Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Review Article
  • Open access
  • Published: 09 December 2023

Research on metaphor processing during the past five decades: a bibliometric analysis

  • Zhibin Peng 1 &
  • Omid Khatin-Zadeh 2 , 3  

Humanities and Social Sciences Communications volume  10 , Article number:  928 ( 2023 ) Cite this article

2039 Accesses

1 Citations

1 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Language and linguistics

Metaphor processing has been the subject of extensive research over the past five decades. A systematic review of metaphor processing publications through bibliometric tools can provide a clear overview of research on metaphor processing. In this study, we used the CiteSpace bibliometric tool to conduct a systematic review of publications related to metaphor processing. A total of 3271 works published and indexed in the Web of Science (WoS) were gathered. These works had been published between 1970 and 2022. We analyzed the co-citations of these works by CiteSpace to identify the most influential publications in metaphor processing research. A co-occurrence term analysis was done to identify dominant topics in this area of research. The results of this analysis showed that Language, comprehension, metaphor, figurative language , and context were the most frequent keywords. The most prominent clusters were students, figurative language, right hemisphere, embodied cognition, comprehension, N400 , and anger . Based on the results of this analysis, we suggest that task properties such as response format and linguistic features should be carefully taken into account in future studies on metaphor processing.

Similar content being viewed by others

research on metaphor analysis

The critical role of interference control in metaphor comprehension evidenced by the drift–diffusion model

research on metaphor analysis

COGMED: a database for Chinese olfactory and gustatory metaphor

research on metaphor analysis

Multimodal metaphors in a Sino-British co-produced documentary

Introduction.

How people understand and produce metaphors has long aroused the interest of scholars from various disciplines such as philosophy, linguistics, and psychology. From the 1970s, scholars began to study the processing of metaphors through experiments. Throughout the past five decades, a large body of experimental and theoretical works on metaphor have been produced, and many journals have started to publish papers related to metaphor. During this period, many researchers in neurolinguistics and psycholinguistics published their works on metaphor. These works have fundamentally changed the ways that researchers have been studying metaphors. This is particularly the case with research on metaphor processing. According to a study conducted by Han et al. ( 2022 ), research on metaphor processing has been the most active area of research on metaphor.

Metaphor processing research is an interdisciplinary area of study on metaphor that involves linguistics, psychology, and neuroscience. A large number of works on metaphor processing have been published in recent years, including reviews directed at selected subtopics. For instance, Rai and Chakraverty ( 2020 ) provided a systematic review of computational models and approaches to metaphor comprehension. This systemic review presented a concise yet representative picture of computational metaphor processing. In a related work, Kertész, Rákosi, and Csatár ( 2012 ) presented a review that was focused on the data, problems, heuristics, and results in cognitive research on metaphor. Some works have presented comprehensive reviews of studies conducted on metaphor comprehension in non-typical populations. For example, Morsanyi et al. ( 2020 ) conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of metaphor processing in autism. Kalandadze et al. ( 2019 ) also presented a systematic review and meta-analysis of studies on metaphor comprehension in individuals suffering from autism. This review specifically focused on task properties. However, among these works, except for a review paper published by Holyoak and Stamenković ( 2018 ), no other publication has specifically focused on theories and evidence related to metaphor processing. Furthermore, the past review papers have been primarily based on subjective judgment rather than bibliometric tools. Therefore, a systematic review conducted by bibliometric tools can shed new light on our understanding of metaphor processing research. In the literature of the field, we found just two works on bibliometrics of conceptual metaphor research (Han et al., 2022 ; Zhao et al., 2023 ). These two works have presented bibliometric assessments of published works on conceptual metaphor theory. However, they are only about conceptual metaphors in general. To fill this gap in the literature of the field, we used CiteSpace to present a systematic review of studies on metaphor processing.

CiteSpace is a bibliometric analysis tool that can provide an exhaustive account of research in any area over a certain period of time. In this way, it can suggest some directions for future research. Compared to those reviews relying on subjective judgment, a review conducted by CiteSpace can help us navigate through the key documents, research fields, and dominant topics in metaphor processing. Importantly, the results of such analysis can be presented in the form of easily understandable diagrams. We intended to identify the most productive and influential journals, authors, and institutions in the field of metaphor processing. Also, we intended to identify the most influential documents, active research areas, and dominant topics in metaphor processing research. Specifically, by analyzing the co-occurrence of keywords associated with metaphor processing, we aimed to depict a cluster picture of related keywords and dominant topics in this area of research. In this way, we intended to answer the following research questions:

Q1: What are the active research areas and dominant topics in metaphor processing research?

Q2: Is it possible to use a cluster picture of related keywords and research topics to identify research features that play a critical role in studies on metaphor processing?

We hypothesized that a cluster picture of related keywords and research topics in metaphor processing can be used to identify critical research properties that can be taken into account in future studies on metaphor processing.

Methodology

Data collection.

As the study was focused on metaphor processing, we collected and analyzed the published documents by conducting an advanced search in the Web of Science (WoS), Thomson Reuters Core Collection. This search incorporated Social Sciences Citation Index (SSCI), Arts and Humanities Citation Index (A and HCI), Science Citation Index Expanded (SCI-EXPANDED), and Conference Proceedings Citation Index-Social Science & Humanities (CPCI-SSH). We chose WoS as the data source for two reasons. Firstly, WoS has established an independent and comprehensive editing process to ensure the excellent quality of the journals and has formed an unparalleled data structure based on more than 50 years of consistent, accurate, and complete indexing. The indexed journals in the Web of Science Core Collection have been carefully selected. Therefore, the articles indexed in WoS are of high quality. Secondly, WoS is CiteSpace’s primary data source. CiteSpace has been designed to work with WoS data. Datasets from other sources have to be transformed before they can be visualized in CiteSpace.

The following fields were used to retrieve the data:

TS = (metaphor*) AND (process* OR comprehen*)), which means that only articles with both “metaphor” and “process” or comprehen(sion) in the title or abstract, or keywords are retrieved.

Time span=1970–2022

Document Type=article OR review

(“*”is a wildcard in WoS that represents any group of characters, including no character. For example, metaphor*=metaphor, metaphors, and metaphorical, etc. In addition, the review articles in this research do not contain book reviews.)

Totally, 8358 papers were collected from 123 WoS categories, including experimental psychology, neurosciences, business, linguistics, management, music, nursing, and law. In our study, we specifically focused on metaphor-processing research in the fields of linguistics, psychology, and neurosciences. Therefore, we chose the WoS categories related to linguistics, psychology, neurosciences, literature, communication, sociology, philosophy, anthropology, religion, history, and law (i.e. “Linguistics” or “Language Linguistics” or “Psychology Experimental” or “Education Educational Research” or “Neurosciences” or “Psychology Multidisciplinary” or “Psychology Clinical” or “Psychology” or “Psychology Psychoanalysis” or “Psychology Educational” or “Psychology Applied” or “Psychology Social” or “Psychology Developmental” or “literature” or “communication” or “sociology” or “philosophy” or “anthropology” or “religion” or “history” and “law”). After excluding those works that were unrelated to metaphor processing, 3271 publications remained for further analysis.

Descriptive analysis

Before visualization by CiteSpace, we conducted a descriptive analysis of yearly publication trends. Our aim was to identify the most productive journals, authors, and institutions. These descriptive analyses were directly done on the data obtained from the WoS website. The number of works published each year has been given on the WoS website. We used SPSS software to obtain the annual trend of publications (see Fig. 1 ). The numbers of publications for each journal, author, and institution have also been given on the WoS website. We selected the top ten for analysis.

figure 1

The diagram reveals the publication number for each year and the general trend.

CiteSpace analysis

The descriptive analysis of WoS provides only a basic overview of the research field. It cannot provide an exhaustive account of the research projects over previous decades and directions for future research. Previous reviews without bibliometric tools mainly relied on prior knowledge and subjective judgment. To address this problem, we used CiteSpace to examine the structures of the knowledge of metaphor processing that have been developed over the past years.

In this study, we used CiteSpace, a bibliometric analysis program developed by Chen ( 2004 , 2006 , 2017 ; also see Chen et al., 2010 ; Chen and Song, 2019 ). Bibliometric analysis offers an objective and quantitative method for examining published works in a certain area of research (Mou et al., 2019 , p. 221; Chen, 2020 ). CiteSpace is a Java application for analyzing co-citations and presenting them in the form of visual co-citation networks (Chen, 2004 ). CiteSpace is one of the most well-known bibliometric tools. It offers a variety of analyses, such as keyword analysis and reference analysis, to help academics identify current and upcoming research trends in a field (Mou et al., 2019 ). The bibliographic data files we collected from WoS were in the field-tagged Institute for Scientific Information Export Format. The “full record and cited references” was selected as the content. In this way, CiteSpace could easily identify the files. Once the files were loaded into the CiteSpace, the following procedural operations were performed on them: time slicing, thresholding, modeling, pruning, merging, and mapping (Chen, 2004 ).

In this study, we conducted two separate visualizing analyses of the data. One was a document co-citation analysis, which helped us to identify the important documents in metaphor processing research. A co-cited reference was called a node, and when several nodes were strongly related to one another, they formed a cluster. The other was a keyword co-occurrence analysis. The purpose of this analysis was to identify the most-discussed areas in research on metaphor processing.

Publication years, journals, productive authors, and institutions on metaphor processing

In the Web of Science core collection, the first article about metaphor processing we obtained was published in 1971 by Laurette ( 1971 ). There was no publication on metaphor processing in the years 1972, 1973, and 1974. From 1995 to 2022, more than 50 works were done each year. The maximum number of annual publications belongs to 2021 with 198 published works. Figure 1 presents the annual publications on metaphor processing. Overall, the results show a steady increase in publications on metaphor processing. Therefore, it can clearly be seen that metaphor processing has caught the attention of more and more researchers worldwide.

