies revealed that the standardized administration of test items (using an educationally sound selection of items) turned out to be highly error-prone for trained interviewers who are usually experts in administering interviews but not tests. In addition, the sensorimotor indicators of developmental status measured by the Bayley Scales have been shown to be rather instable across situations (Attig et al. 2015 ) and infancy (McCall et al. 1977 ) and were hardly predictive for later cognitive functioning (e.g., Fagan and Singer 1983 ). Therefore, an indicator of basic information processing abilities was introduced within the NEPS newborn cohort study which has predominantly been used in baby lab studies, namely, the children’s visual attention and speed of habituation within a habituation–dishabituation paradigm. Within this paradigm, the child’s visual attention and the decrease of her/his visual attention when being presented with a series of identical or categorically similar stimuli are used as indicators of the child’s ability to build up a cognitive representation of a stimulus or a stimulus category (Pahnke 2007 ; Sokolov 1990 ). In addition, a new stimulus (or a stimulus from a new category) is presented in the dishabituation phase of the paradigm and a new increase of the child’s visual attention is interpreted as a signal of her/his ability to distinguish stimuli or categories presented during the two phases of the paradigm and to show a preference toward new information. These measures have been shown to be highly predictive of later intelligence scores or other indicators of cognition and language (Bornstein and Sigman 1986 ; Fagan and Singer 1983 ; Kavšek 2004 ). Thus, this paradigm was used to assess early domain-general information processing/categorization abilities; it was also used to measure early precursors of numeracy and word learning (see Table  1 ). To assure standardization and reliability, pictures were presented on a computer screen and the child’s looking behavior (look at/away from the respective stimulus) was video-taped (as were all other direct measures) and coded afterward on a 30 frames per second basis. A third direct indicator of early child characteristics relevant to learning and education is her/his interactional behavior (cognitive, behavioral, and socio-emotional aspects) in mother–child interaction (see “ Assessment of mother–child interaction: direct measurement of the home-learning environment and of the child’s characteristics in mother–child interaction ” section). Table  1 summarizes the measurements of child characteristics and development assessed in the first three waves of the NEPS newborn cohort study.

In addition to direct assessment, mothers were asked (see Table  1 ) about the child’s skills and development as well as about the child’s health. The questions on the child’s skills and development cover items on cognition (e.g., means-end task and object categorization), communicative gesture (e.g., to draw someone’s attention, negation/headshaking), gross and fine motor skills (e.g., climbing up steps, stacking of toy blocks) as well as language (e.g., size of productive vocabulary, comprehension of short instructions). A short version of the Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ-R, Gartstein and Rothbart 2003 ) was used to assess facets of the child’s temperament, specifically orienting/regulatory capacity (items like “if you sing or speak to <target child’s name>, how often does she/he calm down instantly?”) and negative affectivity (items like “when <target child’s name> can’t have what she/he wants, how often does she/he get angry?”) (Bayer et al. 2015 ). In wave 3, a German language checklist and, for bilingual children, an additional Turkish or Russian language checklist (versions of the well-known MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (CDI); Fenson et al. 1993 ) was introduced.

Measuring learning environments: aims, challenges, and solutions

Likewise, measuring learning environments that impact child development is an important challenge for longitudinal large-scale educational studies. As suggested by bioecological theories (Bronfenbrenner and Morris 2006 ), it is not enough to just focus on the home-learning environment; the use and features of non-parental care and other learning environments like parent–child programs, which 55 % of the children in the newborn cohort study experience in their first year of life, should also be assessed. Moreover, it is not sufficient to only measure quantitative structural characteristics, since domain-general and domain-specific qualitative aspects have been shown to be especially important (e.g., Anders et al. 2012 ; Sylva et al. 2006 ); however, indispensable direct observational measurements are hard to obtain in large-scale studies. It is important to note that the meaningfulness of the specific features/aspects assessed for characterizing the different learning environments and the constraints of the measurements have a large impact on the validity of subsequent analyses and conclusions.

General framework of the NEPS

To deal with these issues coherently across cohorts, the measurement of important characteristics of learning environments draws on a general framework which subdivides three different dimensions: Structural quality , which refers to relatively persistent general conditions; orientational quality , like values, norms, and attitudes of an actor; and process quality , which refers to the interaction of the individual with her/his learning environment (Bäumer et al. 2011 ).

Selection and measurement of indicators

For the assessment of the process quality of the home-learning environment as the central learning environment in the very early years, the NEPS newborn cohort study relies on both interviews/questionnaires and direct observations (see below).

In addition, as approx. 24 % of the children of the newborns’ cohort sample were using supplementary non-parental care settings in wave 2, the dimensions specified above were also surveyed in these child care settings using self-administered drop-off questionnaires for center-based ECEC as well as for child minders. Because the NEPS has to rely on survey data, the validity of the quality of non-parental care settings gained from the questionnaire is tested by conducting a sub-study, which compares observational methods with the questionnaire used in the NEPS study. The questionnaire covers structural characteristics as well as process characteristics (see Table  2 for examples).

Besides external day care, the newborn cohort study of the NEPS places a strong emphasis on the home-learning environment—especially in very early childhood—as it is of central importance for later development (NICHD 1998 ). Large-scale longitudinal studies mostly focus on the structural aspects of the home-learning environment to account for variability in infants’ and toddlers’ cognitive and social skills (Halle et al. 2009 ; Hillemeier et al. 2009 ). However, process variables account for additional variance in both social and cognitive child outcomes and may even mediate the effect of structural characteristics (Flöter et al. 2013 ; NICHD 1998 ). Therefore, the assessment of the home-learning environment is not only limited to measuring structural aspects like sociodemographics, but also includes orientations (see Table  3 ); in particular, special emphasis is given to the assessment of processes . Mothers are asked about issues, such as joint activities and their language use at home and the quality of these interactions is also assessed by means of videotaping mother–child interactions during the first three assessment waves (see Table  3 ; “ Assessment of mother–child interaction: direct measurement of the home-learning environment and of the child’s characteristics in mother-child interaction ” section).

Assessment of mother–child interaction: direct measurement of the home-learning environment and of the child’s characteristics in mother–child interaction

On the one hand, the assessment of mother–child interactions as a dyadic process allows a deeper look into maternal interaction behavior as a crucial characteristic of the home-learning environment; on the other hand, it captures additional information about the relevant characteristics of the child.

The quality of maternal interaction behavior has been shown to impact a child’s language (Nozadi et al. 2013 ; Tamis-LeMonda et al. 2001 ), cognitive (NICHD 1998 ; Pearson et al. 2011 ), and socio-emotional development (Bigelow et al. 2010 ; Meins et al. 2001 ). High-quality maternal interaction behavior in very early childhood is mostly described as interaction behavior that provides the child with emotional support in terms of sensitivity, which is defined as a prompt, warm, and contingent reaction to the child’s needs and signals (Ainsworth et al. 1974 ). But stimulating interaction behavior in the sense of scaffolding behavior (Wood 1989 ) is also regarded as high-quality maternal behavior, even in early childhood.

However, maternal interaction behavior cannot be considered separately from the child’s behavior, as interaction is a dyadic process in which both partners’ behavior refers to each other in a reciprocal way. It is well acknowledged that children play an active role in the dyadic interaction process from the very beginning, initiating interactions (van den Bloom and Hoeksma 1994 ) and influencing their occurrence and appearance (Lloyd and Masur 2014 ). Additionally, the child’s temperament (e.g., fear, excitement, protesting, and crying) can become effective in an interaction (Mayer 2013 ).

Accordingly, the NEPS newborn cohort study assesses maternal as well as filial interaction behavior via observation. The mother–child interactions are videotaped in the family home and are rated afterward by trained coders. The interaction itself takes place in a semi-standardized play situation in which the mother and the child play with a standardized toy set (Sommer et al. 2016 ). The play situation is adapted to the different age-related requirements: In the first wave, the mother–child interaction is videotaped for 5 min in which toys from the NEPS toy set are provided. In waves 2 and 3, the mother and child are observed while carrying out a three-bag procedure in which the mother and child played for 10 min with toys from three different bags in a set order (NICHD 2005 ).

Maternal as well as filial interaction behavior is assessed using a macro analytic rating system whereby various interactional characteristics are evaluated on five-point-rating scales with qualitatively specified graduations ([EKIE]; Sommer and Mann 2015 ). The assessment of maternal behavior covers emotional supportive interaction behavior (like sensitivity to distress and non-distress, positive regard for the child, emotionality) and stimulating interaction behavior, including a common rating for language and play stimulation in the first two waves and differentiating language and mathematical stimulation in wave 3 when children were 2 years of age (see Table  3 ). The mother’s intrusiveness, detachment, and negative regard of the child were also rated. The coding of the child’s behavior and emotions focuses on the child’s mood, activity level, social interest in the mother, and sustained attention to objects.

Some selected results

NEPS data are disseminated among the scientific community for analysis and provide an important basis for substantive longitudinal and comparative research. In particular, the various measurements of child characteristics and the detailed measures of the home-learning environment, including the observation of mother–child interactions, enable in-depth analyses to be conducted. In the first section, the results on the reliability and validity of these direct measures and information on the underlying constructs are given, while the second section contains an analysis of early social disparities in the mother’s behavior and child’s development. In addition to using the data from the newborn cohort study (wave 1), Footnote 2 we also draw on the data obtained from the “ViVA project,” Footnote 3 which aims to validate the NEPS measures as one of its objectives.

Reliability and validity of measures of mother–child interaction

Assessing interactions in a large-scale assessment is challenging with regard to validity and reliability of the measurements and ratings. In the NEPS newborn cohort study, these challenges were solved quite successfully: Weighted inter-rater reliability ranged from 84 to 100 % and the ecologic validity of the observed maternal interaction behavior seems to be high, as the data from the ViVA project show that interaction behavior assessed in the semi-structured play situation is comparable to maternal interaction behavior in other situations, i.e., natural feeding and diapering situations (Friedman test comparing differences between interaction situations: χ 2  = 0.74, p  = 0.69; Intra-Class-Correlations of maternal interaction behavior in different situations: ICC  = 0.68, p  < 0.001; n  = 23–30; Vogel et al. 2015 ).

Assessing the quality of the mother’s interaction behavior is a core construct of the home-learning environment in the first waves of the newborn cohort study and focuses on socio-emotional aspects as well as on stimulation. Although the assessed indicators address different aspects of maternal interaction behavior, some of them are related to each other (see Table  4 ). It is worth noting that aspects, like intrusiveness, detachment, or negative regard, are not simply the negative end of the more or less pronounced positive dimensions.

From a theoretical point of view, high-quality interaction behavior includes both sensitivity and stimulation behavior. To test the assumption that a rather broad composite indicator of quality of interaction behavior is not only theoretically but also empirically meaningful, a confirmatory factor analysis was conducted (see Fig.  1 ). Items in the socio-emotional domain ( sensitivity to non - distress , Footnote 4 positive regard , and emotionality ) as well as stimulation loaded substantially on quality of interaction behavior (all standardized coefficients above 0.45). Positive regard (0.69) and stimulation (0.77) contributed the most to this factor. Internal consistency was high (Cronbach's α = 0.80).

figure 1

Results from confirmatory factor analysis for the latent variable Q uality of interaction behavior (Linberg et al. 2016 ). N  = 2190; Chi 2 (2) = 16.05, p  < .000; RMSEA  = 0.06; CFI  = .99; based on all German-speaking mother–child interactions in wave 1

One should note, however, that this broad measure of the quality of interaction behavior is only slightly, albeit significantly, related to other aspects of the home-learning environment which were assessed via the parents interview: This includes issues, like the overall amount of joint activities with the child ( r  = 0.13, p  < 0.000) and special activities (joint picture book reading, r  = 0.13, p  < 0.000; joint construction play, r  = 0.07, p  < 0.000; and talking to the child, r  = 0.07, p  < 0.000).

Reliability and validity of measures of early child characteristics

Given the sample size and household setting, the available data on child characteristics provide a rather detailed insight into the early stages of development, especially with respect to early cognitive capacities and child temperament, which are both measured by multiple indicators. As expected, the first results revealed that these multiple assessment approaches refer to different facets of early child development.

The mother’s report on the child’s temperament deals with the reactions of the child to stressful situations and her/his susceptibility to calming related behavior. In line with previous evidence, this is hardly related to the indicators of child’s temperament, which were assessed in a fairly relaxed mother–child interaction situation ( r  = 0.05, p  < 0.05; Freund and Weinert 2015 ). At the same time, there is evidence supporting the validity and reliability of these measurements. In the ViVA validation study, the information from the questionnaire has been shown to represent the complete subscales of the IBQ-R from which the items were selected ( r  = 0.51 for negative affectivity/0.70 for orienting/regulatory capacity, p  < 0.01; Bayer et al. 2015 ). In addition, it is correlated with the children’s reactions to stress-inducing maternal behavior in a still-face-paradigm where the mother is instructed not to react to her child’s signals ( r  = 0.34–0.43, p  < 0.05; Freund and Weinert 2015 ).

Likewise this can be shown for the assessments of early cognitive capacities/competencies. In the ViVA study, the items on sensorimotor development (assessment of developmental status) were highly correlated with the complete cognition and motor subscales of the Bayley Scales, respectively ( r  = 0.48–0.63, p  < 0.01; Attig et al. 2015 ). Hence the data on sensorimotor development as well as the data on basic information processing abilities (habituation–dishabituation paradigm; 85 % of the videos codable; non-completion of child <1 %; inter-coder reliability in wave 1: κ = 0.91) both rely on scientifically well-established and successfully applied assessments. Nevertheless, they are hardly correlated with each other and thus seem to cover different aspects of early development ( r  = 0.06/0.14, p  < 0.05; Weinert et al. 2016 ).