The 3271 articles or reviews that were examined in this study were published in a number of journals. Table 1 lists the 10 journals that published the highest number of papers in this area of research. With 116 publications on metaphor processing, Metaphor and Symbol , the only SSCI-indexed journal publishing works on metaphor research, was in first place among journals in terms of the number of publications. Frontiers in Psychology and Journal of Pragmatics were in second and third places, with 98 and 71 publications, respectively. The majority of the top 10 journals, as seen in Table 1 , are in the fields of psychology or neuroscience. When considering a submission, metaphor-processing researchers might use Table 1 to select appropriate journals for their papers.

The 10 authors having the highest number of publications in metaphor processing are listed in Table 2 . The author with the most papers published on metaphor processing was Mashal (36), followed by Faust (28) and Gibbs (26).

Table 3 lists the 10 institutions having the highest number of published works in metaphor processing. The University of California is at the top of this list with 131 publications in total, followed by the University of London with 76 articles and Bar Ilan University with 64 articles (Table 3 ).

Document co-citation analysis

A fundamental measure used by academic communities to assess the impact of a publication is the frequency of citations. The value of a published work and its impact on the field is at least partly dependent on the number of works that have been cited. We can identify the important documents in a knowledge domain by analyzing document co-citations. CiteSpace is an efficient tool that can conduct such analysis.

We analyzed document co-citations of 3271 publications collected from the WoS. We used CiteSpace to visualize the 3271 bibliographic recordings from 1970 to 2022. The top 50 papers having the highest number of citations in each 3-year were chosen using a time slice of three years. In order to include all the references cited in those documents regardless of when they were published, we set the Look Back Years (LBY) parameter to −1. Cutting off long-range citation linkages had a positive impact on the clarity of the results; it could increase the clarity of the network structure because long-distance links frequently go hand in hand with a spaghetti-like network. The results are shown in Fig. 2 . The cited publications and co-citation relationships across the entire data set were represented by 1055 distinct nodes and 5928 linkages, respectively. The top 10 articles in the area of metaphor processing research are shown in Table 4 .

figure 2

The diagram of document co-citations reveals the top 10 most cited articles among the 3271 publications collected from the WoS.

Totally, between 1970 and 2022, 39 documents were cited more than 50 times. The top three most-cited publications in the world’s publications related to metaphor processing are classic books about Conceptual Metaphor Theory (CMT) in general, not about metaphor processing. The work that has received the most citations is “ Metaphors we Live by ” authored by Lakoff and Johnson ( 1980 ). This frequently cited book was a landmark that revolutionized research on metaphor processing. It contends that metaphor is a way of thinking, not just a rhetorical instrument. To put it simply, our conceptual system is fundamentally metaphorical. In contrast to earlier works that looked at metaphor as a purely linguistic figure of speech, this book emphasizes the conceptual nature of metaphor. It defines metaphor as a conceptual process in which a source domain is mapped into a target domain. For example, the conceptual metaphor ARGUMENT IS WAR, in which “argument” is the target and “war” is the source, can be used to explain a statement like “I defended my argument.” Since its introduction in 1980, Conceptual Metaphor Theory (CMT) has gained popularity across various disciplines. The second-most quoted work is also written by Lakoff and Johnson ( 1999 ). This book challenged the Western traditional philosophy by proposing Embodied Philosophy based on the premise that our actions and our languages are based on our bodily experiences. Embodied Philosophy contends that abstract concepts are largely metaphorical. Embodied Philosophy is thus considered as the philosophical basis of Cognitive Linguistics. The third most cited document is a monograph by Gibbs ( 1994 ). Gibbs illustrates that human cognition is inherently poetic and that figurative imagination is central to how we comprehend ourselves and our surroundings. It challenges the traditional understanding of the mind by demonstrating how figurative characteristics of language reflect the poetic structure of the mind. Psychology, linguistics, philosophy, anthropology, and literary theory ideas and research are utilized to demonstrate fundamental ties between the poetic structure of the mind and daily language use. This monograph discusses methods and findings of psycholinguistic and cognitive psychology research to assess current philosophical, linguistic, and literary theories of figurative language. CMT aroused the interest of scholars from different disciplines such as linguistics, cognitive science, neuroscience, and psychology. Scholars in neurolinguistics and psycholinguistics are particularly interested in the cognitive processing of metaphors.

The other publications in Table 4 are not about metaphor in general but about metaphor processing in particular. In an article entitled “An fMRI investigation of the neural correlates underlying the processing of novel metaphoric expressions”, Mashal et al. ( 2007 ) used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate the neural networks involved in the processing of related pairs of words that formed literal, novel, and conventional metaphorical expressions. Four different kinds of linguistic expressions were read by the participants, who then determined the relation between the two words (metaphoric, literal, or unrelated). The results showed that the degree of meaning salience of a linguistic expression, rather than literality or nonliterality, modulated the degree of left hemisphere (LH) and right hemisphere (RH) processing of metaphors. This supported the Graded Salience Hypothesis (GSH, Giora, 1997 , 2003 ), which predicts a selective RH involvement in the processing of novel and nonsalient meanings. In this study, the salient interpretations were represented by conventional metaphors and literal expressions, whereas the nonsalient interpretations were represented by novel metaphorical expressions. Right posterior superior temporal sulcus, right inferior frontal gyrus, and left middle frontal gyrus showed considerably stronger activity when the novel metaphors were directly compared to the conventional metaphors. These findings back up the GSH and point to a unique function of the RH in the processing of novel metaphors. Additionally, verbal creativity may be selectively influenced by the right PSTS.

In order to look into the neural substrates underlying the processing of three different sentence types, Stringarisa et al. ( 2007 ) combined a novel cognitive paradigm with event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (ER-fMRI). Participants were required to read sentences that were either metaphorical, literal, or meaningless before deciding whether or not they made sense. The results of this experiment showed that various types of sentences were processed by various neural mechanisms. Both meaningless and metaphorical sentences activated the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG), but not literal sentences. Furthermore, despite the lack of difference between reaction times of literal and metaphoric sentences, the left thalamus is activated only in deriving meaning from metaphoric utterances. The authors attribute this to metaphoric interpretation’s flexibility and ad hoc concept formation. Their findings do not support the idea that the right hemisphere is primarily involved in metaphor comprehension, in contrast to earlier studies.

The two publications mentioned above used new research methods, such as fMRI and ER-fMRI. Additional research methods, such as repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) by Pobric et al. ( 2008 ) and positron emission tomography (PET) by Bohrn et al. ( 2012 ), were also used in other highly cited papers.

Co-occurring terms analysis

Keywords of any paper present its theme and some kind of summary of the subject that is going to be discussed in it. The occurrence of two keywords in a piece of writing indicates that these words are closely related to one another in the content of the work. The prevailing view is that if two or more terms appear together more frequently, they are more closely related. Betweenness Centrality is one of the functions of CiteSpace that specifies the strength of the relation between two or more terms. This gives us the ability to predict the occurrence of a given term with other terms even in other related topics. If a keyword displays a high Betweenness Centrality value, the keyword may be very significant. In this study, the research areas and dominant topics can be determined utilizing keyword co-occurrence analysis.

We analyzed the keywords to identify the terms and phrases that had co-occurred in at least two separate publications. Highly-frequent terms can show hotspots in a specific field of research (Chen, 2004 ). In this study, we chose the slice length of 3 years, and we set the LBY to 5 years. The results showed language, comprehension, metaphor, figurative language and context were the top 5 key terms having the highest frequencies. The network of related keywords is shown in Fig. 3 , and the terms with a frequency of more than 40 are listed in Table 5 .

figure 3

The keyword co-occurrence network diagram reveals the most popular keywords of metaphor processing research.

Cluster interpretations

We used CiteSpace to perform a cluster analysis on the basis of keyword co-occurrences. Totally, 528 nodes in the co-citation network with a 3-year time slice were obtained from the analysis. The seven greatest clusters in the research area of metaphor processing are displayed in Fig. 4 . Warmer colors represent more current research subjects, whereas cooler colors represent older research topics in the clusters.

figure 4

The network diagram of the keyword co-occurrence cluster reveals the most significant clusters of metaphor processing research.

Table 6 shows the top 7 clusters of keywords in metaphor processing research. It is obtained by using index terms as labels for clusters. Also, the clusters were shown by log-likelihood ratio (LLR). The top 7 clusters are named students, figurative language, right hemisphere, embodied cognition, comprehension, N400 , and anger .

Cluster #0, as the largest cluster, is labeled as ‘students’. For native speakers, using and understanding metaphors is simple. However, understanding figurative statements might be challenging for non-native speakers. Littlemore et al. ( 2011 ) found that at a British university, second-language learners had trouble understanding 40% of metaphorical terms that were easily understood by native speakers. Results of another study showed that second-language learners tended to use metaphors incorrectly and in the wrong contexts (Kathpalia and Carmel, 2011 ). It may be challenging for second-language speakers to comprehend and generate metaphors since the metaphorical meaning of a term is developed in the social and cultural context of native speakers. Metaphorical expressions that are easily and automatically understandable for native speakers of a certain language may not be easily interpretable for second-language speakers of that language due to not having enough exposure to that language and culture (Kecskes, 2006 ). Therefore, one of the main concerns for second-language teachers is to enhance second-language learners’ ability in understanding metaphoric language and to use it efficiently in the cultural context of the second language. As a result, there is a lot of discussion in metaphor research about how to improve students’ ability in using metaphors. For instance, Hu et al. ( 2022 ) performed a randomized controlled trial to assess how metaphors affected the symptoms of anxiety in Chinese graduate students.

Cluster #1 is labeled as ‘figurative language’. This cluster shows that two key components of executive functions (working memory and inhibition) could play significant roles in figurative language processing. Since working memory holds information for a short period of time, it plays an active role in discourse comprehension. Therefore, this component of executive functions helps the individual use discourse clues and contextual information in the process of metaphor interpretation. Contextually relevant information and metaphorically relevant information are put together (Wilson and Sperber, 2012 ), enabling the individual to extract the intended metaphorical meaning from an expression. This is done by the active involvement of working memory. Also, the role of inhibition, as another component of executive functions, has been documented in many studies (e.g., Glucksberg et al., 2001 ). These two components can be in close interaction with one another in the process of metaphor comprehension.

Cluster #2 is labeled as ‘right hemisphere’ by LSI test (Chen et al., 2010 ). This cluster shows that functional magnetic resonance imaging has been a common technique in research on the role of the right hemisphere in metaphor processing. Over the past 20 years, researchers in the fields of neurolinguistics and psycholinguistics have intensively studied the role of hemispheres in metaphor processing. Some scholars have hypothesized that the right hemisphere (RH) may have a special role in the processing of metaphorical language. However, many behavioral studies (e.g., Bohrn et al., 2012 ; Faust and Mashal, 2007 ; Mashal et al., 2007 ; Mashal and Faust, 2008 ) have evidence suggesting that the processing of familiar or conventional metaphors requires more left-lateralized processing, compared to the processing of unfamiliar metaphors. Additionally, bilateral processing of traditional metaphors was also supported by the findings of several studies (e.g., Bambini et al., 2011 ; Diaz et al., 2011 ). These results lend credence to the Graded Salience Hypothesis (GSH), according to which semantic salience plays a key role in metaphor processing (Giora, 1997 , 2003 ). According to this hypothesis, conventional, frequent, recognizable, and prototypical meanings are simpler to process than less-prominent meanings. Therefore, the meaning of a conventional metaphor is more salient and more accessible than its literal counterpart. On the other hand, in a novel metaphor, the literal meaning is more evident and the figurative meaning is disclosed later with the support of contextual clues. The GSH claims that unlike novel metaphors, whose meanings are acquired through integration and inferential processes, conventional metaphors’ prominent meanings are stored in the mental lexicon. The GSH also predicts that the left hemisphere (LH) is more active in comprehending conventional and salient metaphorical meanings, while the right hemisphere (RH) is more active in comprehending innovative and non-salient metaphorical meanings (Giora, 2003 ).

Cluster #3 is labeled as ‘embodied cognition’. Theories of embodied cognition challenge the traditional theories of cognition that are based on amodal symbols. These theories offer new perspectives on human cognitive processes. These theories hold that simulation, situated action, and bodily states play a crucial role in cognitive processes. Cognitive linguistics gave rise to some of the first set of theories that supported grounded cognition. Theories of embodied language processing emphasized the role of body, situation, and simulation in language as opposed to the amodal theories of grammar that emerged in the Cognitive Revolution (e.g., Chomsky, 1957 ). The study of embodiment has caught the interest of researchers working in traditional cognitive science, who have started to incorporate the ideas of embodiment in their works. The role of embodiment in language processing was developed and promoted by George Lakoff, Mark Johnson, Mark Turner, and Rafael Núñez based on advancements in the field of cognitive science (Lakoff, 1987 ; Lakoff and Johnson, 1980 ; Lakoff and Johnson, 1999 ; Lakoff and Turner, 1989 ; Lakoff and Núñez, 2000 ). In their studies, they have found evidence suggesting that people draw on their knowledge of everyday physical phenomena to comprehend concepts. According to theories of embodiment and embodied language processing, cognition and cognitive processes are based on the knowledge that comes from the body. There has been an increase in interest in studying the relationship between embodied cognition and language over the last four decades. According to theories of embodied cognition, when people understand words, their sensorimotor systems are engaged in simulating the concepts the words refer to (Jirak et al., 2010 ). Lakoff and Johnson’s ( 1980 , 1999 ) conceptual metaphor theory (CMT) is one of the most prominent theories of embodied cognition. This theory holds that situated and embodied knowledge serves as the metaphorical foundation for abstract concepts. Specifically, Lakoff and Johnson ( 1980 ) argued that abstract concepts are metaphorically understood in terms of concrete concepts with the support of sensorimotor systems. Many studies in various languages have demonstrated how individuals frequently use physical metaphors to discuss abstract concepts. Literature also uses a lot of these metaphors (e.g., Turner, 1996 ). A crucial question is whether these metaphors only reflect linguistic convention or whether they genuinely represent how we think (e.g., Murphy, 1997 ). There is mounting evidence that these metaphors are essential to our thought (e.g., Boroditsky and Ramscar, 2002 ; Gibbs, 2006 ).

Cluster #4 is labeled as ‘comprehension’. One of the keywords in this cluster is the term context . This supports the key role of context in the process of metaphor comprehension. Context of a conversation can provide some information that contributes to metaphor processing (e.g., Steen et al., 2010 ). It helps the individual disregard non-relevant literal meanings and keeps the metaphorically relevant information to derive the intended metaphorical meaning.

Cluster #5 is labeled as ‘N400’. The N400 is a part of time-locked EEG signals called event-related potentials (ERP). It is a negative-going deflection that normally peaks over centro-parietal electrode sites and occurs 400 ms after the stimulus begins, though it can also last between 250 and 500 ms. The N400 is a typical brain response to words and other meaningful (or potentially meaningful) stimuli, such as visual and auditory words, sign language signs, images, faces, environmental sounds, and odors (Kutas and Federmeier, 2000 , 2011 ). During the past 4 decades, ERP has been one of the techniques most frequently employed in cognitive neuroscience research to examine the physiological correlates of sensory, perceptual, and cognitive activities associated with information processing (Handy, 2005 ). ERP is also widely employed in metaphor processing studies, along with other imaging techniques such as fMRI, PET, and MEG.

Cluster #6 is labeled as ‘anger’. This cluster includes the key terms figurative language and eye tracking . This suggests that the metaphoric conceptualization of some emotional states and emotional terms such as anger can be reflected in eye movements. Interestingly, some works have suggested that this can happen not only for emotion-related concepts but also for other categories of abstract concepts that are metaphorically described in terms of movement (e.g., Singh and Mishra, 2010 ).

This clustering of keywords offers an organized and clear picture of key concepts that have been involved in various lines of research on metaphor processing. This clustering shows which lines of investigation have had a strong relationship with one another in research on metaphor processing. Therefore, the suggested clustering of keywords in metaphor comprehension offers a map for research on metaphor processing. This can be a guiding tool for researchers to have a clearer idea and organized map of how various lines of research on metaphor processing intersect with one another.

Discussion and implication for future studies

Over the past 50 years, metaphor processing has been a widely discussed topic among scholars in various disciplines, particularly researchers in neurolinguistics and psycholinguistics. Through the aforementioned document co-citation analysis, co-occurring word analysis, and cluster visualization which were done by CiteSpace, this study showed that research on metaphor processing has mainly focused on hemispheric processing of metaphors, metaphor comprehension, the embodied cognition basis of metaphor processing, behavioral-experiments study, ERP method and other techniques (fMRI, PET, and MEG, etc.), and the comparision of metaphor processing of adults with children.

Results of this study showed that research projects on metaphor processing are mainly conducted by experiments, including behavioral experiments, ERPs, and other imaging techniques such as fMRI, PET, and MEG. However, there is some conflicting evidence in the research findings. For instance, many studies have shown no statistically significant difference between ASD and TD groups in the understanding of metaphors and figurative language (Hermann et al., 2013 ; Kasirer and Mashal, 2014 ; Mashal and Kasirer, 2011 ; Norbury, 2005 ). These results suggest that factors other than disease-specific traits may account for the differences in results between studies. In the past, it has been discovered that group matching strategy and general language proficiency can account for part of the between-study variability in figurative language comprehension (Kalandadze, et al., 2018 ). However, further pertinent variables need to be examined in order to fully explain the observed variabilities. One reason for these different results may be due to the different theories the researchers adhere to. Another reason for mixed results may be due to the task properties of those experiments.

As for the theories on metaphor processing, there are two models that are widely used to study the processing of metaphors, namely, the Direct Access View (Gibbs, 1984 , 1994 ; Gibbs and Gerrig, 1989 ) and the Graded Salience Hypothesis (Giora, 1997 , 2003 ). According to the Direct Access View, in metaphor processing, the non-literal meaning of the metaphor can be directly processed, without inferring and discarding the literal meaning in an initial stage. Based on the Direct Access View, the Parallel Hypothesis was proposed, which holds that understanding figurative language is not different from that of literal language. Therefore, it is not necessary to assume any special cognitive mechanisms to process figurative language such as metaphors (Glucksberg et al., 1982 ). However, the Parallel Hypothesis can only hold if the literal and figurative meanings are fully understood. When the literal and figurative meanings are inconsistent, the coexistence of literal and figurative meanings cannot be explained by the Parallel Hypothesis. This does not mean that the literal meaning is abandoned before it is processed. Rather, the context facilitates the understanding of the inconsistent literal meanings. Therefore, the Direct Access View also supports the Context-dependent Hypothesis, which holds that we have a direct understanding of the figurative meanings with the help of sufficient contextual information.

Another theory on metaphor processing that is widely used to support metaphor research findings is the GSH (Giora, 1997 , 2003 ). As mentioned, the GSH holds that metaphor processing is influenced by the degree of semantic prominence. That is, conventional, frequent, recognizable, and prototypical meanings are easier to assimilate than less-salient meanings. One prediction of GSH is that the right hemisphere (RH) is more active in perceiving creative and non-salient metaphorical meanings, while the left hemisphere (LH) is more active in comprehending conventional and salient metaphorical meanings (Giora, 2003 ).

While the Direct Access View holds that metaphorical meaning is directly accessible, the Graded Salience Hypothesis assumes that metaphorical meaning is activated after the activation of the salient literal meaning. Depending on which theoretical framework is taken for certain research, different and conflicting results may be obtained. However, it should be noted that metaphor processing is a complex phenomenon and a large number of factors may be involved in it. Therefore, a single theory may not be able to describe all aspects of metaphor processing for all types of metaphors. The Direct Access View can describe the processing of highly conventional metaphors and idiomatic expressions. In daily conversations, people can easily produce and understand conventional metaphors and idiomatic expressions automatically. But, in some cases, this theory fails to describe the processing of novel metaphors. On the other hand, the Graded Salience Hypothesis may provide a better picture of how novel metaphors are processed. Therefore, in order to explain the discrepancies in research findings, we may need to take broader frameworks. When a single theoretical framework cannot explain discrepancies, two complementary frameworks can be taken and combined to explain and reconcile the conflicting results. Furthermore, a given theoretical framework may be more applicable to certain groups of people. For example, the GSH may be more applicable to ASD than the AD group, while the Direct Access View may be more applicable to TD than the ASD group. In other words, types of metaphors (e.g., conventional vs. novel metaphor), features of comprehenders (ASD vs. TD group), and possibly many other factors determine which theory of metaphor processing is most applicable. Putting various theoretical frameworks together and trying to make broader theoretical frameworks is a potential solution for responding to some questions that have not been answered yet.