Although the findings always have to be considered within the context in which the assessments were made (e.g., short version/time), the validity of the various measurements of child characteristics and maternal interaction behavior seems to be apparent.

Early roots of social disparities in child development

The data of the NEPS newborn cohort study allow for an analysis of early social disparities with respect to both early child characteristics and their mother’s interaction behavior. Analyses of data from the first assessment wave when children were 6–8 months of age are in accordance with a bioecological model of child development (Weinert et al. 2016 ). As hypothesized, the mother’s interaction behavior in the video-taped mother–child interaction situation varied significantly according to her educational background. With regard to the broad concept of quality of interaction behavior described above, the mother’s education accounted—even in these early phases of child development—for 4 % ( p  < 0.001) of the variance within the German subgroup of participants. However, as expected we did not find substantial disparities in child characteristics in early childhood, like basic information processing abilities (habituation–dishabituation paradigm), developmental status (sensorimotor scale), or socio-emotional child characteristics coded during mother–child interaction. Interestingly, some early roots of social disparities were observed in child’s characteristics, such as sustained attention to objects and activity level in mother–child interaction. Notably, as predicted, mother–child interaction turned out to be a mutual endeavor: Interactional characteristics of the child (especially the child’s mood, her/his social interest, and continuing sustained attention to objects) and the child’s temperament (orienting/regulatory capacity) accounted for 29 % ( p  < 0.001) of the differences in the overall quality of the mother’s interaction behavior, over and above the control variables (age, sex) and socio-economic conditions (equivalized family income, education of mother, living in partnership) (Weinert et al. 2016 ). Of course, it is still an open question whether the differences observed between children result from former or actual differences in the mother’s behavior or whether the differences in child characteristics and behavior are effective in eliciting their mother’s behavior. In fact, the interrelation between mother and child behavior may vary according to other factors, e.g., additional protective or risk factors (Freund et al. 2016 ). Future findings from the NEPS cohort study of newborns will contribute to explaining how social disparities (suspected at age two and beyond) emerge, how they change over time, which mechanisms contribute to their emergence, and how they impact future development and education.

Prospects and conclusions

Insights and conclusions from longitudinal studies and analyses on the conditions which influence early developmental progress, the emergence of disparities, and their impacts are relevant to educational facilities and social policy and thus to the individual child as well as to society. The present paper focused on the first waves of a large-scale German cohort study of newborns. The various measures will help to better understand the stabilities, changes, and effects of qualitative and quantitative characteristics that early learning environments and other influential conditions have. They also illustrate how the very early outcomes of infant development act as a basis for future development. The child’s development will be measured by testing the development of mathematical, language, and early natural science competencies. Domain-general cognitive abilities will also be assessed (i.e., non-verbal categorization, delay of gratification, verbal memory, and executive functions) along with indicators of socio-emotional development (subscales of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ), Goodman 1997 ), temperament (subscales of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ), Rothbart et al. 2001 ), and personality (BigFive; short version of the Five Factor Questionnaire for Children (FFFK); Asendorpf and van Aken 2003 ). Learning environments will be measured by interviews and questionnaires which draw on the general framework described above and will be supplemented with assessments of different facets of parenting style. To ensure standardization and reduce administration errors, all tests are carried out on tablet computers in child-oriented, playful settings.

It is worth noting that the kindergarten cohort of the NEPS, which started in 2010, also assessed comparable measures from age five onward. Here a sample of about 3000 children (institutional sample from 279 ECEC centers and 720 groups) was included. Despite differences between cohort designs (e.g., individual vs. institutional sample; child assessments at the children’s home vs. in preschool; playful test administration with vs. without tablet computers; CAPI vs. CATI interviews of the parents) the two cohort studies allow for comparisons while at the same time being characterized by partially complementary strengths and weaknesses (e.g., more elaborate information on home-learning environment vs. on institutional characteristics; extensive assessment of early roots vs. extensive assessment of further development). Among other things, this allows for an in-depth analysis of the interrelation between variations as well as an analysis of the constancies and changes in learning environments and child development, and it also relays important information concerning relevant aspects of early education and how it impacts development, educational career, and future prospects.

A better understanding of the relevant factors and conditions influencing early child development and learning together with their impact on children’s future development, educational success, and well-being is of special importance for ECEC policy. Longitudinal studies are needed because they allow analyses of the mechanism and processes of change in these decisive variables. While in cross-sectional studies causal effects cannot be inferred, longitudinal studies—especially those that enable complex group-specific growth-curve modeling and the modeling of intra-individual change—combined with experimental and quasi-experimental comparisons not only contribute significantly to gaining deeper insights into developmental and educational processes and the conditions influencing them but can also answer important questions relevant to ECEC policy such as how does early compared to late entry to institutional care impact later development in various cognitive and non-cognitive domains? Is early institutional care especially valuable (and to what extent) for different subgroups of children/families (e.g., disadvantaged families, children/families with specific risk factors, children with a migration background, refugees, multilingual children, e.g., children learning German as an (early) second or third language)? What are the determinants of the quality of home-learning environment and its effects on child development and education? What are specific risk (or protective) factors and is it possible to compensate for (or to draw on) them?

Obviously, even longitudinal studies will not deliver straightforward conclusions for ECEC policy. However, they provide an important and essential basis for evidence-based policy by informing about relevant conditions of early child education and how they impact later development (e.g., successful future development, educational drawbacks or opportunities in the social, socio-emotional, and cognitive domain). In fact, it has been suggested that high-quality early education is of special importance from a psychological, an educational, a sociological, and an economic perspective and thus is of significant relevance not only to the individual but also to society as a whole (Heckman 2013 ; Sylva et al. 2011 ). NEPS data are especially helpful when it comes to gaining a better understanding of the development of competencies and decisive conditions over the life course—the samples are carefully drawn, the validity of data is high, and longitudinal data are available in a user-friendly form for analyses and even for international comparisons.

From 2008 to 2013, NEPS data were collected as part of the Framework Program for the Promotion of Empirical Educational Research funded by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF). As of 2014, NEPS is carried out by the Leibniz Institute for Educational Trajectories (LIfBi) at the University of Bamberg, Germany, in cooperation with a nation-wide network.

NEPS Starting Cohort Newborns, doi: 10.5157/NEPS:SC1:2.0.0 .

“Video-based Validity Analyses of Measures of Early Childhood Competencies and Home Learning Environment” (ViVA)—project funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG; grant to S. Weinert) within the priority program 1646.

Distress was hardly observed during mother–child interaction.

Ainsworth, M., Bell, S., & Stayton, D. (1974). Infant-mother attachment and social development: Socialization as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In M. Richards (Ed.), The integration of a child into a social world (pp. 99–135). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar  

Anders, Y., Große, C., Roßbach, H. G., Ebert, S., & Weinert, S. (2013). Preschool and primary school influences on the development of children’s early numeracy skills between the ages of 3 and 7 years in Germany. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 24 , 195–211. doi: 10.1080/09243453.2012.749791 .

Article   Google Scholar  

Anders, Y., Roßbach, H. G., Weinert, S., Ebert, S., Kuger, S., Lehrl, S., et al. (2012). Home and preschool learning environments and their relations to the development of early numeracy skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27 , 231–244. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.08.003 .

Asendorpf, J. B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2003). Validity of big five personality judgments in childhood. A 9 year longitudinal study. European Journal of Personality, 17 , 1–17. doi: 10.1002/per.460 .

Aßmann, C., Zinn, S., & Würbach, A. (2015). Sampling and weighting the sample of the early childhood cohort of the National Educational Panel Study (Technical Report of SUF SC1 Version 2.0.0). https://www.neps-data.de/Portals/0/NEPS/Datenzentrum/Forschungsdaten/SC1/2-0-0/SC1-2-0-0_Weighting.pdf .

Attig, M., Freund, J. D., & Weinert, S. (2015). Ein Vergleich der sensomotorischen Skala des Nationalen Bildungspanels mit den Bayley Scales bei 7 bzw. 8 Monate alten Kindern. . Presentation. 22. Tagung der Fachgruppe Entwicklungspsychologie. Frankfurt.

Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to rearing environment depending on dopamine-related genes: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 23 , 39–52. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000635 .

Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (2006). Life span theory in developmental psychology. In W. Damon & M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 569–664). New York: Wiley.

Bäumer, T., Preis, N., Roßbach, H. -G., Stecher, L., & Klieme, E. (2011). Education processes in life-course-specific learning environments. In H. -P. Blossfeld, H. -G. Roßbach, & J. von Maurice (Eds.), Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft: Sonderheft. Special issue. Education as a lifelong process: The German National Educational Panel Study (NEPS) (Vol. 14, pp. 87–101). Wiesbaden: VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften. doi: 10.1007/s11618-011-0183-6 .

Bayer, M., Wohlkinger, F., Freund, J. D., Ditton, H., & Weinert, S. (2015). Temperament bei Kleinkindern: Theoretischer Hintergrund, Operationalisierung im Nationalen Bildungspanel (NEPS) und empirische Befunde aus dem Forschungsprojekt VIVA (NEPS Working Paper No. 58) . Bamberg: Leibniz-Institut für Bildungsverläufe, Nationales Bildungspanel. https://www.neps-data.de/Portals/0/Working%20Papers/WP_LVIII.pdf

Bayley, N. (2006). Bayley scales of infant and toddler development (3rd ed.). San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment.

Bigelow, A. E., MacLean, K., Proctor, J., Myatt, T., Gillis, R., & Power, M. (2010). Maternal sensitivity throughout infancy: continuity and relation to attachment security. Infant Behavior & Development, 33 , 50–60. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.10.009 .

Blossfeld, H. -P., Roßbach, H. -G., von Maurice, J. (2011). Education as a lifelong process: The German National Educational Panel Study (NEPS) [Special issue]. In Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft: Sonderheft, Vol. 14. Wiesbaden: VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften. doi: 10.1007/s11618-011-0198-z .

Bornstein, M., & Sigman, M. (1986). Continuity in mental development from infancy. Child Development, 57 , 251–274. doi: 10.2307/1130581 .

Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53 , 371–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233 .

Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 793–828). New York: Wiley.

Brooks-Gunn, J., & Duncan, G. J. (1997). The effects of poverty on children. Future of Children, 7 , 55–71. doi: 10.2307/1602387 .

Brunner, M., Lang, fr, & Lüdtke, O. (2014). Erfassung der fluiden kognitiven Leistungsfähigkeit über die Lebensspanne im Rahmen der National Educational Panel Study: Expertise (NEPS Working Paper No. 42) . Bamberg: Leibniz-Institut für Bildungsverläufe, Nationales Bildungspanel.

Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Harrington, H., et al. (2003). Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301 (5631), 386–389. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968 .

Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: their structure, growth, and action . Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Dubowy, M., Ebert, S., von Maurice, J., & Weinert, S. (2008). Sprachlich-kognitive Kompetenzen beim Eintritt in den Kindergarten. Ein Vergleich von Kindern mit und ohne Migrationshintergrund. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 40 , 124–134. doi: 10.1026/0049-8637.40.3.124 .

Ebert, S., Lockl, K., Weinert, S., Anders, Y., Kluczniok, K., & Roßbach, H. G. (2013). Internal and external influences on vocabulary development in preschool children. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 24 , 138–154. doi: 10.1080/09243453.2012.749791 .

Fagan, J. F., & Singer, L. T. (1983). Infant recognition memory as a measure of intelligence. Advances in Infancy Research, 2 , 31–78.

Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., et al. (1993). Mac-Arthur communicative development inventories . San Diego: Singular Publishing Group.

Flöter, M., Egert, F., Lee, H. J., & Tietze, W. (2013). Kindliche Bildung und Entwicklung in Abhängigkeit von familiären und außerfamiliären Hintergrundfaktoren. In W. Tietze, F. Becker-Stoll, J. Bensel, A. G. Eckhardt, G. Haug-Schnabel, B. Kalicki, & H. Keller (Eds.), Nationale Untersuchung zur Bildung, Betreuung und Erziehung in der frühen Kindheit (NUBBEK) (pp. 107–137). Kiliansroda: Verlag das Netz.

Freund, J.D., Linberg, A. & Weinert, S. (Forthcoming). Grenzen der Belastbarkeit — Wann ein schwieriges Temperament die Mutter - Kleinkind - Interaktion beeinträchtigt. (working title) .

Freund, J. D., & Weinert, S. (2015). Evaluation der Erfassung frühkindlicher Temperamentsfacetten im Nationalen Bildungspanel (NEPS) über eine 9-Item Version des IBQ-R-VSF . Presentation. 22. Tagung der Fachgruppe Entwicklungspsychologie der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie. Frankfurt.

Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behavior & Development, 26 , 64–86.

Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38 , 581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x .

Goswami, U. (2008). Cognitive development—the learning brain . Hove: Psychology Press.

Halle, T., Forry, N., Hair, E., Perper, K., Wandner, L., Wessel, J., et al. (2009). Disparities in early learning and development: lessons from the early childhood longitudinal study—birth cohort (ECLS-B) . Washington, DC: Child Trends.

Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young American children . Baltimore: Paul H. Brooks Publishing Co.

Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1999). Social world of children learning to talk . Baltimore: Paul H. Brooks Publishing Co.

Heckman, J. J. (2013). Giving kids a fair chance . Cambridge: MIT Press.

Hillemeier, M. M., Farkas, G., Morgan, P. L., Martin, M. A., & Maczuga, S. A. (2009). Disparities in the prevalence of cognitive delay: how early do they appear? Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 23 , 186–198. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2008.01006.x .