Another reason for the differences in results between studies on metaphor processing may be the task properties of those experiments. There is a consensus in the literature of behavioral and neuroimaging studies that factors such as clinical populations, task characteristics, response format (i.e., multiple-choice vs. verbal explanation task), and lack of linguistic context can affect participants’ capacity to interpret metaphors (Pouscoulous, 2011 , 2014 , Rossetti et al., 2018 ). For instance, when assessed with an act-out rather than a verbal explanation task, children with TD demonstrate earlier proficiency in metaphor understanding. This may be because verbal and other types of tasks place different demands on a child’s linguistic and cognitive abilities (Pouscoulous, 2011 ). A similar explanation for how people with ASD perform metaphor tasks is based on response format. For instance, people with ASD may grasp metaphors similarly to people with TD, but they may have more trouble conveying them orally because of problems with expressive language (Kwok et al., 2015 ). Other aspects of the metaphors may also play a role, such as the amount and type of contextual information that is available to interpret the expression, or the degree of familiarity with the expression (Pouscoulous, 2011 , 2014 ). By combining the preceding studies utilizing the techniques of systematic review and a meta-analysis, Kalandadze et al. ( 2019 ) collated the knowledge that is currently available concerning task properties. Their aim was to find out how task properties affect metaphor comprehension ability in people with ASD compared to people with TD. They discovered that previous studies had used various kinds of materials and tasks that were either created by the researchers who designed the studies or were adapted from earlier research. The possible impact of the task properties was rarely taken into account in the previous studies, despite the fact that the task properties varied widely. Degree of individual’s familiarity with the metaphor (conventionality/novelty), degree of complexity of syntactic structure, linguistic and non-linguistic context (physical context) of the metaphoric expression, modality of stimulus (e.g., audio, visual), response format (verbal or non-verbal), and timing of the task are important task properties that can affect results of studies and their interpretations. Therefore, in order to obtain more accurate results, these factors need to be taken into account.

Implication for future studies

Based on the discussion in the section “Discussion”, we suggest that two issues deserve more consideration in future studies on metaphor processing. The first one is the theories that are employed to support the findings of metaphor processing studies. As different theories on metaphor processing may generate different conclusions, it is suggested that researchers discuss the results from different theoretical perspectives, rather than a single theory.

The second issue that merits more consideration is task properties. Task properties are important but have been neglected. The existing research on metaphor processing has paid little attention to the relevance of task properties in performance on metaphor comprehension tasks. Therefore, we contend that task properties including response format and linguistic features (i.e., metaphor familiarity, the syntactic structure of the metaphor, linguistic context, and stimulus modality) should be carefully considered in future investigations on metaphor processing. The systematic review and meta-analysis by Kalandadze et al. ( 2019 ) revealed that some task properties, including metaphor familiarity, are more frequently taken into account than others when determining the impact of a task. The least studied property in previous research is syntactic structure. Also, research on metaphor processing has not done a good job to examine the influence of contextual information on different groups of people. In future metaphor processing studies, these task properties merit additional consideration. When creating and reporting task properties in metaphor studies, researchers need to be extremely careful.

It should be noted that metaphor processing is a complex and multidimensional process. Therefore, in order to obtain a clear picture of various aspects of metaphor processing, researchers of various fields need to collaborate in interdisciplinary research projects. Neuroimaging data collected by neurolinguistics experts, behavioral data collected by researchers in psycholinguistics and cognitive science, and even corpus-based data can be combined to offer a broader picture of metaphor processing. Various types of evidence can complement each other and fill the gaps. This is a crucial point that should be considered in future research on metaphor processing.

As noted by Han et al. ( 2022 ), metaphor processing has been the most studied research area in metaphor research. Since the 1970s, how metaphors are processed in the brain has been extensively investigated by scholars in linguistics, neurolinguistics, and psycholinguistics. However, up to now, bibliometric tools like CiteSpace have not been used to systematically review literature on metaphor processing. In our study, a total of 3271 bibliometric recordings were collected from the Web of Science Core Collection. These documents had been published between 1970 and 2022. The descriptive analysis revealed a yearly increase in the number of publications, indicating that metaphor processing has caught the interest of academics from a variety of disciplines. Metaphor and Symbol , the sole SSCI-indexed journal devoted to metaphor research, took the first position among journals in terms of publishing yield with 116 publications on metaphor processing. Mashal, Faust, and Gibbs are the three most prolific authors in terms of publications on metaphor processing.

These bibliometric analyses through the CiteSpace software showed that language, comprehension, metaphor, figurative language , and context were the five most frequent keywords. Also, the most prominent clusters were students, figurative language, right hemisphere, embodied cognition, comprehension, N400 , and anger . These findings showed that research on metaphor processing has largely focused on the hemispheric processing of metaphors, metaphor comprehension, and embodiment in metaphor processing. Behavioral experiments, ERP and other techniques, such as fMRI, PET, and MEG were the common techniques in metaphor processing research. The current review through CiteSpace indicates that putting various theoretical frameworks together and trying to make broader theoretical frameworks is a potential solution for responding to some questions that have not been answered yet. This review also suggests that in future studies on metaphor processing, task properties such as response format and linguistic features should be carefully taken into account.

Although the current study aimed to be comprehensive within its defined scope, it was subject to some inevitable limitations. Firstly, being limited to WoS documents was one of the limitations of this study. Other databases such as Scopus, Google Scholar, Index Medicus, and Microsoft Academic Search were not included in this study. Secondly, publishers’ labeling of document types was not always correct. Some articles presented as reviews by WoS, for example, were not review papers at all (Yeung, 2021 ). Thirdly, we used only one scientometric instrument. Fourthly, while several prospective papers have recently been published, these studies were not acknowledged. Furthermore, because of obliteration, the citation count for some earlier published works was low.

Nonetheless, this study comprises a ground-breaking bibliometric assessment of global research on metaphor processing and provides a clear overview of global publications related to metaphor processing. Hence, it can be a helpful source for researchers interested in metaphor and metaphor processing. The results of this review have both theoretical and practical implications for the study of metaphor processing and metaphor in general.

Data availability

All data analyzed during this study can be accessed at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/JFRP5W .

Bambini V, Gentili C, Ricciardi E, Bertinetto PM, Pietrini P (2011) Decomposing metaphor processing at the cognitive and neural level through functional magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Res Bull 86:203–216

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Barsalou W (2008) Grounded cognition. Annu Rev Psychol 59:617–645

Bohrn IC, Altmann U, Jacobs AM (2012) Looking at the brains behind figurative language—a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on metaphor, idiom, and irony processing. Neuropsychologia 55:2669–2683

Article   Google Scholar  

Bowdle BF, Gentner D (2005) The career of metaphor. Psychol Rev 112(1):193–216. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.193

Boroditsky L, Ramscar M (2002) The roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychol Sci 13:185–188

Chen C (2004) Searching for intellectual turning points: progressive knowledge domain visualization. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101(Suppl 1):5303–5310

Article   ADS   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Chen C (2006) CiteSpace II: detecting and visualizing emerging trends and transient patterns in scientific literature. J Am Soc Inf Sci Technol 57(3):359–377. https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.20317

Chen C (2017) Science mapping: a systematic review of the literature. J Data Inf Sci 2(2):1–40. https://doi.org/10.1515/jdis-2017-0006

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

Chen C (2020) A glimpse of the first eight months of the COVID-19 literature on microsoft academic graph: themes, citation contexts, and uncertainties. Front Res Metr Anal 5:607286. https://doi.org/10.3389/frma.2020.607286

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Chen C, Ibekwe-SanJuan F, Hou J (2010) The structure and dynamics of cocitation clusters: a multiple-perspective cocitation analysis. J Am Soc Inf Sci Technol 61(7):1386–1409. https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.21309

Chen C, Song M (2019) Visualizing a field of research: a methodology of systematic scientometric reviews. PLoS ONE 14(10):e0223994. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0223994

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Chomsky N (1957) Syntactic structures. Mouton, The Hague

Diaz MT, Barrett KT, Hogstrom LJ (2011) The influence of sentence novelty and figurativeness on brain activity. Neuropsychologia 49:320–330

Faust M, Mashal N (2007) The role of the right cerebral hemisphere in processing novel metaphoric expressions taken from poetry: a divided visual field study. Neuropsychologia 45:860–870

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Gibbs RW (1984) Literal meaning and psychological theory. Cogn Sci 813:275–304

Gibbs RW (1994) The poetics of mind. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

Google Scholar  

Gibbs RW (2006) Embodiment and cognitive science. Cambridge University Press, New York

Gibbs RW, Gerrig RG (1989) How context makes metaphor comprehension seem “special”. Metaphor Symbol Activity 4/3:144–158

Giora R (1997) Understanding figurative and literal language: The graded salience hypothesis. Cogn Linguist 8:183–206

Giora R (2003) On our mind: salience, context, and figurative language. Oxford University Press

Glucksberg S, Gildea P, Bookin HB (1982) On understanding nonliteral speech: can people ignore metaphors. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 21:85–98

Glucksberg S, Newsome MR, Goldvarg Y (2001) Inhibition of the literal: filtering metaphor-irrelevant information during metaphor comprehension. Metaphor Symbol 16(3-4):277–293. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327868MS1603&4_8

Han Y, Peng Z, Chen H (2022) Bibliometric assessment of world scholars’ international publications related to conceptual metaphor. Front Psychol 13:1071121. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1071121

Handy TC (2005) Event related potentials: a methods handbook. Bradford/MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