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1999). Beyond modularity—a developmental perspective on cognitive science . Cambridge: MIT Press.

Kavšek, M. (2004). Predicting later IQ from infant visual habituation and dishabituation: a meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 25 , 369–393. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2004.04.006 .

Kreyenfeld, M., & Krapf, S. (2016). Soziale Ungleichheit und Kinderbetreuung—Eine Analyse der sozialen und ökonomischen Determinanten der Nutzung von Kindertageseinrichtungen. In R. Becker & W. Lauterbach (Eds.), Bildung als Privileg (pp. 119–144). Wiesbaden: Springer.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Law, J., King, T., & Rush, R. (2014). Newcastle University evidence paper for the read on, get on coalition: An analysis of early years and primary school age language and literacy data from the millennium cohort study . London: Save the Children.

Linberg, T., Bäumer, T., & Roßbach, H. G. (2013). Data on early child education and care learning environments in Germany. International Journal of Child Care and Education Policy, 7 , 24–42. doi: 10.1007/2288-6729-7-1-24 .

Linberg, A., Freund, J.D., Mann, D. Bedingungen sensitiver Mutter-Kind-Interaktionen. In H. Wadepohl, K. Mackowiak, K. Fröhlich-Gildhoff, D. Weltzien (Eds.), Interaktionsgestaltung in Familie und Kindertagesbetreuung. Wiesbaden: VS-Verlag. (in press) .

Lloyd, C. A., & Masur, E. F. (2014). Infant behaviors influence mothers’ provision of responsive and directive behaviors. Infant Behavior & Development, 37 , 276–285. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.04.004 .

Mayberry, R., Lock, E., & Kazmi, H. (2002). Linguistic ability and early language exposure. Nature, 417 , 38. doi: 10.1038/417038a

Mayer, S. (2013). Kindliches Temperament im ersten Lebensjahr und mütterliche Sensitivität. Masterarbeit . Winterthur: Züricher Hochschule für Angewandte Wissenschaften.

McCall, R. B., Eichorn, D. H., Hogarty, P. S., Uzgiris, I. C., & Schaefer, E. S. (1977). Transitions in early mental development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 42 , 1–108. doi: 10.2307/1165992 .

Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2001). Rethinking maternal sensitivity: mothers’ comments on infants’ mental processes predict security of attachment at 12 months. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42 , 637–648. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00759 .

NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1998). Relations between family predictors and child outcomes: are they weaker for children in child care? Developmental Psychology, 34 , 1119–1128.

NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2005). Child care and child development. Results from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development . New York: Guilford.

Noble, K. G., McCandliss, B. D., & Farah, M. J. (2007). Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science, 10 , 464–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00600.x .

Nozadi, S. S., Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Bolnick, R., Eggum-Wilkens, N. D., Smith, C. L., et al. (2013). Prediction of toddlers’ expressive language from maternal sensitivity and toddlers’ anger expressions: a developmental perspective. Infant Behavior & Development, 36 , 650–661. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.06.002 .

Pahnke, J. (2007). Visuelle Habituation und Dishabituation als Maße kognitiver Fähigkeiten im Säuglingsalter . Dissertation. Heidelberg: Ruprecht-Karls-Universität Heidelberg.

Pearson, R. M., Heron, J., Melotti, R., Joinson, C., Stein, A., Ramchandani, P. G., et al. (2011). The association between observed non-verbal maternal responses at 12 months and later infant development at 18 months and IQ at 4 years: a longitudinal study. Infant Behavior & Development, 34 , 525–533. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.07.003 .

Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget’s theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (Vol. I, pp. 703–732). New York: Wiley.

Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: the children’s behavior questionnaire. Child Development, 72 , 1394–1408.

Schlesiger, C., Lorenz, J., Weinert, S., Schneider, T., & Roßbach, H. G. (2011). From birth to early child care. In H. P. Blossfeld, H. G. Roßbach, & J. von Maurice (Eds.), Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft: Special issue. Education as a lifelong process: The German National Educational Panel Study (NEPS) (Vol. 14, pp. 187–202). Wiesbaden: VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften. doi: 10.1007/s11618-011-0186-3 .

Sokolov, Y. N. (1990). The orienting response, and future directions of its development. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 25 , 142–150.

Sommer, A., Hachul, C., & Roßbach, H. G. (2016). Video-based assessment and rating of parent-child-interaction within the National Educational Panel Study. In H. P. Blossfeld, J. von Maurice, M. Bayer, & J. Skopek (Eds.), Methodological issues of longitudinal surveys. The example of the National Educational Panel Study (Vol. 14, pp. 151–167). Wiesbaden: Springer.

Sommer, A., & Mann, D. (2015). Qualität elterlichen Interaktionsverhaltens: Erfassung von Interaktionen mithilfe der Eltern-Kind-Interaktions-Einschätzskala im Nationalen Bildungspanel (NEPS Working Paper No. 56). Bamberg: Leibniz-Institute für Bildungsverläufe, Nationales Bildungspanel. https://www.neps-data.de/Portals/0/Working%20Papers/WP_LVI.pdf .

Statistisches Bundesamt. (2016). Kindertagesbetreuung regional 2015 . Wiesbaden: Statistisches Bundesamt.

Sylva, K., Melhuish, E., Sammons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I., & Taggart, B. (2011). Preschool quality and educational outcomes at age 11: low quality has little benefit. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 9 , 109–124.

Sylva, K., Siraj-Blatchford, I., Taggart, B., Sammons, P., Melhuish, E. C., Elliot, K., et al. (2006). Capturing quality in early childhood through environmental rating scales. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21 , 76–92. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2006.01.003 .

Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72 , 748–767.

van den Bloom, D. C., & Hoeksma, J. B. (1994). The effect of infant irritability on mother-infant interaction: a growth-curve analysis. Developmental Psychology, 30 , 581–590.

Vogel, F., Freund, J. D., & Weinert, S. (2015). Vergleichbarkeit von Interaktionsmaßen über verschiedene Situationen bei Säuglingen: Ergebnisse des Projekts ViVA . Poster. 22. Tagung der Fachgruppe Entwicklungspsychologie der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie. Frankfurt.

Waldfogel, J. (2001). International policies toward parental leave and child care. Future Child, 11 , 98–111.

Weinert, S., Artelt, C., Prenzel, M., Senkbeil, M., Ehmke, T., & Carstensen, C. H. (2011). Development of competencies across the life span. In H. P. Blossfeld, H. G. Roßbach, & J. von Maurice (Eds.), Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft: Special issue. Education as a lifelong process: The German National Educational Panel Study (NEPS) (Vol. 14, pp. 67–86). Wiesbaden: VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften. doi: 10.1007/s11618-011-0182-7 .

Weinert, S., Attig, M. & Roßbach, H.G. (2016). The emergence of social disparities—evidence on early mother–child interaction and infant development from the German National Educational Panel Study (NEPS). In H.P. Blossfeld, N. Kulic, J. Skopek, & M. Triventi (Eds.), Childcare, early education, and social inequality — an international perspective . Cheltenham, Northampton: Edward Elgar Publishing. (in press) .

Weinert, S., & Ebert, S. (2013). Spracherwerb im Vorschulalter: soziale Disparitäten und Einflussvariablen auf den Grammatikerwerb. Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft, 16 , 303–332. doi: 10.1007/s11618-013-0354-8 .

Weinert, S., Ebert, S., & Dubowy, M. (2010). Kompetenzen und soziale Disparitäten im Vorschulalter. Zeitschrift für Grundschulforschung, 1 , 32–45.

Weinert, S., Ebert, S., Lockl, K., & Kuger, S. (2012). Disparitäten im Wortschatzerwerb: Zum Einfluss des Arbeitsgedächtnisses und der Anregungsqualität in Kindergarten und Familie auf den Erwerb lexikalischen Wissens. Unterrichtswissenschaft, 40 , 4–25.

Wood, D. (1989). Social interaction as tutoring. In M. H. Bornstein & J. S. Bruner (Eds.), Crosscurrents in contemporary psychology. Interaction in human development (pp. 59–80). Hillsdale: L. Erlbaum Associates.

Download references

Authors’ contributions

SW conceptualized and drafted the overall manuscript, sequence alignment, and revisions. In addition she cooperatively conceived the design and assessments of the studies, in particular the assessment of early child competencies, and the analyses of social disparities. AL especially drafted the part on the learning environments and the assessment of mother-child interaction; she conducted the data analyses on mother-child interaction and supported the analyses on ecologic validity of mother-child-interaction. MA contributed to the description of the overall design and did the analyses on early roots of social disparities. She is also involved in the conceptualization and coordination of data assessment of the infant cohort study. TL drafted the part on regulations in Germany and contributed to the description of the assessment of learning environments. He is also involved in the conceptualization of the assessment of this data. JDF did the analyses on the reliability and validity of measures of early child characteristics; he drafted this part and cooperatively planned and conducted the validation study. All authors were involved in the sequence alignment and revisions, and approved the final manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Bamberg, 96045, Bamberg, Germany

Sabine Weinert & Jan-David Freund

Department of Early Childhood Education, University of Bamberg, 96045, Bamberg, Germany

Anja Linberg

Leibniz Institute for Educational Trajectories, Wilhelmsplatz 3, 96047, Bamberg, Germany

Manja Attig & Tobias Linberg

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sabine Weinert .

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License ( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Weinert, S., Linberg, A., Attig, M. et al. Analyzing early child development, influential conditions, and future impacts: prospects of a German newborn cohort study. ICEP 10 , 7 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40723-016-0022-6

Download citation

Received : 11 April 2016

Accepted : 27 September 2016

Published : 07 October 2016

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s40723-016-0022-6

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Birth cohort
  • Longitudinal study
  • Early child capacities and development
  • Learning environment
  • Social disparities

research paper child development

  • How It Works
  • PhD thesis writing
  • Master thesis writing
  • Bachelor thesis writing
  • Dissertation writing service
  • Dissertation abstract writing
  • Thesis proposal writing
  • Thesis editing service
  • Thesis proofreading service
  • Thesis formatting service
  • Coursework writing service
  • Research paper writing service
  • Architecture thesis writing
  • Computer science thesis writing
  • Engineering thesis writing
  • History thesis writing
  • MBA thesis writing
  • Nursing dissertation writing
  • Psychology dissertation writing
  • Sociology thesis writing
  • Statistics dissertation writing
  • Buy dissertation online
  • Write my dissertation
  • Cheap thesis
  • Cheap dissertation
  • Custom dissertation
  • Dissertation help
  • Pay for thesis
  • Pay for dissertation
  • Senior thesis
  • Write my thesis

178 Original Child Development Research Paper Topics

child development research paper topics

Child development is represented by all the changes that occur in a child from the time of their birth to adulthood. The changes covered by child development include all the emotional, physical, thought and language changes.

During the process of development, a child transitions from being dependent on his parents to being an independent young adult. It is generally accepted that there are 5 main stages of child development: newborn, infant, toddler, preschool, and school-age.

If you are pursuing a bachelor of science or even a Master of Science degree, you will inevitably have to write at least one research paper about child development. The good news is that writing the paper shouldn’t be too difficult because the Internet contains plenty of information about most aspect of child development.

However, finding the right child development research paper topics for your papers can pose a problem. Don’t worry, we’ve got you covered. We have a list of 178 original topics on this page that should work great in 2023.

How Does A Good Research Paper Look Like?

OK, but what does a good research paper look like? Well, to help you out, we’ve put together a simple outline that shows you exactly what your paper should contain:

Introduction Background information on the topic Thesis statement Body #1 (first major subtopic) Statement + a little background information Supporting evidence Body #2 (second major subtopic) Statement + a little background information Supporting evidence Body #3 (third major subtopic) Statement + a little background information Supporting evidence Conclusion Restate the thesis Summarize the key points Call to action Works Cited/Bibliography Appendix

Of course, to be able to write the research paper as fast as possible, you need to find the best possible topic. Stop wasting your time scouring the Internet and choose one of these original topics:

Best Child Development Topics

We will start our list of topics with the best child development topics (or what we consider to work great in 2023). Check out this list of awesome ideas:

  • Discuss the Piaget theory on child development
  • Research the mechanisms of child development
  • Analyze the Toddler stage (1-3 years)
  • Research motor skills during child development
  • The socio-emotional development of children
  • Discuss the 5 stages of development
  • Talk about cognitive development
  • Research the Preschool stage (3-4 years)
  • How does development shape a person’s life?
  • Research the role of the environment on development
  • Research hands-on experience learning

Easy Child Research Topics

Next on our list we have some of the easiest topics you can find. Our easy child research topics are meant to help you write the paper faster and save time for other activities:

  • Discuss the Erik Erikson theory on child development
  • Effects of parasites on child development
  • A closer look at the Infant stage (3-12 months)
  • Effects of race on child development
  • The intellectual development of children
  • Discuss maternal drug use effects
  • Effects of neglect on the development of children
  • The 5 stages of child development
  • Talk about the Newborn stage
  • Analyze the Pre-school stage (4-5 years)

Child Development Project Topics

Are you looking to start a child development project? You certainly need a great idea if you want an A+. Here are some child development project topics you could try:

  • Research asynchronous development
  • A project on physical growth
  • A project on gender role
  • A project on language and communication
  • Talk about the effects of malnutrition
  • Postnatal depression and child development
  • Weight growth during child development
  • The speed of height growth
  • Analyze the mechanisms of change
  • A project about individual differences
  • Research the development of children with disabilities

Research Topics On Children

If you are looking for interesting research topics on children, you have arrived at the right place. Take a look at these ideas and choose the one you like the most:

  • Discuss the Behavioral theory on child development
  • An in-depth look at secure attachment
  • How do emotions change over time?
  • Discuss the transition from dependent to independent
  • The effect of family on child development
  • The role of movement
  • The effect of school on child development
  • The importance of playing with other children
  • The importance of the surroundings
  • The effects of the Covid-19 pandemic
  • Psychological changes during development
  • Research the effects of classroom environment

Child Development Research Topics

Of course, we have a lot of child development research topics for students of all ages. Here are some of our best, original ideas that should be excellent for 2023:

  • Discuss the Vygotsky theory on child development
  • The role played by genetics
  • Compare and contrast the toddler and the infant
  • Compare and contrast the infant and the preschool child
  • The importance of a psychologist
  • Discuss the role of caregivers
  • Compare and contrast the toddler and the newborn
  • Effects of positive social interactions
  • Research disorganized families
  • Compare and contrast the preschool and the school age child
  • The negative effects of divorce
  • The role of a good early childhood life

Research Paper Topics Children Love

In case you are looking for some research paper topics children love, we have some of the best ideas right here. Check them out and start working on your paper now:

  • Latest news in child development
  • The importance of a good school
  • The importance of eating breakfast
  • The role of the mother
  • The role of the father
  • Teaching sign language to children
  • The effects of bullying on children
  • Discuss the importance of tangible rewards
  • Nurturing good habits
  • Sports in the life of a child

Child Development Topics For High School

So what if you’re a high school student? You can write about child development too. We even have some excellent child development topics for high school student right here:

  • Discuss ADHD and its effects
  • Negative effects of social media on child development
  • The role of technology in child development
  • Research differently-abled children
  • Study the emotional development of children
  • An in-depth look at the role of good nutrition
  • Research the role of sport in child development
  • Research the occurrence of depression
  • The struggles of preschool children
  • Negative effects of smartphones on child development
  • Effects of mass media on school children

Child Development Psychology Topics

Interested in discuss the psychological part of child development? Have a look at our child development psychology topics; you’ll surely find something of interest:

  • The different stages of psychological development
  • What makes children intelligent?
  • Discuss intellectual growth from 3 to 9 years of age
  • What makes children put their health at risk?
  • Research memory in preschool children
  • The problem-solving capabilities of a toddler
  • Language learning skills at the age of 3
  • Do toddlers have a self-preservation instinct?
  • How do toddlers understand the world around them?
  • Discuss a school psychology topic
  • Research how children think

Interesting Child Development Topics For Papers

Our writers have compiled a list of the most interesting child development topics for papers. All you have to do is choose one of our ideas and start working on your research paper:

  • Why is playing so important?
  • Talk about the effects of climate change on child development
  • How to ensure your child develops properly?
  • Discuss the role of social interaction
  • Is social media good for child development?
  • How do children form their ego?
  • The history of child development
  • Are some children more intelligent than most adults?
  • Research the way children understand life
  • Discuss the role of peers on child development

Great Children Research Topics

Are you in search of some great children research topics? You are in luck because we have a long list of such topics right here:

  • Research a child’s social development
  • Discuss speech development
  • Gross motor skills development
  • How to monitor child development effectively?
  • How important is attention?
  • Birth disorders and their negative effects
  • Talk about behavioral child development
  • The importance of music in child development

Child Development Research Paper Questions

Did you know that the best way to get started on your research paper is to look at some child development research paper questions? Here are some for you:

  • How to identify a child genius?
  • How does the community affect children?
  • Can children overcome trauma?
  • How important are birthday celebrations?
  • How important is the mother in child development?
  • How does the loss of hearing affect children?
  • Does the child need to visit a psychologist?
  • How does autism influence child development?
  • Does Facebook negatively affect a child’s development?
  • What is the role of genetics?
  • What defines a toddler?
  • How important is socialization?
  • How much time should a parent spend with his child?
  • What is the sociocultural theory?

Current Child Development Topics

Talking about the latest research in child development will surely get you some bonus points. Here are some current child development topics:

  • Latest advancements in cognitive child development
  • Autistic children development
  • Are video games dangerous for child development?
  • Severe psychological problems
  • Teaching your child a new language
  • Research into deviant behavior
  • Best games for children to play
  • Effects of a poor diet
  • The negative effects of a lack of physical exercise
  • How does technology influence child development?

Advanced Child Psychology Research Topics

If you want to write about complex topics, we also have a list of advanced child psychology research topics. Choose one of them for free right now:

  • The effects of talking on child development
  • Discuss the most important development milestones
  • The effects of the Covid-19 pandemic lockdowns
  • Discuss how television affects child development
  • Research the diseases that can hinder child development
  • Analyze the connection between a toddler and his parents
  • Research child development in single-parent families

Child Development Topics For College

College students will surely appreciate our long list of child development topics for college. Remember, this list is updated periodically so that everyone can get fresh topics:

  • The effects of family violence
  • Innovative child development techniques
  • How much time should you spend with your child?
  • Games that stimulate mental growth
  • Best ways to solve a behavioral issue
  • Negative effects of substance abuse
  • Negative effects of divorce

Controversial Child Development Topics

Don’t worry, your teacher will surely appreciate your courage. You shouldn’t be afraid to talk about controversial topics in your research paper. In fact, here are some topics to help you get started:

  • Books that children should avoid
  • The need for physical punishment
  • How important are schools?
  • Should children be allowed to play video games?
  • The effects of poverty on child development
  • Discuss the effects of social insecurity
  • Child development in the Indian society

Child Mental Health Essay Topics

Are you interested in talking about the mental health of children? No problem, we have a great list of child mental health essay topics for you:

  • Best ways to develop a positive mentality
  • An in-depth look at anxiety in toddlers
  • Discuss the stages of mental development
  • The occurrence of depression in preschool children
  • Discuss the cause and effects of ADHD
  • Can children suffer from PTSD?
  • Research the oppositional defiant disorder

Early Childhood Essay Topics

If you want to cover early childhood in your research paper, you have definitely arrived at the right place. Here are some awesome early childhood essay topics:

  • Tourette syndrome in toddlers
  • Discuss eating disorders in preschool children
  • The effects of social media on young children
  • Sports that children should play
  • Events that can negatively impact a child’s development
  • Ways to ensure maximum mental growth
  • Discuss the 5 stages of development and make a comparison between them
  • Does music affect the mathematical skills of young children?

Topics Related To Child Psychology

Our experts have created a list of interesting topics related to child psychology that you can use for free. Pick one of these ideas and start writing an A+ paper today:

  • Talk about what makes a child intelligent
  • Things that negative affect a child’s psychological wellbeing
  • Stress in young children in the United States
  • Should you child see a psychologist?
  • Sports that curb child delinquency
  • Discuss the effects of watching excessive television
  • The effects of religious orientation on a child’s mental health

ADHD Child Development Essay Topics

Writing about child development of children suffering from the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder is not easy, we know. However, we have some great ADHD Child development essay topics for you:

  • How do ADHD children cope with boring situations?
  • Controlling the activity levels of ADHD children
  • Discuss ways to make ADHD children pay attention
  • ADHD’s effects on the ability to focus
  • Why do ADHD children have learning disabilities?

Get Research Help From The Best

Are you looking for a comprehensive, in depth research paper on an important child development topic, or just need some psychology dissertation help ? Perhaps it’s time to get some research help from our knowledgeable experts and fantastic dissertation writers for hire . All our academic writers have a Master’s or PhD degree and are native English speakers (ENL writers). This means they can be trusted to write college and university level research papers for any class – no matter the degree you are pursuing.

We offer affordable academic writing services online to any student in need, anywhere in the world. We can assure you that our custom papers will be written from scratch and will be 100 percent original (plagiarism report provided for free). In addition, we can edit and proofread your paper to make sure it is 100% accurate and free of any typos or spelling/grammar errors.

Mental Health Research Topics

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Comment * Error message

Name * Error message

Email * Error message

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

As Putin continues killing civilians, bombing kindergartens, and threatening WWIII, Ukraine fights for the world's peaceful future.

Ukraine Live Updates

  • Open access
  • Published: 20 August 2024

Psychosocial interventions for improving the physical health of young people and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a scoping review

  • John Headley Ward   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-9108-7900 1 , 2 , 3 , 4 ,
  • Audrey McBride   ORCID: orcid.org/0009-0003-9000-6667 1 ,
  • Anna Price   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-9147-1876 1 &
  • Tamsin Newlove Delgado   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-5192-3724 1  

BMC Psychiatry volume  24 , Article number:  569 ( 2024 ) Cite this article

2 Altmetric

Metrics details

Young people and adults with ADHD are at risk of a range of physical health problems. There is limited guidance on how to approach health problems in ADHD, and especially around 16-25 year olds who will be transitioning from paediatric to adult care. The aim of this scoping review was to identify psychosocial interventions that target physical health in young people and adults with ADHD.

We constructed searches in MEDLINE, PsycInfo, EMBASE of adolescents, young people and adults. Inclusion criteria were; studies of psychosocial interventions examining a component of physical health, applicable to people aged 16-25, with clinical or research diagnoses of ADHD. Data were extracted using a data extraction tool and tabulated, including study intervention framing/aims, population, intervention, and relevant outcomes (including specific statistics where relevant).

Our search identified 22 unique papers covering, psychosocial interventions targeting at least one of sleep ( n= 7), smoking ( n= 3), substance/alcohol use ( n= 4), physical health/exercise ( n= 6) and general health ( n= 3). Studies examined psychotherapy/behaviour interventions ( n= 12), psychoeducation ( n= 4), digital ( n= 2) and social interventions ( n= 4). There was significant heterogeneity in intervention framing, outcome measures and population.

Further work on the impact of targeted physical health interventions, with explicit reference to a conceptual framework of poor health in ADHD is required. Furthermore, future work standardising reporting of physical health outcomes in ADHD is crucial for the development of an evidence base in this field.

Peer Review reports

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental condition characterised by combinations of hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention, thought to affect 5.6% of 12-18 year olds and 2.58% of adults [ 1 , 2 ]. Whilst traditionally thought of as a disorder of childhood, with a typical onset before the age of 12, it is now understood that symptoms can persist into adulthood and have a significant impact on many aspects of life, including physical health [ 3 , 4 , 5 , 6 ].

In respect to the links in research between ADHD and physical health, firstly there is a wealth of literature on the association between ADHD and higher rates of health risk behaviours, including smoking, alcohol abuse, substance misuse, risk-taking behaviour, self-harm, obesity and sleep disorders [ 6 , 7 , 8 , 9 , 10 , 11 ]. These findings are reproduced in studies using various methods including traits-based approaches, mendelian randomisation, case-control and longitudinal follow-up studies.

Secondly, there is a growing body of evidence demonstrating links between ADHD and non-communicable diseases. A large genetically informed Swedish registry study found that participants with ADHD were at higher risk for 34 out the 35 conditions studied compared to those without ADHD, including nervous system and respiratory disorders [ 12 ]. Other studies have also demonstrated high rates of neurological and respiratory disease, as well as gastrointestinal disorders and cardiovascular disease [ 13 , 14 , 15 ]. Furthermore, Stickley and colleagues [ 15 ] demonstrated that multimorbidity was predictive of whether study participants had ADHD. In respect to mortality, several longitudinal studies have noted increased mortality rates amongst people with ADHD. Whilst these appear to be driven by accidental and unnatural deaths, the cause remains contested [ 6 , 16 , 17 , 18 ].

There have been various attempts to explain the inequalities in physical health outcomes for this population. It has previously been suggested that people with ADHD may be less likely to follow government recommendations on health promotion, even when controlling for socioeconomic status [ 19 ]. This is echoed in work by Cherkasova et al , who reported that the persistence of ADHD symptoms into adulthood mediated poorer functional outcomes [ 6 ]. However, the large sibling analysis study of DuRietz et al highlights the importance of genetic risk factors in the association between ADHD and physical health, supported by their finding that shared genetic factors explained 60-69% of the relationship between ADHD and respiratory, musculoskeletal and metabolic disorders in their sample [ 20 ].

There have been previous studies suggesting that some of the health risks in ADHD may be mitigated by appropriate treatment of ADHD using medication (e.g., meta-analyses demonstrating the efficacy of medication in improving sleep or substance misuse [ 7 , 21 , 22 ]). In addition to medication, psychosocial interventions are likely to be important in the prevention and mitigation of health risks in ADHD, when provided as part of a holistic approach. Importantly, psychosocial interventions can also constitute health promotion and support health autonomy, which may be of particular significance to young adults transitioning to adult care [ 23 , 24 , 25 , 26 , 27 ]. There is a small and heterogenous body of research examining the efficacy of psychosocial interventions in the management of physical health problems associated with ADHD [ 28 , 29 , 30 , 31 , 32 , 33 ]. However, this has not yet been synthesised to identify the nature and extent of existing research or indicate targets for future research and intervention development. This is a significant evidence gap given the poorer physical health of people with ADHD, which adversely affects quality of life and economic, social and health outcomes [ 34 , 35 , 36 , 37 ].

This scoping review aims to identify and describe existing psychosocial interventions for physical health in young people and adults with ADHD, including those in preliminary stages (e.g. feasibility trials).

Given the lack of previous reviews in this field, and the need to provide a broad overview of available evidence, a scoping review was chosen to identify psychosocial interventions addressing physical health in ADHD. Scoping reviews are suitable for identifying research gaps, summarising research findings, clarifying concepts, and making recommendations for future research [ 38 ]. This scoping review aimed to identify relevant literature using an inclusive approach incorporating different methodologies and reflecting varying levels of quality [ 39 ].