Hermann I, Haser V, van Elst LT, Ebert D, Müller-Feldmeth D, Riedel A, Konieczny L (2013) Automatic metaphor processing in adults with Asperger syndrome: a metaphor interference effect task. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 263(Suppl 2):S177–S187. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-013-0453-9

Holyoak KJ, Stamenković D (2018) Metaphor comprehension: a critical review of theories and evidence. Psychol Bull 144(6):641–671. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000145

Hu J, Zhang X, Li R, Zhang J, Zhang W (2022) A randomized controlled trial to evaluate the effect of metaphors on anxiety symptoms among Chinese graduate students: the mediation effect of worry. Appl Res Qual Life https://doi.org/10.1007/s11482-022-10107-2

Jirak D, Menz MM, Buccino G, Borghi AM, Binkofski F (2010) Grasping language—a short story on embodiment. Conscious Cogn 19(3):711–720. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.020

Kalandadze T, Bambini V, Næss K (2019) A systematic review and meta-analysis of studies on metaphor comprehension in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: do task properties matter? Appl Psycholinguist 40:1421–1454. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716419000328

Kalandadze T, Norbury C, Nærland T, Næss BK-A (2018) Figurative language comprehension in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analytic review. Autism 22:99–117. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361316668652

Kathpalia SS, Carmel HLH (2011) Metaphorical competence in ESL student writing. RELC J 42(3):273–290

Kasirer A, Mashal N (2014) Verbal creativity in autism: comprehension and generation of metaphoric language in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder and typical development. Front Hum Neurosci 8:615. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00615

Kecskes I (2006) On my mind: thoughts about salience, context and figurative language from a second language perspective. Second Language Res 22:219–237

Kertész A, Rákosi C, Csatár P (2012) Data, problems, heuristics and results in cognitive metaphor research. Language Sci 34(6):715–727. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2012.04.011

Kutas M, Federmeier KD (2000) Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends Cogn Sci 4(12):463–470. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01560-6

Kutas M, Federmeier KD (2011) Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annu Rev Psychol 62:621–647. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123

Kwok EYL, Brown HM, Smyth RE, Cardy JO (2015) Meta-analysis of receptive and expressive language skills in autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectrum Disord 9:202–222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.10.008

Lakoff G, Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

Lakoff G, Johnson M (1999) Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. Basic Books

Lakoff G, Turner M (1989) More than cool reason: a field guide to poetic metaphor. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

Book   Google Scholar  

Lakoff G (1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things: what categories reveal about the mind. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

Lakoff G, Núñez RE (2000) Where mathematics comes from. Basic Books

Laurette P (1971) Metaphoric process and metonymic process-consideration of poem by valery. Linguistics 66:34–55

Littlemore J, Chen PT, Koester A, Barnden J (2011) Difficulties in metaphor comprehension faced by international students whose first language is not English. Appl Linguist 32:408–429

Mashal N, Faust M (2008) Right hemisphere sensitivity to novel metaphoric relations: application of the signal detection theory. Brain Language 104:103–112

Mashal N, Faust M, Hendler T, Jung-Beemane M (2007) An fMRI investigation of the neural correlates underlying the processing of novel metaphoric expressions. Brain Language 100(2):115–126

Mashal N, Kasirer A (2011) Thinking maps enhance metaphoric competence in children with autism and learning disabilities. Res Dev Disabil 32:2045–2054. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.08.012

Morsanyi K, Stamenković D, Holyoak K (2020) Metaphor processing in autism: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Dev Rev 57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2020.100925

Mou J, Cui Y, Kurcz K (2019) Bibliometric and visualized analysis of research on major e-commerce journals using CiteSpace. J Electron Commer Res 20(4):219–237

Murphy GL (1997) Reasons to doubt the present evidence for metaphoric representation. Cognition 62:99–108

Norbury CF (2005) The relationship between theory of mind and metaphor: evidence from children with language impairment and autistic spectrum disorder. Br J Dev Psychol 23:383–399. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X26732

Pobric G, Mashal N, Faust M, Lavidor M (2008) The role of the right cerebral hemisphere in processing novel metaphoric expressions: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. J Cogn Neurosci 20(1):170–181. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20005

Pouscoulous N (2011) Metaphor: for adults only? Belg J Linguist 25:51–79. https://doi.org/10.1075/bjl.25.04pou

Pouscoulous N (2014) “The elevator’s buttocks” metaphorical abilities in children. In: Matthews D (ed.) Pragmatic development in first language acquisition. Benjamins, Amsterdam

Rai S, Chakraverty S (2020) A survey on computational metaphor processing. ACM Comput Entertain 53:2. https://doi.org/10.1145/3373265 . Article 24, 35 pp

Rapp A, Leube D, Erb M, Grodd W, Kircher T (2004) Neural correlates of metaphor processing. Cogn Brain Res 20(3):395–402

Rossetti I, Brambilla P, Papagno C (2018) Metaphor comprehension in schizophrenic patients. Front Psychol 9:670. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00670

Schmidt G, Seger C (2009) Neural correlates of metaphor processing: the roles of figurativeness, familiarity and difficulty. Brain Cogn 71(3):375–386. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.001

Singh N, Mishra RK (2010) Simulating motion in figurative language comprehension. Open Neuroimaging J 4:46–52. https://doi.org/10.2174/1874440001004010046

Steen GJ, Dorst A, Herrmann JB, Kaal AA, Krennmayr T (2010) Metaphor in usage. Cogn Linguist 21(4):765–796. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2010.024

Stringarisa A, Medford N, Giampietro V, Brammer M, Davida A (2007) Deriving meaning: distinct neural mechanisms for metaphoric, literal, and non-meaningful sentences. Brain Language 100(2):150–162

Turner M (1996) The literary mind. Oxford University Press, New York

Wilson D, Sperber D (2012) Meaning and relevance. Cambridge University Press, New York

Yeung AWK (2021) Is the influence of Freud declining in psychology and psychiatry? A bibliometric analysis. Front Psychol 12:631516. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.631516

Zhao X, Zheng Y, Zhao X (2023) Global bibliometric analysis of conceptual metaphor research over the recent two decades. Front Psychol 14:1042121. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1042121

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors extend their appreciation to the Humanities and Social Science Research Projects of the Chinese Ministry of Education [Grant Number: 19YJA740044].

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Foreign Language Research Department, Beijing Foreign Studies University, No. 2, North Street of West Sanhuan Road, Haidian District, 100089, Beijing, P. R. China

Zhibin Peng

School of Foreign Languages, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, No. 2006, Xiyuan Avenue, West Hi-Tech Zone, 611731, Chengdu, Sichuan, P. R. China

Omid Khatin-Zadeh

University of Religions and Denominations, Pasdaran, 37185-178, Qom, Iran

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

ZP performed the data collection and analyses, and wrote the first version of the manuscript; ZP and OK-Z contributed to the revision of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Zhibin Peng .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethical approval

This article does not contain any studies with human participants performed by any of the authors.

Informed consent

Additional information.

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

3,271 bibliometric recordings from the web of science, rights and permissions.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Peng, Z., Khatin-Zadeh, O. Research on metaphor processing during the past five decades: a bibliometric analysis. Humanit Soc Sci Commun 10 , 928 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-023-02465-5

Download citation

Received : 06 June 2023

Accepted : 27 November 2023

Published : 09 December 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-023-02465-5

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

research on metaphor analysis

Metaphor research as a research strategy in social sciences and humanities

  • Published: 11 March 2023
  • Volume 58 , pages 227–248, ( 2024 )

Cite this article

research on metaphor analysis

  • Sepehr Ghazinoory   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-6761-4694 1 &
  • Parvaneh Aghaei   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-5185-3589 1  

995 Accesses

3 Citations

Explore all metrics

Metaphors have so far inspired many researchers to explain complex concepts or new theorizing. But there is no clear instruction for metaphor-based research and its validation principles. Here, we first locate metaphor research in social sciences and humanities (SSH) and classify different types of its use. Then we describe the basics of metaphor including its concept, types, components and characteristics. Since the methodology is of special importance in SSH, we introduce the metaphor research strategy in the research onion. Then, inspired by the stages of creative thinking, we present a specific process to carry out this strategy. The stages of proposed process include "expression of rationale and research question", "identification of possible metaphors and selection of preferable one", "evidence collection and cross-domain mapping", and "fitting test". We also propose a fitting test consisting of seven essential principles, which ensure that the final design of the metaphor is appropriate and works properly. The results of this research can formalize and validate the metaphor research as an efficient strategy for theorizing, futurology and describing complex concepts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

research on metaphor analysis

Similar content being viewed by others

research on metaphor analysis

Using Metaphors in Sociology: Pitfalls and Potentials

research on metaphor analysis

The Role of Metaphors in Model-Building Within the Sciences of Meaning

research on metaphor analysis

Do Metaphors Mean or Point? Davidson’s Hypothesis Revisited

Explore related subjects.

  • Artificial Intelligence

Humanities is the subjective study of humans and their history, culture, and societies with an analytical and critical approach (2021), while social science is the objective study that deals with human behavior in its social and cultural aspects with a scientific and evidence-based approach (Webster 1981 ).