The review followed a five-stage process as described by Arksey and O'Malley: identifying the question, identifying relevant studies, study selection, charting the data and collating, summarising and reporting the results [ 40 , 41 ]. We found no previous scoping reviews or systematic reviews examining this topic. We have reported our scoping review according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Scoping Review Extension checklist (see supplementary materials) [ 42 ]. We did not pre-register this review protocol.

Eligibility criteria

Studies were eligible for inclusion in the review if they met four key eligibility criteria: a population aged 16 years or older (or including substantial representation of this group), diagnosed with ADHD (either clinically diagnosed, self-reported, or using a standardised diagnostic measure) (population and context), the introduction of a form of structured psychosocial intervention within an experimental trial (concept) and the measurement of a physical health outcome (outcome).

In respect to defining the population of interest, we sought interventions that would be relevant to young people and adults, of a transition care age group (16-25 year olds). This age bracket is important for two reasons. Firstly, it is well-understood that ADHD care begins to decline for older adolescents, as medication adherence and service access decline [ 25 , 26 ]. Secondly, the transition to adulthood is when adolescents begin to take responsibility for their own health and health behaviour, and therefore support around independent health management would be timely [ 23 , 24 , 27 ].

To meet the scoping review’s aim of identifying all interventions relevant to the population, we included studies of under 16s, and some studies of adults over 25 years old. This was the case where the proposed intervention had methods or findings that were clearly applicable to 16–25-year-olds. ‘Applicable’ in this context was defined as interventions that could be utilised in our population of interest without need for modification of the intervention itself. Given that this was a scoping review, we judged this was an appropriately inclusive approach to exploring a limited literature, with similar expanded definitions of age categories having been used in previous scoping reviews [ 43 , 44 , 45 ]. We did however exclude school-based interventions, (e.g., those which use a school or classroom-based approach in childhood) [ 46 ]. This was because our team considered that interventions set within classroom environments would not be replicable in young adults. Where inclusion criteria were borderline, decisions were made on a case-by-case basis and discussed by at least two reviewers (TND and JW).

For the purposes of this review, psychosocial interventions were defined as structured interventions that adopt a psychological, educational, or social framework. This definition was adapted from Ruddy and House, adapting the definition to include digital health interventions, which have taken prominence since the publication of their definition in 2005 [ 47 , 48 , 49 ]. In this review, we included search terms pertaining to psychotherapeutic interventions, behavioural interventions, digital interventions, peer/support groups, exercise-based interventions, and psychoeducational interventions.

Study outcomes were checked during the screening process to identify physical health outcomes. We defined physical health broadly as a chronic physical health problem and/or a current behaviour that confers a long-term physical health risk (e.g., unprotected sex, smoking). This definition was formed in collaboration with the Managing Young People with ADHD in Primary Care (MAP) study academic team and research advisory group (RAG) which was composed of people with lived experience [ 50 ]. Physical health outcomes included scales (e.g., sleep indices, health-related quality of life), objective measures (e.g., reductions in alcohol consumption, abstinence) and subjective measures (e.g., sleep diaries). Studies were excluded if there was no clear physical health outcome recorded in either the published paper or its supplementary materials. We deliberately did not pre-define specific physical health concerns in our search. This was because, in conjunction with our MAP study advisory team, we felt this would be a more appropriate approach for an initial exploration aimed at capturing the breadth of health problems being addressed. In pre-defining health problems, we considered there would be a risk of excluding studies which incidentally notes physical health outcomes.

Studies from the grey literature were excluded, as well as studies not written in the English language, due to resource limitations. We also excluded non peer-reviewed scientific literature (e.g. dissertations, preprints, conference proceedings). We did not exclude studies where participants received a biological therapy (e.g. medication, bright light therapy), if they also received a psychosocial intervention. This approach enabled a broad spread of relevant included studies and met the review’s objective of identifying feasible interventions.

Search strategy

The search strategy was developed with information specialists at the at the University of Exeter. Searches were performed using MEDLINE (1946 onwards), Embase (1974 onwards ) and PsycInfo (1803 onwards ), via the Ovid © platform.

Searching took place in two phases, to ensure adequate inclusion of young people/youths and adults (Fig. 1 ). In the first phase, we used our young people/youth search terms and our adult terms (search dates 7 th October 2022 and 24 th November 2022 respectively). The process of search design through the completion of title and abstract screening was from September 2022 to December 2022, with full-text inclusions from this search decided by January 2023.

figure 1

Flow diagram of database searches, title/abstract screening and full text screening

In order to ensure that we included publications relating to applicable interventions that were tested in a younger population (i.e. 13-17 year olds), a second phase included a search using adolescent search terms, conducted in February 2024, with title/abstract and full text-screening screening completed in March 2024. This search included literature published up until February 2024. Full details of all searches can be seen in supplementary materials.

ADHD search terms were adapted from the search terms used by the National Institute for Care and Health Excellence (NICE) in their development of ADHD guidance [ 51 ]. Search terms for psychosocial interventions were adapted from the search terms used by NICE in the development of their guidance for lower back pain and diabetes [ 52 , 53 ]. These were adapted as appropriate for our search and are available in supplementary materials.

Study selection

The study selection process is highlighted in Figure 1 . The first set of searches (young people/adults, October-November 2022) yielded 5258 results, and the second (adolescents, February 2024) 4293.

These studies were then screened by title and abstract independently by two reviewers (JW, TND), against inclusion criteria, with disagreements resolved by discussion. Where disagreements were not resolved, this was taken to the wider study team and MAP study principal investigator (PI) (AP). Citation chasing was also performed by examining references from relevant review and protocol studies to identify any further studies not found in our search.

This left 61 records for full-text screening. These were again double-screened (JW, TND), with disagreements resolved by discussion, and where necessary the wider study team, and principal investigator (AP), leaving 22 articles which were included at full-text screening. Reasons for full-text exclusions are given in Figure 1 .

Data extraction and charting

Studies were collated using a data extraction form and shared spreadsheet in which the data could be recorded. This data extraction tool was piloted on five papers initially, reviewed and then used for all papers. These were single entered by researchers (JW, AM), with all entries were re-reviewed after first entry (JW). The data extraction tool can be found in supplementary materials.

The data extraction process used narrative synthesis to collate information on what the intervention was targeting (and rationale), what outcomes were measured and how these were measured. This involved extracting study details (i.e., title, author), study design (comparator, methodology), population characteristics (age, gender, location of study), intervention framing of study (i.e. rationale for intervention chosen), ADHD definition (e.g., meeting DSM-V criteria, clinically diagnosed ADHD), relevant additional inclusion/exclusion criteria, primary and secondary outcomes of the studies and measures used, physical health findings, attendance reporting and ADHD symptom findings. Where studies had noted qualitative feedback on the interventions used, this was also collated and charted under outcomes.

Data were then charted in tabular form, by physical health problem addressed, with the extracted details on the studies provided alongside this.

Twenty-two studies met our inclusion criteria (Table 1 ). Physical health outcomes targeted or reported included sleep (seven studies), smoking (three studies), substance misuse (four studies), physical activity and/or weight (six studies) and more general/broad physical health outcomes (three studies). It should be noted that one study (Bjork et al .) covered two health outcomes (smoking, physical activity) [ 54 ]. Studies covered an overall age range of 11-65. Of these, eleven studies examined adolescents, ten studies examined adults (although four did not provide a precise age range), and one study examined both (14–30-year-olds) [ 55 ]. Only one included study had participants exclusively between 16-25, but this was a college-based study [ 56 ].

In respect to ADHD concept, 14/22 studies were based on clinical diagnosis, whilst 6/22 were criteria based, 1/22 used self-reported ADHD and 1/22 used ‘documented’ ADHD (Table 1 ). Where studies reported gender ( n= 16), 10 studies were at least 50% female, ranging from a 32% to 83% male sample. 14/22 studies included a physical health outcome as a primary outcome or target, whilst 5/22 studies included physical health as a secondary outcome [ 55 , 56 , 60 , 61 , 74 , 75 ]. 12/22 of the studies were of a psychotherapeutic or behavioural intervention, 4/22 of psychoeducation, 4/22 social/exercise interventions and 2/22 digital interventions. Most of the studies were conducted in the USA (8/22), followed by Sweden (5/22), the Netherlands (2/22) and Germany (2/22), with the others located in Brazil, Norway, Belgium, Denmark and China.

In respect to methodology, 13/22 studies were randomised controlled trials (RCTs) [ 56 , 63 , 66 , 67 , 68 , 70 , 75 , 76 , 77 ], 6/22 studies were single group (before and after) comparisons [ 54 , 57 , 59 , 60 , 69 , 73 ], one study was secondary analysis of RCT data [ 58 ], one study compared a single-group pre/post intervention with a previous study data [ 74 ], and finally one study compared two groups before and after comparison (not RCT) [ 64 ]. Apart from one study which was unfunded [ 73 ], and one study which did not clarify its funding [ 54 ], 20/22 studies were funded from non-commercial sources.

Sleep was examined as a health outcome in seven included studies, as described below in Table 2 [ 55 , 57 , 58 , 59 , 60 , 61 , 77 ]. All used different interventions, which can be broadly divided into psychoeducational interventions and psychotherapeutic interventions (e.g., adapted cognitive or dialectical behavioural therapy). One study examined primarily bright light therapy, however used psychoeducation around sleep in their methodology for both active arms and the placebo arm (hence its inclusion). Only one study included people with diagnosed sleep problems and ADHD, with the others including either a general ADHD population or those with self-reported sleep problems. Four of the studies included are completed or ongoing RCTs, with the rest being pilot/feasibility studies or single group intervention studies. Included studies had small sample sizes (three had fewer than 20 participants, only one had a sample size greater than 100) and covered a wide age range (13-63). Three studies suggested a rationale for their intervention within ADHD in relation to health needs; ADHD symptoms/executive dysfunction impacting on habits and sleep hygiene as the mechanism of sleep problems in ADHD [ 58 , 59 , 62 ] and delayed circadian rhythm/preference [ 58 , 59 ]. Whilst Becker et al referenced problems of adolescence in sleep, they did not explain the rationale within ADHD [ 57 ]. Van Andel et al (RCT (melatonin versus placebo versus melatonin and bright light therapy, where all arms received psychoeducation) did not find improvements in sleep for any group [ 58 ]. Meyer et al (RCT) also did not find sustained improvements in sleep for either their behavioural or control (psychoeducation group) [ 55 ]. However, the single group intervention and pilot studies did find evidence supporting behavioural and psychosocial interventions, including pilot feasibility data from Keuppens et al RCT [ 77 ]. Results also demonstrated tolerability and feasibility of these sleep interventions in ADHD; all completed studies noted good attendance at and compliance with interventions, whilst Becker et al. , Jernelov et al . and Keuppens et al. noted participants’ satisfaction with interventions. The subjective positive feedback received in Becker et al. included increased responsibility for health, working with a therapist and increased knowledge [ 57 ]. In Jernelov et al ., feedback received was the use of routines and structure [ 59 ]. For Keuppens et al. , thematic analysis generated themes for adolescents around having more control and independence around sleep, and that both parents and adolescents had better understanding of the impact of ADHD on sleep [ 77 ].

Smoking was examined as a health outcome in three studies, as presented in Table 3 [ 54 , 63 , 64 ]. These studies had an average sample size of 45 and covered both adolescents and adults. One of the studies was a randomised controlled trial (RCT), whilst the other two were a single-group intervention study and an ADHD vs non-ADHD single intervention study, neither with control groups. Two of the studies used psychoeducation, including components about smoking, whilst one of the studies used monetary incentives to encourage participants to stop smoking. In respect to mechanisms, two of the three studies suggested a rationale for their choice of intervention; targeting executive dysfunction in ADHD that may perpetuate smoking [ 54 , 64 ] and mental health difficulties in ADHD precipitating poor health behaviour [ 54 ]. The results were variable. The two studies examining tobacco use (Kollins et al ., Bjork et al .—non-randomised group trials) found no interventions with sustained effects, reporting that participants largely went back to smoking (irrespective of ADHD). Corona et al.’s study (also an RCT) found that the attitudes of participants towards substance misuse changed significantly following specific work around tobacco, however they did not examine tobacco use directly. Bjork et al and Corona et al both noted that participants generally adhered well to the intervention [ 54 , 63 ]. In respect to specific positives of interventions, Björk et al. cite peer support dynamics in their group [ 54 ].

Substance misuse

Outcomes related to alcohol and substance misuse were examined in four studies, as seen in Table 4 [ 65 , 66 , 67 , 68 ]. These all had relatively larger sample sizes (range=70-303) and were RCTs. They all included participants who had diagnosable substance misuse disorders, rather than subclinical problematic substance use, (in contrast to the studies of sleep). Two of the studies examined cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) paradigms, whilst one study evaluated at motivational interviewing (MI) and behavioural action and one used both CBT/MI. Two studies identified a rationale for their choice of intervention; untreated ADHD symptoms being associated with poorer outcomes in substance use disorder [ 67 , 68 ], and the challenges of people with ADHD within a college environment putting them at greater risk for long-term substance misuse [ 66 ]. All four studies (RCTs) reported significant improvements in measured outcomes with a psychotherapeutic intervention, including Riggs et al. and Thurstone et al. which found that behavioural therapy and medication had comparable effects in the treatment of substance use disorder in patients with ADHD [ 67 , 68 ].

Physical activity/weight

Studies reporting physical activity or weight outcomes were much more heterogenous in their design, including two RCT protocols, two completed RCTs and two single group interventions (Table 5 ). Studies in this category had generally small sample sizes (n<50), except Lindvall et al ( N= 120) [ 71 ] and comprised a younger adult demographic (11-30). Only two studies provided a rationale for intervention explicitly highlighting health in ADHD (both referencing poor health behaviour in ADHD) [ 54 , 71 ]. All involved promoting physical activity, through structured exercise classes, wearable technology/social media and psychoeducation respectively. Furthermore, Schoenfelder et al. report qualitative feedback that the intervention increased awareness of activity levels and ADHD symptoms [ 69 ]. Both RCTs (Silva et al, Converse et al [ 56 , 72 ]) reported improved physical functioning (Converse et al using a questionnaire, Silva et al using objective biometrics), as did both single group intervention studies (Schoenfelder et al finding an increase in step count, Bjork et al in weekly physical activity [ 54 , 69 ]).

Unspecified physical health outcomes

Three studies examined unspecified physical health outcomes related to quality of life and did not fit well into other categories (Table 6 ) [ 73 , 74 , 75 ]. Enggaard et al. reported a study of adolescents with a comorbid physical health disorder, examining guided self-determination as a way of improving their engagement in physical healthcare, given the association of ADHD with physical comorbidity. They found that guided self-determination was effective in improving patients’ self-confidence in managing their conditions, and that adolescents were positively engaged in creating the self-management strategies. The second study examined effects of medication versus cognitive behavioural therapy in 124 young adults on core ADHD symptoms and secondarily recorded improvements in physical health as part of questions on the World Health Organisation (WHO) quality of life scale [ 78 ]. This study [ 74 ] did not identify a rationale for their intervention’s impact physical health. Thirdly, Geissler et al developed a modular treatment programme for adolescents with continual ADHD-related impairment (under routine care). Their rationale was the breadth of functional impairment faced by adolescents with ADHD, and the lack of related interventions. Their developed RCT protocol includes a health-related quality of life questionnaire as a secondary outcome, and their intervention includes a module on substance use [ 75 ].

This scoping review aimed to identify psychosocial interventions that have been designed for physical health problems in ADHD, and which physical health problems they target. We found 22 studies of interventions which measured at least one physical health outcome, with 16 specifically targeting physical health outcomes. In the other studies, measures of physical health (including sleep quality or health-related quality of life measures) were included as secondary outcomes of interventions primarily targeting reduction of core ADHD symptoms [ 55 , 56 , 60 , 61 , 74 , 75 ]. Included studies were grouped under five categories, dependent on the outcomes explored. These were sleep, smoking, substance misuse, physical activity/weight, and general health outcomes, utilising psychoeducational, behavioural and social paradigms (Table 1 ).

The main finding from this scoping review is a relative paucity of research into interventions targeting physical health outcomes in ADHD, and furthermore the lack of larger programmes of research aiming to address the health problems identified. Generally, the included studies lacked detail on the framing and theoretical basis both of individual health problems (who is affected, and how that health problem is quantified) as well as of health problems in ADHD in general (the 'mechanism' of ill health targeted by such interventions). Fortunately, there is some similarity amongst the identified literature explored in respect to the psychosocial interventions used and identified positive aspects of interventions, which may form a basis for the development of a more coherent evidence base in this field.

Framing of health problems

There is substantial heterogeneity in this literature in respect to how health problems were defined and measured, with variable inclusion criteria and outcome measures between studies. For example, when exploring sleep, some studies examined those with formal sleep diagnoses [ 58 ], whilst others specifically excluded those with diagnosed sleep problems [ 59 , 62 ]. In relation to outcome measures, smoking was conceptualised in a different way by each of the studies included (attitudes to tobacco, carbon monoxide levels and self-reported reductions). Some studies used standardised quality of life measures, such as the WHOQL-BREF, SF-36, and KIDSCREEN-10 [ 56 , 74 , 75 ]. However, quality of life measures are of limited utility in assessing physical health outcomes, often being too broad and multi-factorial. We also note a recent scoping review finding that the SF-36 is frequently erroneously reported as a global measure of quality of life, which although Converse and colleagues did not do, highlights a wider problem with the misapplication of quality-of-life measures [ 79 ]. The heterogeneity of included studies’ outcome measures highlights the need for consensus in respect to measures used in assessing the physical health outcomes of populations with ADHD. It was interesting that three of the six studies that did not explicitly target a health problem used sleep outcome measures in behavioural interventions [ 55 , 60 , 61 ], which may highlight the importance of sleep to young people and families, and the impact poor sleep has on symptoms and functioning [ 80 , 81 ].

Such extensive variability in inclusion criteria and outcome measurements limits both the clinical and academic applicability of studies’ findings. This variability is likely to result from the absence of a common framework that mechanistically relates ADHD and physical health outcomes.

Whilst there was some commonality in intervention modalities (e.g., behavioural interventions, educational interventions), authors tended not to explain clearly which mechanism within ADHD their health intervention was targeting, beyond a select few [ 54 , 57 , 58 , 59 , 62 , 64 , 66 , 71 , 73 ]. Challenges surrounding poor conceptual framework of mechanisms of ADHD in relation to health are alluded to in the discussions of some of the included studies [ 54 , 58 , 59 , 64 , 66 , 67 , 73 ]. As mentioned in the introduction, the ‘causal pathway’ of increased health risk in ADHD is likely to be complex and multifactorial, however a sound understanding and explicit logic model is an important basis for the development of preventative interventions in this population. Findings from this scoping review suggest that that clearer framing of the problem is required to properly develop interventions, through better definition of health problems with inputs from existing research and stakeholder perspectives (which may explain the currently disjointed view of this field) [ 82 ].

Positive aspects of interventions

Psychoeducation was common amongst health interventions studied, with 4/22 solely examining a psychoeducational intervention, and Meyer et al comparing a behavioural intervention with psychoeducation as control [ 55 ]. These studies tended to recruit younger participants who were ‘at risk of’ particular health problems, with only Bjork et al using psychoeducation in an adult context. This raises questions about where future research work should focus, primary or secondary prevention in young people and adults with ADHD.

‘Self-efficacy’ or independence over one’s health was also a concept referenced explicitly in the qualitative feedback from participants included in several of the studies we reviewed [ 57 , 73 , 77 ]. Furthermore, all the interventions in included studies all required commitment to interventions and required people to actively engage in their own care, the importance of which has been studied previously in patients with chronic conditions [ 83 , 84 , 85 ]. Self-efficacy is widely cited as being important in ADHD management [ 86 , 87 ]. This is explicitly highlighted by Enggaard et al. , who demonstrated that their guided self-determination intervention promoted efficacy and strategy formation amongst patients with ADHD [ 73 ].

Regular and consistent interventions (regular sessions, commitment to a regimen), were explicitly highlighted in the qualitative participant feedback on several interventions [ 59 , 69 ]. This is particularly pertinent in ADHD, where difficulty with day-to-day structure and organisation is something that people highlight as a contributor to health and social outcome inequalities [ 88 , 89 , 90 ].

Peer dynamics were also referenced by several papers [ 54 , 56 ]. Bjork et al. reported that participants found the peer support dynamic of such interventions useful, whilst Converse et al. reported that participants from an earlier survey used in the development of their intervention would have preferred a mixed ADHD/non-ADHD group [ 91 ]. From a brief review of the literature, the perspectives on peer support in adult ADHD have not yet been formally studied but could be looked at in future work. It should be noted that, in discussions about ADHD in online spaces, community and identity appear to be important themes in living with ADHD [ 87 , 92 ].

If the literature in this field were more coherent, it would make it easier to explore facets of interventions in this field more rigorously, using methods such as intervention component analysis. This would be especially interesting given the findings of Meyer et al, which suggest comparable effects between psychoeducation and behavioural intervention [ 55 ].

Strengths and limitations

This study addresses a novel research question in the literature and our search strategy identified papers in line with research aims. By not defining physical health in our search strategy, we were able to identify a broad range of interventions, targeting for example sleep, smoking, alcohol/substance misuse, physical activity, weight, and physical comorbidity.

Limitations of our scoping review include challenges around defining population age range. Of the 22 studies included, only one study explicitly fell within the 16-25 age range [ 56 ]. Whilst it may have been preferable to strictly apply the lower age limit of 16 years, doing so would have risked losing studies with applicability to our 16-25 age group (e.g., Schoenfelder and colleague’s study of digital health, many of the sleep behavioural interventions) [ 57 , 61 , 69 ]. The same would apply to the upper age limit, where studies such as Björk et al would be excluded if a strict age basis was applied (despite this study having clear relevance to our question) [ 54 ]. Therefore, we adopted a pragmatic approach, informed by consultation with MAP study colleagues and our RAG. We accept that the interventions would likely have different effects than those reported by the studies, if they were to be repeated in a strictly 16-25 age group. This would be an important subject of future work, supported by framework development.

By defining ‘types’ of interventions we were interested in for our search (psychotherapeutic, behavioural, technological, support groups, exercise-based, psychoeducational) we may have inadvertently precluded the inclusion of other interventions in the field. However, to deliver the review with the resources available, and following consultation with our RAG, it was decided to prioritise an open approach to defining physical health problems, which came at the cost of being more restrictive in terms of types of intervention reviewed. As this research area matures, and concepts related to ADHD and physical health become more clearly defined, it will become easier to conduct evidence syntheses of literature on this topic.

Furthermore, it was notable that there were limited interventions surrounding established chronic physical disorders targeted at adults with ADHD, given the known associations of ADHD with chronic health problems [ 12 , 14 , 93 ]. However, this is likely because our search filters examining psychosocial interventions would not have been inclusive of tailored medical interventions (e.g. if a study were examining supporting people with ADHD and diabetes in their medication compliance). A focussed review of chronic disease management in ADHD in adults would be useful in exploring this important area.

This scoping review set out to identify existing psychosocial interventions for physical health in ADHD, with a focus on interventions applicable to a transition care age range (16-25 year olds). Findings demonstrate that whilst such interventions have been developed and reported, the small evidence base surrounding them limits their current application. Future work in this field needs to focus on the development of a conceptual framework for the origins of the physical health challenges and linked health inequalities we see in ADHD. Alongside this, more research is needed into creating standardising how health outcomes are measured and reported in ADHD research in this field, such that evidence can be better synthesised and ultimately realised into clinical applications.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are available via the MEDLINE ( https://www.nlm.nih.gov/medline/medline_overview.html ), APA PsycInfo ( https://www.apa.org/pubs/databases/psycinfo ) and EMBASE ( https://www.embase.com/ ) databases.

Abbreviations

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

Cognitive Behavioural Therapy

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fifth Edition

Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia Present and Lifetime

Mapping ADHD services in Primary Care Study

National Institute of Health and Care Excellence

Research Advisory Group

Randomised Control Trial

36-item Short Form Survey

World Health Organisation

World Health Organisation Quality of Life (Brief) Scale

Song P, Zha M, Yang Q, Zhang Y, Li X, Rudan I. The prevalence of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A global systematic review and meta-analysis. J Glob Health. 2021;11:1–9.

Article   Google Scholar  

Salari N, Ghasemi H, Abdoli N, Rahmani A, Shiri MH, Hashemian AH, Akbari H, Mohammadi M. The global prevalence of ADHD in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ital J Pediatr. 2023;49:1–12.

Biederman J, Petty CR, Woodworth KY, Lomedico A, Hyder LL, Faraone S, v. Adult Outcome of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Controlled 16-Year Follow-Up Study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2012;73:577.

Arnold LE, Hodgkins P, Kahle J, Madhoo M, Kewley G. Long-Term Outcomes of ADHD: Academic Achievement and Performance. J Atten Disord. 2020;24:73–85.

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:593–602.

Cherkasova MV, Roy A, Molina BSG, et al. Review: Adult Outcome as Seen Through Controlled Prospective Follow-up Studies of Children With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Followed Into Adulthood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2022;61:378–91.

Taubin D, Wilson JC, Wilens TE. ADHD and Substance Use Disorders in Young People: Considerations for Evaluation, Diagnosis, and Pharmacotherapy. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2022;31:515–30.

Ward JH, Curran S. Self-harm as the first presentation of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adolescents. Child Adolesc Ment Health. 2021;26:303–9.

Ricketts EJ, Sturm A, McMakin DL, McGuire JF, Tan PZ, Smalberg FB, McCracken JT, Colwell CS, Piacentini J. Changes in Sleep Problems Across Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Treatment: Findings from the Multimodal Treatment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Study. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2018;28:690–8.

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Leppert B, Riglin L, Wootton RE, Dardani C, Thapar A, Staley JR, Tilling K, Davey Smith G, Thapar A, Stergiakouli E. The Effect of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder on Physical Health Outcomes: A 2-Sample Mendelian Randomization Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2021;190:1047.

Brook JS, Balka EB, Zhang C, Brook DW. Longitudinal Smoking Patterns: Do They Predict Symptoms of ADHD in Adults? J Atten Disord. 2020;24:86–93.

Du Rietz E, Brikell I, Butwicka A, et al. Mapping phenotypic and aetiological associations between ADHD and physical conditions in adulthood in Sweden: a genetically informed register study. Lancet Psychiatry. 2021;8:774–83.

Semeijn EJ, Kooij † J J Sandra, Comijs HC, Michielsen M, Dorly †, Deeg JH, Beekman ATF,. Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Physical Health, and Lifestyle in Older Adults. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.12261 .

Pan PY, Bölte S (2020) The association between ADHD and physical health: a co-twin control study. Scientific Reports 2020 10:1 10:1–13.

Stickley A, Koyanagi A, Takahashi H, Ruchkin V, Inoue Y, Kamio Y. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and physical multimorbidity: A population-based study. Eur Psychiatry. 2017;45:227–34.