Adams, R.B., Funk, P.: Beyond the glass ceiling: does gender matter? Manage. Sci. 58 , 219–235 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.1110.1452

Article   Google Scholar  

Afshari-Mofrad, M., Ghazinoory, S., Montazer, G.A., Rashidirad, M.: Groping toward the next stages of technology development and human society: a metaphor from an Iranian poet. Technol. Forecast. Soc. Change 109 , 87–95 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.techfore.2016.04.029

Alberts, B., Johnson, A., Lewis, J., Raff, M., Roberts, K., Walter, P.: Molecular Biology of the Cell, 4th edition. New York: Garland Science (2002)

Alty, J.L., Knott, R.P., Anderson, B., Smyth, M.: A framework for engineering metaphor at the user interface. Interact. Comput. 13 (2), 301–322 (2000)

Bager-Elsborg, A., Greve, L.: Establishing a method for analysing metaphors in higher education teaching: a case from business management teaching. High. Educ. Res. Dev. 38 , 1329–1342 (2019)

Barnet-Verzat, C., Wolff, F.C.: Gender wage gap and the glass ceiling effect: a firm-level investigation. Int. J. Manpow. 29 , 486–502 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1108/01437720810904185

Bartee, L., Shriner, W., Creech, C.: Principles of Biology: Biology 211, 212, and 213. Open Oregon Educational Resources (2017)

Baxter, J., Wright, E.O.: The glass ceiling hypothesis: a comparative study of the United States, Sweden, and Australia. Gender Soc. 14 (2), 275–294 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1177/089124300014002004

Beaty, R.E., Silvia, P.J.: Metaphorically speaking: cognitive abilities and the production of figurative language. Mem. Cognit. 41 , 255–267 (2013). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0258-5

Benton, T., Craib, I.: Philosophy of social science: The philosophical foundations of social thought. Macmillan Education UK (2010)

Birdsell, B.J.: Creative Metaphor Production in a First and Second Language and the Role of Creativity Doctoral Dissertation. University of Birmingham, Birmingham (2018)

Google Scholar  

Botha, E.: Why metaphor matters in education. South African J. Educ. 29 , 431–444 (2009). https://doi.org/10.15700/saje.v29n4a287

Boxenbaum, E., Rouleau, L.: New knowledge products as bricolage: metaphors and scripts in organizational theory. Acad. Manag. Rev. 36 , 272–296 (2011). https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.2009.0213

Boyd, R.N.: Metaphor and theory change. In: Ortony, A. (ed.) Metaphor and Thought. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1993)

Brown, N.: Identity boxes: using materials and metaphors to elicit experiences. Int. J. Soc. Res. Methodol. 22 , 487–501 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1080/13645579.2019.1590894

Burke, K.: A grammar of motives. Univ of California Press, California (1969)

Book   Google Scholar  

Burnyeat, M.: The Theaetetus of Plato. Hackett Publishing, Indianapolis (1990)

Cao, M., Zhang, Q.: Supply chain collaboration: impact on collaborative advantage and firm performance. J. Op. Manag. 29 , 163–180 (2011)

Cassell, C., Lee, B.: Driving, steering, leading, and defending: journey and warfare metaphors of change agency in trade union learning initiatives. J. Appl. Behav. Sci. 48 , 248–271 (2012)

Chang, L.P.L., Jonathan, L.Y.: The role of scientific terminology and metaphors in management education. Eur. J. Soc. Sci. Educ. Res. 6 , 33–43 (2019)

Chiesa, V., Manzini, R.: Organizing for technological collaborations: a managerial perspective. R&D Manag. 28 , 199–212 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9310.00096

Chowdhury, R.: An appreciation of metaphors in management consulting from the conceptual lens of holistic flexibility. Syst. Res. Behav. Sci. 38 , 137–157 (2021)

Chun, L.: Cognitive Linguistics and Metaphoric Study. Foreign Language Teaching and Research press, Beijing (2005)

Cornelissen, J.P.: Beyond compare: metaphor in organization theory. Acad. Manag. Rev. 30 , 751–764 (2005)

Cornelissen, J.P.: Making sense of theory construction: metaphor and disciplined imagination. Organ. Stud. 27 , 1579–1597 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1177/0170840606068333

Cornelissen, J.P., Kafouros, M.: Metaphors and theory building in organization theory: what determines the impact of a metaphor on theory? *. Br. J. Manag. 19 , 365–379 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8551.2007.00550.x

Cornelissen, J.P., Oswick, C., Thøger Christensen, L., Phillips, N.: Metaphor in organizational research: context, modalities and implications for research—Introduction. Org. Stud. 29 , 7–22 (2008)

Crisp, P.: Allegory: conceptual metaphor in history. Lang. Lit. Int. J. Stylist. 10 , 5–19 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1177/0963-9470-20011001-01

Cruz, E.F., Cruz, A.M.R. Da: Design Science Research for IS/IT Projects: Focus on Digital Transformation. In: 2020 15th Iberian Conference on Information Systems and Technologies (CISTI), IEEE, pp. 1–6. (2020)

Danaeifard, H., Javan Ali Azar, M.: Methodology of metaphorizing in organizational and management studies: a descriptive approach. Working paper (2021)

Dancygier, B., Sweetser, E.: Figurative Language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (2014)

Dastranj, R., Ghazinoory, S., Dastranj, N., Shayan, A.: Assessment of big data ecosystem in Iran with metaphor of millennium ecosystem assessment model. Iran. J. Inf. Process. Manag. 34 , 1613–1642 (2019)

Demjén, Z., Semino, E.: Using metaphor in healthcare: Physical health. In: The Routledge handbook of metaphor and language. pp. 403–417. Routledge (2016)

Doctorow, E.L.: “False Documents.” American Review 26, (1977)

Dörfler, V., Baracskai, Z., Velencei, J.: Understanding creativity. Trans. Adv. Res. 6 (2), 18–26 (2010)

Fadaee, E.: Symbols, metaphors and similes in literature: a case study of “Animal Farm.” J. English Lit. 2 , 19–27 (2011)

Ghazinoory, S., Phillips, F., Afshari-Mofrad, M., Bigdelou, N.: Innovation lives in ecotones, not ecosystems. J. Bus. Res. 135 , 572–580 (2021)

Ghazinoory, S., Nasri, S., Dastranj, R., Sarkissian, A.: “Bio to bits”: the Millennium Ecosystem Assessment (MA) as a metaphor for Big Data ecosystem assessment. Inf. Technol. People 35 (2), 835–858 (2022)

Harvey, M.: Human resource management in Africa: Alice’s adventures in wonderland. Int. J. Hum. Resour. Manag. 13 , 1119–1145 (2002)

Hazy, J.K.: More than a Metaphor: Complexity and the New Rules of Management. In: Allen, P., Maguire, S., and McKelvey, B. (eds.) Sage Handbook of Complexity and Leadership. Sage Publications (2011)

Hekkala, R., Stein, M., Rossi, M.: Metaphors in managerial and employee sensemaking in an information systems project. Inf. Syst. J. 28 , 142–174 (2018)

Hidalgo-Downing, L., Kraljevic-Mujic, B.: Metaphor and Persuasion in Commercial Advertising. Routledge Handbook Publications, Routledge (2017)

Humanities: https://www.britannica.com/topic/humanities (2021)

Hunt, S.D., Menon, A.: Metaphors and competitive advantage: evaluating the use of metaphors in theories of competitive strategy. J. Bus. Res. 33 , 81–90 (1995)

Johnson-Sheehan, R.D.: The emergence of a root metaphor in modern physics: max Planck’s ‘quantum’metaphor. J. Tech. Writ. Commun. 27 , 177–190 (1997)

Judge, A.J.N.: Metaphor and the language of futures. Futures 25 , 275–288 (1993)

Koch, S., Deetz, S.: Metaphor analysis of social reality in organizations. J. Appl. Commun. Res. 9 , 1–15 (1981)

Konttinen, J.: Managing the Creative Process in Game Development. Bachelor’s thesis, School of Business and Management, LUT University, Finland (2018)

Kövecses, Z.: Conceptual metaphor theory. In: Semino, Elena, and Zsófia Demjén, (eds.) The Routledge handbook of metaphor and language. pp. 31–45. Routledge (2016)

Kuhn, T.S.: Metaphor in science. Metaphor Thought 2 , 533–542 (1979)

Lakoff, G.: The Contemporary Theory of Metaphor Metaphor and Thought. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1993)

Lakoff, G., Johnson, M.: Conceptual metaphor in everyday language. J. Philos. 77 , 453 (1980). https://doi.org/10.2307/2025464

Lakoff, G., Johnson, M.: Metaphors We Live By. The university of Chicago press, London (2008)

Lubart, T.I.: Models of the creative process: past, present and future. Creat Res. J. 13 , 295–308 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326934CRJ1334_07

Lubart, T.I., Getz, I.: Emotion, Metaphor, and the creative process. Creat Res. J. 10 , 285–301 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1004_1

MacCormac, E.R.: Metaphor and Myth in Science and Religion. Duke Univ Press, Durham (1976)

Mahootian, F.: Metaphor in chemistry: an examination of chemical metaphor. Philos. Chem. Growth New Discip. (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9364-3_9

Martin, R.M.: Of time and the null individual. J. Philos. 62 , 723–736 (1965)

McCulloch, W.S., Pitts, W.: A logical calculus of the ideas immanent in nervous activity. Bull. Math. Biophys. 5 , 115–133 (1943)

McMullen, J.S.: Organizational hybrids as biological hybrids: Insights for research on the relationship between social enterprise and the entrepreneurial ecosystem. J. Bus. Ventur. 33 (5), 575–590 (2018)

Moore, J.F.: The death of competition: Leadership and strategy in the age of business ecosystem. Harper Paperbacks (1996)

Morgan, G.: Paradigms, metaphors, and puzzle solving in organization theory. Adm. Sci. Q. (1980). https://doi.org/10.2307/2392283

Morgan, G.: Images of Organization. Berrett-Koehler Publishers, California (1998)

Morgan, G.: Images of organization. Berrett-Koehler Publishers (1986)

Nasrollahi, M., Ramezani, J.: A model to evaluate the organizational readiness for big data adoption. International Journal of Computers, Communications and Control. 15, (2020)

Oakley, T.: Conceptual integration. In: Handbook of Pragmatics. pp. 1–24. John Benjamins Publishing Company, Amsterdam (2011)

Oswick, C., Keenoy, T., Grant, D.: Note: metaphor and analogical reasoning in organization theory: Beyond orthodoxy. Acad. Manag. Rev. 27 , 294–303 (2002)

Paley, W.: Natural theology. Evolution and creationism 22–26 (2007)

Pulaczewska, H.: Aspects of Metaphor in Physics: Examples and Case Studies. Walter de Gruyter, Berlin (2011)

Saunders, M., Lewis, P., Thornhill, A.: Reserach methods for business students. Essex: Prentice Hall: Financial Times (2009)

Sawyer, R.K.: The Stages of the Creative Process. In: Explaining Creativity: The Science of Human Innovation. New York: Oxford University Press (2006)

Shaw, M.P.: The Eureka Process: a structure for the creative experience in science and engineering. Creat Res. J. 2 , 286–298 (1989). https://doi.org/10.1080/10400418909534325

Simpson, J., Weiner, E.: Oxford English dictionary. Oxford University Press (1989)

Steinhart, E.: The Logic of Metaphor: Analogous Parts of Possible Worlds. Springer, Berlin (2001)

Tesch, B.J., Wood, H.M., Helwig, A.L., Nattinger, A.B.: Promotion of women physicians in academic medicine: glass ceiling or sticky floor? JAMA 273 , 1022–1025 (1995)

Van Mulken, M., Le Pair, R., Forceville, C.: The impact of perceived complexity, deviation and comprehension on the appreciation of visual metaphor in advertising across three European countries. J. Pragmat. 42 , 3418–3430 (2010)

Van Mulken, M., Van Hooft, A., Nederstigt, U.: Finding the tipping point: visual metaphor and conceptual complexity in advertising. J. Advert. 43 , 333–343 (2014)

Veblen, T.: Why is economics not an evolutionary science? Q. J. Econ. 12 , 373 (1898). https://doi.org/10.2307/1882952

Vu, N.N.: Structural, orientational, ontological conceptual metaphors and implications for language teaching. Ho Chi Minh City Open Univ. J. Sci. Soc. Sci. 5 (1), 49–53 (2015)

Wallas, G.: The Art of Thought. Harcourt, Brace (1926)

Webster, N.: Webster’s Third New International Dictionary of the English Language, Unabridged. Merriam-Webster, Springfield (1981)

Wexler, M.: Uncertainty as a root metaphor in social science. Free Inq. Creat. Sociol. 9 , 31–38 (1981)

Williams, L.: V: Teaching for the Two-Sided Mind. Simon and Schuster, New York (1986)

Winter, G.: Liberating creation: Foundations of religious social ethics. Crossroad (1981)

Zhang, X.: Development and critiques of conceptual metaphor theory. Theory Pract. Lang. Stud. 11 , 1487–1491 (2021). https://doi.org/10.17507/tpls.1111.18

Zhang, F., Hu, J.: A study of metaphor and its application in language learning and teaching. Int. Educ. Stud. 2 , 77–81 (2009)

Download references

No funding was received for conducting this study.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Information Technology Management, Tarbiat Modares University, Tehran, Iran

Sepehr Ghazinoory & Parvaneh Aghaei

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Parvaneh Aghaei .

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest.

The authors have no competing interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Ghazinoory, S., Aghaei, P. Metaphor research as a research strategy in social sciences and humanities. Qual Quant 58 , 227–248 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-023-01641-8

Download citation

Accepted : 22 February 2023

Published : 11 March 2023

Issue Date : February 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-023-01641-8

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Conceptual metaphor
  • Metaphor research
  • Research strategy
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research
  • Architecture and Design
  • Asian and Pacific Studies
  • Business and Economics
  • Classical and Ancient Near Eastern Studies
  • Computer Sciences
  • Cultural Studies
  • Engineering
  • General Interest
  • Geosciences
  • Industrial Chemistry
  • Islamic and Middle Eastern Studies
  • Jewish Studies
  • Library and Information Science, Book Studies
  • Life Sciences
  • Linguistics and Semiotics
  • Literary Studies
  • Materials Sciences
  • Mathematics
  • Social Sciences
  • Sports and Recreation
  • Theology and Religion
  • Publish your article
  • The role of authors
  • Promoting your article
  • Abstracting & indexing
  • Publishing Ethics
  • Why publish with De Gruyter
  • How to publish with De Gruyter
  • Our book series
  • Our subject areas
  • Your digital product at De Gruyter
  • Contribute to our reference works
  • Product information
  • Tools & resources
  • Product Information
  • Promotional Materials
  • Orders and Inquiries
  • FAQ for Library Suppliers and Book Sellers
  • Repository Policy
  • Free access policy
  • Open Access agreements
  • Database portals
  • For Authors
  • Customer service
  • People + Culture
  • Journal Management
  • How to join us
  • Working at De Gruyter
  • Mission & Vision
  • De Gruyter Foundation
  • De Gruyter Ebound
  • Our Responsibility
  • Partner publishers

research on metaphor analysis

Your purchase has been completed. Your documents are now available to view.

book: Current Approaches to Metaphor Analysis in Discourse

Current Approaches to Metaphor Analysis in Discourse

  • Edited by: Ignasi Navarro i Ferrando
  • X / Twitter

Please login or register with De Gruyter to order this product.

  • Language: English
  • Publisher: De Gruyter Mouton
  • Copyright year: 2019
  • Audience: Researchers, Students
  • Front matter: 6
  • Main content: 381
  • Illustrations: 23
  • Coloured Illustrations: 29
  • Keywords: Cognitive Linguistics ; Human Communication ; Metaphor ; Discourse
  • Published: September 2, 2019
  • ISBN: 9783110629460
  • ISBN: 9783110625882
  • Published: September 20, 2021
  • ISBN: 9783110764321

(Stanford users can avoid this Captcha by logging in.)

  • Send to text email RefWorks EndNote printer

Metaphor Analysis : Research Practice in Applied Linguistics, Social Sciences and the Humanities

Available online.

  • EBSCO Academic Comprehensive Collection

More options

  • Find it at other libraries via WorldCat
  • Contributors

Description

Creators/contributors, contents/summary.

  • Section 1 Introducing Metaphor Analysis 1 What is Metaphor and Why Does it Matter? Lynne Cameron 2 A Selective Survey of Research Practice in Published Studies Using Metaphor Analysis Zazie Todd, University of Leeds, and Graham Low, University of York Section 2 Contemporary Theories of Metaphor 3 The Cognitive View of Metaphor: Conceptual Metaphor Theory Alice Deignan, University of Leeds 4 Between Mind and Language: A journey Worth Taking David Ritchie, Portland State University 5 The Discourse Dynamics Framework for Metaphor Lynne Cameron Section 3 Metaphor Analysis 6 Identifying Metaphors in Discourse Data Lynne Cameron and Robert Maslen 7 Finding Systematicity in Metaphor Use Lynne Cameron, Robert Maslen and Graham Low 8 Metaphors and Discourse Activity Lynne Cameron 9 Corpus Techniques for Metaphor Analysis Alice Deignan and Elena Semino, Lancaster University 10 Working with Large Amounts of Metaphor Data Robert Maslen 11 Multimodal Metaphor Analysis Alan Cienki, Vrije Universiteit Section 4 Bringing it all Together 12 Guidelines for Good Practice in Meaphor Analysis Graham Low and Zazie Todd 13 Metaphor Analysis in a PhD Study: Analysing Online Discussions
  • Learning by Word of Mouse? Sue Rivers, Coventry University 14 Metaphor Analysis in a Social Sciences Project: Using Metaphor Analysis to Compare Expert and Public Perceptions of the Risk of Terrorism Lynne Cameron and Robert Maslen Appendix.
  • (source: Nielsen Book Data)

Bibliographic information

Browse related items.

Stanford University

  • Stanford Home
  • Maps & Directions
  • Search Stanford
  • Emergency Info
  • Terms of Use
  • Non-Discrimination
  • Accessibility

© Stanford University , Stanford , California 94305 .

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Research trends in multimodal metaphor: a bibliometric analysis.

\r\nZenan Zhong

  • School of Foreign Languages and Cultures, Guangdong University of Finance, Guangzhou, China

The concept of multimodal metaphor has generated a growing body of literature over the past decades. However, a systemic review of the domain seems to be lacking in relevant literature. This study, therefore, is an attempt to conduct a bibliometric analysis of the field of multimodal metaphor during 1977–2022, with a focus on 397 relevant publications retrieved from the Web of Science Core Collection (WoSCC) with the visualization tool VOSviewer. Some major quantitative findings are: (i) the number of publications in multimodal research began to surge in 2010 upon the seminal work of Forceville’s (2009); (ii) USA, China and Spain are the most productive countries; (iii) journals in the field of advertising, communication and linguistics are important sources of publications; and (iv) eleven clusters of keywords are identified, such as “visual metaphor”, “persuasion”, “pictures”, “impact”, “multimodal metaphor”, “model”, etc., representing crucial areas of interests. We also identified, by qualitative observations, three research trends in multimodal metaphor, driven by cognitive linguistic theory, the theory of pragmatics and visual/multimodal rhetoric theory, respectively. Various theoretical perspectives may shed light on possible further research on multimodal metaphor.

1. Introduction

The concept of multimodal/visual/pictorial metaphor has generated a growing body of literature over the past decades since the seminal works of Forceville (1996) . Multimodal metaphor, according to Forceville and Urios-Aparisi (2009 : 4), is a phenomenon that the target and source are each represented “exclusively or predominately” in different modes, while the visual/pictorial metaphor is considered a phenomenon where the target and source are represented predominately in one mode. However, Eggertsson and Forceville (2009 : 430) argued the definition of multimodal metaphor was a “pure” or “strict” metaphor and was “distinguished for analytical purposes only.” They further explained that since “the majority of multimodal metaphors in moving images cue target and/or source in more than one mode simultaneously,” they could be labeled as multimodal metaphors in the broad sense. The definition of multimodal metaphor thus in a broad sense presents its potential to be an umbrella term embracing visual or pictorial metaphors. For the convenience of discussion, we use multimodal metaphor as a general term to name such type of phenomenon in this review.