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

London AS, Landes SD. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and adult mortality. Prev Med (Baltim). 2016;90:8–10.

Schiavone N, Virta M, Leppämäki S, Launes J, Vanninen R, Tuulio-Henriksson A, Järvinen I, Lehto E, Michelsson K, Hokkanen L. Mortality in individuals with childhood ADHD or subthreshold symptoms – a prospective perinatal risk cohort study over 40 years. BMC Psychiatry. 2022;22:1–10.

Klein RG, Mannuzza S, Ramos Olazagasti MA, Roizen E, Hutchison JA, Lashua EC, Castellanos FX. Clinical and Functional Outcome of Childhood Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder 33 Years Later. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:1295–303.

Loewen OK, Maximova K, Ekwaru JP, Ohinmaa A, Veugelers PJ. Adherence to Life-Style Recommendations and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Population-Based Study of Children Aged 10 to 11 Years. Psychosom Med. 2020;82:305–15.

Du Rietz E, Barker AR, Michelini G, Rommel A-S, Vainieri I, Asherson P, Kuntsi J. Beneficial effects of acute high-intensity exercise on electrophysiological indices of attention processes in young adult men. Behavioural brain research. 2019;359:474–84.

Kidwell KM, van Dyk TR, Lundahl A, Nelson TD. Stimulant Medications and Sleep for Youth With ADHD: A Meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2015;136:1144–53.

Zulauf CA, Sprich SE, Safren SA, Wilens TE. The complicated relationship between attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and substance use disorders. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2014;16:436.

Singh SP, Tuomainen H. Transition from child to adult mental health services: needs, barriers, experiences and new models of care. World Psychiatry. 2015;14:358.

Schuiteman S, Chua KP, Plegue MA, Ilyas O, Chang T. Self-Management of Health Care Among Youth: Implications for Policies on Transitions of Care. J Adolesc Health. 2020;66:616.

Eke H, Ford T, Newlove-Delgado T, Price A, Young S, Ani C, Sayal K, Lynn RM, Paul M, Janssens A. Transition between child and adult services for young people with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): findings from a British national surveillance study. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2020;217:616–22.

Young S, Adamou M, Asherson P, et al. Recommendations for the transition of patients with ADHD from child to adult healthcare services: A consensus statement from the UK adult ADHD network. BMC Psychiatry. 2016;16:1–10.

Rigby E, Hagell A, Davis M, Gleeson H, Mathews G, Turner G. Getting health services right for 16–25 year-olds. Arch Dis Child. 2021;106:9–13.

Waldron HB, Turner CW (2008) Evidence-Based Psychosocial Treatments for Adolescent Substance Abuse. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410701820133 37:238–261.

Alfonso JP, Caracuel A, Delgado-Pastor LC, Verdejo-García A. Combined Goal Management Training and Mindfulness meditation improve executive functions and decision-making performance in abstinent polysubstance abusers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;117:78–81.

Pagoto SL, Curtin C, Bandini LG, Anderson SE, Schneider KL, Bodenlos JS, Ma Y (2013) Weight loss following a clinic-based weight loss program among adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. Eating and Weight Disorders - Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity 2010 15:3 15:e166–e172.

Hiscock H, Sciberras E, Mensah F, Gerner B, Efron D, Khano S, Oberklaid F. Impact of a behavioural sleep intervention on symptoms and sleep in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and parental mental health: randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1136/BMJ.H68 .

Sciberras E, Fulton M, Efron D, Oberklaid F, Hiscock H. Managing sleep problems in school aged children with ADHD: a pilot randomised controlled trial. Sleep Med. 2011;12:932–5.

Keshavarzi Z, Bajoghli H, Mohamadi MR, Salmanian M, Kirov R, Gerber M, Holsboer-Trachsler E, Brand S. In a randomized case-control trial with 10-years olds suffering from attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) sleep and psychological functioning improved during a 12-week sleep-training program. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry. 2014;15:609–19.

Libutzki B, Ludwig S, May M, Jacobsen RH, Reif A, Hartman CA. Direct medical costs of ADHD and its comorbid conditions on basis of a claims data analysis. European Psychiatry. 2019;58:38–44.

Kittel-Schneider S, Arteaga-Henriquez G, Vasquez AA, et al. Non-mental diseases associated with ADHD across the lifespan: Fidgety Philipp and Pippi Longstocking at risk of multimorbidity? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2022;132:1157–80.

Harpin VA. The effect of ADHD on the life of an individual, their family, and community from preschool to adult life. Arch Dis Child. 2005;90:i2–7.

Harpin V, Mazzone L, Raynaud JP, Kahle J, Hodgkins P. Long-Term Outcomes of ADHD: A Systematic Review of Self-Esteem and Social Function. J Atten Disord. 2016;20:295–305.

Munn Z, Peters MDJ, Stern C, Tufanaru C, McArthur A, Aromataris E. Systematic review or scoping review? Guidance for authors when choosing between a systematic or scoping review approach. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2018;18:1–7.

Peterson J, Pearce PF, Ferguson LA, Langford CA. Understanding scoping reviews: Definition, purpose, and process. J Am Assoc Nurse Pract. 2017;29:12–6.

Arksey H, O’Malley L (2007) Scoping studies: towards a methodological framework. https://doi.org/10.1080/1364557032000119616 8:19–32.

Tricco AC, Lillie E, Zarin W, et al. PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): Checklist and explanation. Ann Intern Med. 2018;169:467–73.

PRISMA. http://www.prisma-statement.org/Extensions/ScopingReviews . Accessed 3 Nov 2023.

Mitchell DL, Shlobin NA, Winterhalter E, Lam SK, Raskin JS. Gaps in transitional care to adulthood for patients with cerebral palsy: a systematic review. Child’s Nervous System. 2023;39:3083–101.

Nesbitt AE, Sabiston CM, deJonge ML, Barbic SP, Kozloff N, Nalder EJ (2023) A scoping review of resilience among transition-age youth with serious mental illness: tensions, knowledge gaps, and future directions. BMC Psychiatry 2023 23:1 23:1–27.

McCrory A, Best P, Maddock A. The relationship between highly visual social media and young people’s mental health: A scoping review. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2020;115: 105053.

(2018) School-based interventions. The Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health. https://doi.org/10.13056/ACAMH.1088 .

Guan Lim C, Lim-Ashworth NSJ, Fung DSS. Updates in technology-based interventions for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2020;33:577.

Lakes KD, Cibrian FL, Schuck SEB, Nelson M, Hayes GR. Digital health interventions for youth with ADHD: A mapping review. Computers in Human Behavior Reports. 2022;6: 100174.

Ruddy R, House A. Psychosocial interventions for conversion disorder. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2005. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD005331.PUB2/MEDIA/CDSR/CD005331/REL0002/CD005331/IMAGE_N/NCD005331-CMP-003-05.PNG .

Price A, Smith JR, Mughal F, Salimi A, Melendez-Torres GJ, Newlove-Delgado T. Protocol for the mixed methods, Managing young people (aged 16–25) with Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Primary care (MAP) study: mapping current practice and co-producing guidance to improve healthcare in an underserved population. BMJ Open. 2023;13: e068184.

National Institute for Care and Health Excellence (2021) Search strategy | How this topic was developed | Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder | CKS | NICE. https://cks.nice.org.uk/topics/attention-deficit-hyperactivity-disorder/how-this-topic-was-developed/search-strategy/ . Accessed 30 Sep 2022.

National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (2022) Search strategy | How this topic was developed | Back pain - low (without radiculopathy) | CKS | NICE. In: National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. https://cks.nice.org.uk/topics/back-pain-low-without-radiculopathy/how-this-topic-was-developed/search-strategy/ . Accessed 30 Sep 2022.

National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (2017) Type 2 diabetes: prevention in people at high risk [A] Evidence reviews for interventions for people at high risk of type 2 diabetes.

Björk A, Rönngren Y, Wall E, Vinberg S, Hellzen O, Olofsson N (2020) A nurse-led lifestyle intervention for adult persons with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in Sweden. https://doi.org/10.1080/0803948820201771768 74:602–612.

Meyer J, Ramklint M, Hallerbäck MU, Lööf M, Isaksson J. Evaluation of a structured skills training group for adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a randomised controlled trial. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2022;31:1–13.

Converse AK, Barrett BP, Chewning BA, Wayne PM. Tai Chi training for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A feasibility trial in college students. Complement Ther Med. 2020;53: 102538.

Becker SP, Duraccio KM, Sidol CA, Fershtman CEM, Byars KC, Harvey AG. Impact of a Behavioral Sleep Intervention in Adolescents With ADHD: Feasibility, Acceptability, and Preliminary Effectiveness From a Pilot Open Trial. J Atten Disord. 2022;26:1051–66.

van Andel E, Bijlenga D, Vogel SWN, Beekman ATF, Kooij JJS. Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Delayed Sleep Phase Syndrome in Adults: A Randomized Clinical Trial on the Effects of Chronotherapy on Sleep. J Biol Rhythms. 2022;37:673–89.

Jernelov S, Larsson Y, Llenas M, Nasri B, Kaldo V. Effects and clinical feasibility of a behavioral treatment for sleep problems in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a pragmatic within-group pilot evaluation. BMC Psychiatry. 2019;19:226.

Morgensterns E, Alfredsson J, Hirvikoski T. Structured skills training for adults with ADHD in an outpatient psychiatric context: an open feasibility trial. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2016;8:101–11.

Nøvik TS, Haugan ALJ, Lydersen S, Thomsen PH, Young S, Sund AM. Cognitive-behavioural group therapy for adolescents with ADHD: study protocol for a randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open. 2020;10: e032839.

Keuppens L, Marten F, Baeyens D, Boyer B, Danckaerts M, Van Der Oord S. Protocol: Sleep IntervEntion as Symptom Treatment for ADHD (SIESTA)-Blended CBT sleep intervention to improve sleep, ADHD symptoms and related problems in adolescents with ADHD: Protocol for a randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open. 2023;13:65355.

Corona R, Dvorsky MR, Romo S, Parks AM, Bourchtein E, Smith ZR, Avila M, Langberg J. Integrating Tobacco Prevention Skills into an Evidence-Based Intervention for Adolescents with ADHD: Results from a Pilot Efficacy Randomized Controlled Trial. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2020;48:1439–53.

Kollins SH, McClernon FJ, Van Voorhees EE. Monetary Incentives Promote Smoking Abstinence in Adults With Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2010;18:221.

van Emmerik-van Oortmerssen K, Vedel E, Kramer FJ, Blankers M, Dekker JJM, van den Brink W, Schoevers RA. Integrated cognitive behavioral therapy for ADHD in adult substance use disorder patients: Results of a randomized clinical trial. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2019;197:28–36.

Meinzer MC, Oddo LE, Vasko JM, Murphy JG, Iwamoto D, Lejuez CW, Chronis-Tuscano A. Motivational interviewing plus behavioral activation for alcohol misuse in college students with ADHD. Psychol Addict Behav. 2021;35:803–16.

Riggs PD, Winhusen T, Davies RD, et al. Randomized controlled trial of osmotic-release methylphenidate with cognitive-behavioral therapy in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and substance use disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50:903–14.

Thurstone C, Riggs PD, Salomonsen-Sautel S, Mikulich-Gilbertson SK. Randomized, controlled trial of atomoxetine for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adolescents with substance use disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:573–82.

PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Schoenfelder E, Moreno M, Wilner M, Whitlock KB, Mendoza JA. Piloting a mobile health intervention to increase physical activity for adolescents with ADHD. Prev Med Rep. 2017;6:210–3.

Mayer JS, Hees K, Medda J, et al. Bright light therapy versus physical exercise to prevent co-morbid depression and obesity in adolescents and young adults with attention-deficit / hyperactivity disorder: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials. 2018;19:140.

Lindvall MA, Holmqvist KL, Svedell LA, Philipson A, Cao Y, Msghina M. START – physical exercise and person-centred cognitive skills training as treatment for adult ADHD: protocol for a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry. 2023;23:1–14.

Da Silva LA, Doyenart R, Henrique Salvan P, Rodrigues W, Felipe Lopes J, Gomes K, Thirupathi A, De Pinho RA, Silveira PC. Swimming training improves mental health parameters, cognition and motor coordination in children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Int J Environ Health Res. 2020;30:584–92.

Enggaard H, Laugesen B, DeJonckheere M, Fetters MD, Dalgaard MK, Lauritsen MB, Zoffmann V, Jørgensen R. Impact of the Guided Self-Determination Intervention among Adolescents with Co-Existing ADHD and Medical Disorder: A Mixed Methods Study. Issues Ment Health Nurs. 2021;42:1–12.

Mei-Rong P, Huang F, Zhao M-J, Wang Y-F, Wang Y-F, Qian Q-J. A comparison of efficacy between cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and CBT combined with medication in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Psychiatry Res. 2019;279:23–33.

Geissler J, Jans T, Banaschewski T, et al. Individualised short-term therapy for adolescents impaired by attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder despite previous routine care treatment (ESCAadol)-Study protocol of a randomised controlled trial within the consortium ESCAlife. Trials. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1186/S13063-018-2635-2 .

van Emmerik-van Oortmerssen K, Blankers M, Vedel E, Kramer F, Goudriaan AE, van den Brink W, Schoevers RA. Prediction of drop-out and outcome in integrated cognitive behavioral therapy for ADHD and SUD: Results from a randomized clinical trial. Addictive Behaviors. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106228 .

Keuppens L, Marten F, Baeyens D, Boyer BE, Danckaerts M, van der Oord S. A Pilot Study of a Cognitive-Behavioral Sleep Intervention Specifically for Adolescents With ADHD and Sleep Problems: A Qualitative and Quantitative Evaluation. Cogn Behav Pract. 2024;31:367–82.

Skevington SM, Lotfy M, O’Connell KA. The World Health Organization’s WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment: psychometric properties and results of the international field trial. A report from the WHOQOL group. Qual Life Res. 2004;13:299–310.

Lins L, Carvalho FM. SF-36 total score as a single measure of health-related quality of life: Scoping review. SAGE Open Med. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1177/2050312116671725 .

Arias-Mera C, Paillama-Raimán D, Lucero-González N, Leiva-Bianchi M, Avello-Sáez D. Relation between sleep disorders and attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Res Dev Disabil. 2023. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.RIDD.2023.104500 .

Hvolby A. Associations of sleep disturbance with ADHD: implications for treatment. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2015;7:1–18.

O’Cathain A, Croot L, Duncan E, Rousseau N, Sworn K, Turner KM, Yardley L, Hoddinott P. Guidance on how to develop complex interventions to improve health and healthcare. BMJ Open. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1136/BMJOPEN-2019-029954 .

Rees S, Williams A. Promoting and supporting self-management for adults living in the community with physical chronic illness: A systematic review of the effectiveness and meaningfulness of the patient-practitioner encounter. JBI Libr Syst Rev. 2009;7:492–582.

PubMed   Google Scholar  

Aboumatar H, Pitts S, Sharma R, Das A, Smith BM, Day J, Holzhauer K, Yang S, Bass EB, Bennett WL. Patient engagement strategies for adults with chronic conditions: an evidence map. Syst Rev. 2022. https://doi.org/10.1186/S13643-021-01873-5 .

Smith BM, Sharma R, Das A, Aboumatar H, Pitts SI, Day J, Holzhauer K, Bass E, Bennett WL. Patient and family engagement strategies for children and adolescents with chronic diseases: A review of systematic reviews. Patient Educ Couns. 2021;104:2213–23.

Wenderlich AM, Baldwin CD, Fagnano M, Jones M, Halterman J. Responsibility for Asthma Management among Adolescents with and without ADHD. J Adolesc Health. 2019;65:812.

Gajaria A, Yeung E, Goodale T, Charach A. Beliefs about attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and response to stereotypes: youth postings in Facebook groups. J Adolesc Health. 2011;49:15–20.

Knox E, Muros JJ. Association of lifestyle behaviours with self-esteem through health-related quality of life in Spanish adolescents. Eur J Pediatr. 2017;176:621.

Schrevel SJC, Dedding C, van Aken JA, Broerse JEW. ‘Do I need to become someone else?’ A qualitative exploratory study into the experiences and needs of adults with ADHD. Health Expectations. 2016;19:39–48.

Ginapp CM, Macdonald-Gagnon G, Angarita GA, Bold KW, Potenza MN. The lived experiences of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A rapid review of qualitative evidence. Front Psychiatry. 2022;13:1853.

Sulzer SH, Trueba C, Converse AK. The appeal of tai chi and complementary therapies for college students with ADHD. J Am Coll Health. 2023. https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2021.1990071 .

Yeung A, Ng E, Abi-Jaoude E. TikTok and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Cross-Sectional Study of Social Media Content Quality. Can J Psychiatry. 2022;67:899–906.

Landes SD, London AS (2018) Self-Reported ADHD and Adult Health in the United States. 25:3–13. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054718757648 .

Download references

Acknowledgements

we would like to express our thanks to our public patient involvement group and research advisory group, who facilitated our approach to this important research topic. We would also like to thank Reviewer 2, whose detailed and thoughtful comments have greatly strengthened the content and form of this manuscript.

AP is funded by a National Institute for Health and Care Research (NIHR) Three Research Schools’ Mental Health Research Fellowship (located within the Exeter NIHR School for Primary Care Research (SPCR); Grant Reference Number MHF008). Tamsin Newlove-Delgado was funded by a NIHR Advanced Fellowship (NIHR300056) whilst undertaking this work. The views expressed in this publication are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR, NHS or the UK Department of Health and Social Care.

JW is supported by the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre (BRC)

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

2.05 South Cloisters, 2.05 South Cloisters, St Luke’s Campus, University of Exeter Medical School, Heavitree Road, Exeter, EX2 4TH, UK

John Headley Ward, Audrey McBride, Anna Price & Tamsin Newlove Delgado

Royal Devon University NHS Foundation Trust, Exeter, UK

John Headley Ward

Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK

Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

JW was responsible for hypothesis generation, design of literature search, title/abstract screening, data extraction, analysis and manuscript preparation. AM was responsible for data extraction and manuscript preparation. AP was responsible for design of literature search and manuscript preparation, as well as project oversight. TND was responsible for title/abstract screening and manuscript preparation, as well as project oversight.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John Headley Ward .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate.

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Supplementary material 1, supplementary material 2, supplementary material 3, rights and permissions.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Ward, J.H., McBride, A., Price, A. et al. Psychosocial interventions for improving the physical health of young people and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a scoping review. BMC Psychiatry 24 , 569 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-024-06009-2

Download citation

Received : 12 September 2023

Accepted : 08 August 2024

Published : 20 August 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-024-06009-2

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Preventative medicine
  • Psychosocial intervention

BMC Psychiatry

ISSN: 1471-244X

research paper child development

  • DOI: 10.36948/ijfmr.2024.v06i04.24920
  • Corpus ID: 271449179

A Study of Moral Development of Children and Good Habit Formation on Indian Child Rearing Practices Context

  • Bhavna Ramaiya
  • Published in International Journal For… 20 July 2024
  • Education, Sociology

7 References

Related papers.

Showing 1 through 3 of 0 Related Papers

IMAGES

  1. 178 Current Child Development Research Paper Topics For 2023

    research paper child development

  2. ⇉Child Development Research Paper: Prenatal development Essay Example

    research paper child development

  3. ⇉Child Development Research Paper "Child Development" Essay Example

    research paper child development

  4. Research paper on child development behavior

    research paper child development

  5. ⇉Child Development Research Paper Child DevelopmentBabies Essay Example

    research paper child development

  6. 📗 Child Development Observation Paper Examples

    research paper child development

COMMENTS

  1. A STUDY ON CHILDHOOD DEVELOPMENT IN EARLY STAGE

    This is a descriptive research paper, where secondary information produced by different . ... Child development is a crucial aspect of human growth, encompassing various physical, intellectual ...

  2. Parenting and Child Development: A Relational Health Perspective

    Abstract. A child's development is embedded within a complex system of relationships. Among the many relationships that influence children's growth and development, perhaps the most influential is the one that exists between parent and child. Recognition of the critical importance of early parent-child relationship quality for children's ...

  3. Child Development

    Child Development, the flagship journal of the Society for Research in Child Development, has published articles, essays, reviews, and tutorials on various topics in the field of Child Development for almost 100 years. We have a wide readership including researchers, theoreticians, child psychiatrists, clinical psychologists, psychiatric social workers, specialists in early childhood education ...

  4. Child Development

    As the flagship journal of the Society for Research in Child Development, Child Development has published articles, essays, reviews, and tutorials on various topics in the field of child development since 1930. Spanning many disciplines, the journal provides the latest research, not only for researchers and theoreticians, but also for child psychiatrists, clinical psychologists, psychiatric ...

  5. Journal of Early Childhood Research: Sage Journals

    The Journal of Early Childhood Research is a peer-reviewed journal that provides an international forum for childhood research, bridging cross-disciplinary areas and applying theory and research within the professional community. This reflects the world-wide growth in theoretical and empirical research on learning and development in early childhood and the impact of this on provision.

  6. Early childhood development: an imperative for action and measurement

    Population-based measures of early child development and proxies of children at risk give an indication of prevalence, and indicators of disparity can be derived according to gender, urban-rural location and socioeconomic status. ... Washington DC, USA: Policy Research Working Paper, 2018. The Research Support Team. [Google Scholar] 11.

  7. Early Childhood Research Quarterly

    About the journal. (ECRQ) publishes research on and from birth through 8 years of age. ECRQ publishes predominantly empirical research (quantitative or qualitative methods) on issues of interest to early childhood development, theory, and educational practice. The journal also occasionally publishes practitioner and/or policy perspectives and ...

  8. Child Development Perspectives

    Child Development Perspectives is an SRCD journal publishing brief articles spanning the entire spectrum of modern developmental science and its applications. We welcome papers from all fields that inform modern developmental science, written in accessible language for a wide audience. ... including Child Development, the Journal of Research on ...

  9. Interactive technology use and child development: A systematic review

    This paper reports a systematic review examining the impact of digital technology use on child development. The studies reviewed in this review were predominantly correlational or comparative in nature and focused on cognitive domains of learning rather than a specific developmental outcome.

  10. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development

    I. Place-Based Developmental Research: Conceptual, Methodological, and Empirical Advances in the Study of Development in Context. Developmental scientists have, for some time, recognized that development unfolds in numerous settings, such as schools, extracurricular activities, hang-outs (i.e., places individuals may engage for recreation and pleasure, often unstructured environments), and ...

  11. Child Development

    Child Development. Since its inception in 1930, Child Development has been devoted to original contributions on topics in child development from the fetal period through adolescence. It is a vital source of information not only for researchers and theoreticians, but for a broad range of psychiatrists and psychologists, educators, and social ...

  12. PDF Child Development and Early Learning: A Foundation for Professional

    The Biology of Early Child Development. the role of the developing brain and other biological systems in early childhood. development:The developmental window (rapidity of brain development during early child-hood). The brain develops through a dynamic interac. ion between underlying biologi-cal processes and exposures and e.

  13. Early childhood social and emotional development ...

    This paper frames the subject of this special issue — how the field currently measures social and emotional development in early childhood. We first describe the relationship of social and emotional development to child functioning and overall well-being, and then present major measurement challenges associated with this domain, including a lack of clarity around conceptualizations of the ...

  14. Theories of Child Development and Their Impact on Early Childhood

    Developmental theorists use their research to generate philosophies on children's development. They organize and interpret data based on a scheme to develop their theory. A theory refers to a systematic statement of principles related to observed phenomena and their relationship to each other. A theory of child development looks at the children's growth and behavior and interprets it. It ...

  15. PDF The Science of Early Childhood Development

    that have emerged from decades of rigorous research in neurobiology, developmental psychology, and the economics of human capital formation, and considers their implications for a range of issues in pol-icy and practice. Core Concepts of Development • Child development is a foundation for community development and economic development, as capable

  16. Quality Early Education and Child Care From Birth to Kindergarten

    Early brain and child development research unequivocally demonstrates that human development is powerfully affected by contextual surroundings and experiences. 17, - 19 A child's day-to-day experiences affect the structural and functional development of his or her brain, including his or her intelligence and personality. 17, - 19 Children ...

  17. Developmental theories: Past, present, and future

    The Society for Research in Child Development lists "integrating diversity" as one of the goals of its strategic plan because "Human development is inextricably linked to, and shaped by, the diversity of human biology, society, and culture. ... One lesson from the changes in theorizing described in this paper is that development can be ...

  18. Child Development and Early Learning

    The domains of child development and early learning are discussed in different terms and categorized in different ways in the various fields and disciplines that are involved in research, practice, and policy related to children from birth through age 8. To organize the discussion in this report, the committee elected to use the approach and overarching terms depicted in Figure 4-1.

  19. InBrief: The Science of Early Childhood Development

    The science of early brain development can inform investments in early childhood. These basic concepts, established over decades of neuroscience and behavioral research, help illustrate why child development—particularly from birth to five years—is a foundation for a prosperous and sustainable society.

  20. (PDF) Exploring Parenting Styles and Their Impact on Child Development

    This research explores the relationship between parenting styles and child development in the community context. It delves into the various parenting styles, including authoritarian, democratic ...

  21. Single Parenting: Impact on Child's Development

    Parenting is a process and a state of being a parent. It includes nourishing, protecting, and guiding a child through development. 1 Traditionally, parenting included dichotomous gender-linked roles, with the mother being involved in nurturing and providing emotional support to the child, whereas the father was involved in providing protection and disciplining the child. 2 With the attaining ...

  22. Analyzing early child development, influential conditions, and future

    The paper provides an overview of a German cohort study of newborns which includes a representative sample of about 3500 infants and their mothers. The aims, challenges, and solutions concerning the large-scale assessment of early child capacities and skills as well as the measurements of learning environments that impact early developmental progress are presented and discussed. First, a brief ...

  23. 178 Current Child Development Research Paper Topics For 2023

    Here are some child development project topics you could try: Research asynchronous development. A project on physical growth. A project on gender role. A project on language and communication. Talk about the effects of malnutrition. Postnatal depression and child development.

  24. Psychosocial interventions for improving the physical health of young

    Young people and adults with ADHD are at risk of a range of physical health problems. There is limited guidance on how to approach health problems in ADHD, and especially around 16-25 year olds who will be transitioning from paediatric to adult care. The aim of this scoping review was to identify psychosocial interventions that target physical health in young people and adults with ADHD.

  25. A Study of Moral Development of Children and Good Habit Formation on

    DOI: 10.36948/ijfmr.2024.v06i04.24920 Corpus ID: 271449179; A Study of Moral Development of Children and Good Habit Formation on Indian Child Rearing Practices Context @article{Ramaiya2024ASO, title={A Study of Moral Development of Children and Good Habit Formation on Indian Child Rearing Practices Context}, author={Bhavna Ramaiya}, journal={International Journal For Multidisciplinary Research ...