Multimodal metaphor could appear in various multimodal discourses, such as picture books, posts, magazines, TV shows, films, etc., which comprises different modes (written language, images, sound, gestures, etc.). The interpretation of multimodal metaphors is highly related to traditional metaphor studies. It could be classified into three major dimensions. One is studied within the framework of rhetoric. In rhetoric, the metaphor was used for persuasion or decoration. The representative scholar is Barthes (1977) , who first applies the theory to image studies. Another perspective is offered by pragmatics, which considers metaphor as creativity ( Sperber and Wilson, 1995 ) and should be understood with respect to context. Two relevant theories are developed under the perspective: the interaction theory and the blending theory. The interaction theory is developed by Indurkhya (1992) . It focuses on “interaction” and tries to invent a relation between the source and one of its activated features. Metaphor interpreted in this approach is often coined as a creative metaphor. The blending theory proposed by Fauconnier and Turner (2002) presupposes that different input spaces merge to create a new “blended space”. It combines selected elements from the input spaces, and as a result yields new, emergent meaning that is not present in either of the input spaces. The third dimension proceeds from a cognitive linguistic perspective. It is inspired by Lakoff and Johnson’s (1980) monograph Metaphor we live by . A metaphor is not only a rhetorical device but also a way of thinking and acting, whereas language is just an external manifestation of metaphor ( Lakoff and Johnson, 1980 ). Multimodal metaphor within this theory, according to Forceville (1996) , is a strictly directional phenomenon, positing a relationship between pairs of mental representations. It is concerned with entrenched conceptual relationships and how they may be elaborated.

In Forceville’s (2006) review, a number of issues concerning multimodal metaphor research are pointed out. Those issues include, such as the nature of multimodal metaphor, the difference between structural and creative metaphor, how important genre is for the construal and interpretation of metaphor, etc., ( Forceville, 2006 : 379). To address those issues, more and more scholars have come to extend the multimodal metaphor research to discourses of various genres such as advertising, political cartoons, comics, animation, TV news, films, etc. Those works have not only enriched multimodal metaphor studies but also improved theoretical models due to observations on data of greater varieties.

Despite the significant academic advancement made in multimodal metaphor research by far, the existing pile of literature has received scanty attention of systemic review. An investigation is thus necessary to take stock of the current state of the studies over the past decades. Therefore, this paper conducts a bibliometric analysis, using information visualization methods to make quantitative analysis and observe the indicators of authors, journals, countries, institutions, references and keywords of worldwide literature in a certain field. In this way, we can consolidate the understanding of the nature of multimodal metaphor and propose implications and research directions for future work to promote multimodal metaphor research. Our analysis is guided by the following research questions:

Q1: Who are the most influential authors on the subject of multimodal metaphor?
Q2: What countries/regions and journals are the most influential in the research field of multimodal metaphor?
Q3: What are the most important sub-fields of multimodal metaphor studies?
Q4: What are the research trends and possible future directions in the field of multimodal metaphor?

2. Data and methodology

We retrieved the data in our study on Oct 28, 2022 from the Web of Science (WoS) Core Collection Database in all editions excluding Conference Proceedings Citation Index - Science (CPCI-S), Current Chemical Reactions (CCR-EXPANDED), and Index Chemicus (IC). We searched “Topic” with the keywords “multimodal metaphor,” “pictorial metaphor” and “visual metaphor.” The procedure above generated a search result of 397 articles. Then we exported full record and cited references of the 397 studies and imported it to VOSviewer (version 1.6.18) for further analysis. The basic information of all documents including publication year, author, and country is also exported to an EXCEL file for analysis of possible patterns. We have adopted a minimal intervention approach in the data retrieval process to ensure that the quantitative results generated in our research presents least bias.

Following Brika et al. (2022) , we have gone through seven steps in the whole study process: study design, research questions, selected types of analysis (co-authorship, co-occurence, citation, bibliographic coupling, and co-citation), data compilation, exportation of basic document information including publication years, author, country/region to EXCEL, visualization (to both network maps in VOSviewer and curve/bar chart in EXCEL) and discussion. We have opted for a relatively low threshold and corresponding weight in visualization in VOSviewer for each type of analysis to present a thorough view of link strength in every network, as shown in Table 1 . In the final step, the three authors discussed and illuminated on the quantitative findings for interpretation.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Threshold and visualization for each type of analysis in VOSviewer networks.

3. Quantitative results

3.1. publication features, 3.1.1. publication years.

As is shown in Figure 1 , the multimodal metaphor research exhibits an overall increasing trend over the decades. Several features are noteworthy here. First, the period from 1977 to 2010 witnessed relatively small amounts of literature in this field with a peak at 7 studies in 1998. Second, the number of papers in the domain of multimodal metaphor began to surge in 2010, and reached a record high at 48 in 2020, since Forceville (2009) brought the term “multimodal metaphor” into the spotlight and began to draw increasing scholarly interests in the field.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Trends of annual publication of research in multimodal metaphor.

3.1.2. Country/region participation

A total of 53 countries or regions were involved in research related to multimodal metaphor across the whole world. As shown in Figure 2 , there are only 11 countries with more than 10 publications. The most productive countries or regions are USA (80), People’s Republic of China (41), Spain (41), England (34), and Netherlands (23). It is interesting to note that Canada (19), Germany (18), Australia (16), Italy (16), and France (15) share very similar total counts.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Countries or regions that participated in research related to multimodal metaphor.

Figure 3 reveals the result from citation analysis in terms of participating countries or regions. The top four countries are USA with a total number of documents of 75, People’s Republic of China with 73, Spain with 41 and England with 34. However, the number of links in England is 20, exceeding that in People’s Republic of China (18) and that in Spain (15). In other words, England is the second most influential country in the number of citation links, while People’s Republic of China and Spain are the third and the fourth. All the countries or regions involved present a certain degree of collaboration with others, albeit with a few of them including Slovenia, Norway, Chile, Lithuania, Saudi Arab and South Africa the least collaborative with only 1 link separately.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Countries or regions that show collaboration by citation in multimodal metaphor research (min number of citations of a country/region = 1; visualization by weight of documents).

3.1.3. Journal participation: citation analysis; co-citation analysis

Figure 4 reveals the most important sources of publication in terms of co-citation analysis. According to the assigned total link strength, the most influential journals for multimodal metaphor research are (as shown in Table 2 ): Année Psychologique (12), Journal of Advertising (44), Metaphor and Symbolic Activity/Metaphor and Symbol (59) (in separate clusters), Journal of Pragmatics (66), Sage Open (7), Accounting Education (9), Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development/Social Semitoics (6), Multimodal Communication in the 21st century: Professional and Academic (9), Frontiers in Psychology (13), Food Research International (7), Review of Cognitive Linguistics (28), Metaphor and the Social World (13), Visual Communication (44), Semiotica (20), Tydskrif vir Geesteswetenskappe (2) and Discourse and Communication (4). Among all the sources, Journal of Advertising and International Journal of Advertising are clearly the most influential, with a link strength of nine. This is followed by the linkage between Journal of Pragmatics and Visual Communication with a strength of eight. It is worth noticing that the connection between Journal of Pragmatics and Metaphor and Symbol is also strong with a link strength of four. However, when it comes to the number of total citations, Journal of Advertising is the most influential with 358 publications, while Journal of Pragmatics, the second, has 301. The fact that the two journals, Journal of Advertising and Journal of Pragmatics, have such considerable impact shows how the research of multimodal metaphor becomes recognized in the academia of related disciplines.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Citation analysis by sources with all sources in account (min number of citations of a source = 1; visualization by weight of documents).

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Journal(s), citation and link strength in the 17 clusters from citation analysis by sources.

In addition, it is clear from the network map in Figure 5 that a significant number of these sources are co-cited. As shown in Table 3 , five sources seem to hold a dominate position over the others: Applied Cognitive Linguistics (263 citations and 8,665 total link strength), Journal of Advertising (217 citations and 8,600 total link strength), Journal of Consumer Research (215 citations and 7,972 total link strength), Journal of Pragmatics (192 citations and 7,369 total link strength) and Metaphors We Live By (1980) (115 citations and 3,370 total link strength). It can be seen that although Lakoff and Johnson (1980) set out by defining conceptual metaphor and implicated its potential in multimodal meaning making, it is other sources that serve to extend the application of CMT theory in the multimodal fields.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Co-citation Analysis of Cited Sources (min number of citations of a source = 3; visualization by weight of citations).

www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Top five sources according to link strength from co-citation analysis by sources.

3.1.4. Participating authors: co-authorship of authors, co-citation of authors

Figure 6 illustrates the partnership network between all the authors as the co-authorship analysis is considered with a minimum of two citations of an author, showing the most influential authors. As is shown, the co-authorship network presents seven prominent authors divided into two clusters. The leading authors in Cluster #1 are Gerhard Schmalz and Dirk Ziebolz, each with three documents and total link strength of 12. In Cluster #2, Tom Sensky is slightly more influential than the other two authors, as he features three documents and total link strength of 10.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Co-authorship of authors (min number of citations of an author = 2; visualization by weight of documents).

The citation pattern of the other authors is shown in Figure 7 with a minimum of two citations per author. These authors are grouped into four clusters. Charles Forceville is clearly the most influential author in Figure 7 , with the total link strength reaching 76 and the total number of citations standing at 361. It is found that Charles Forceville is the most important author in two clusters generated by VOSviewer, whereas in the remaining two, Amitash Ojha and Peter Kravanja contribute the most with the total link strength of 35 and 22, and the total number of citations, 26 and 11.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Network of cited authors (min number of citations of an author = 2; visualization by weight of documents).

3.1.5. Citation: most cited reference, bibliographic coupling

The citation analysis of documents as revealed in Figure 8 shows there are 15 clusters with a minimum of one citation accounted. As is presented in Table 4 , the leading references in each of these clusters are: Kogan et al. (1980) , Johns (1984) , Forceville (2002) , Teng and Sun (2002) , Tsakona (2009) , van Mulken et al. (2010) , Bounegru and Forceville (2011) , Delbaere et al. (2011) , Hidalgo Downing and Kraljevic Mujic (2011) , Ortiz (2011) , Yu (2011) , Feng and O’Halloran (2013) , Indurkhya and Ojha (2013) , Danado and Paternò (2014) , Hart (2017) .

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. VOSviewer network map showing the most cited reference (min number of citations of a document = 1; visualization by weight of citations).

www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The representative document in the 15 clusters according to citation analysis.

Bibliographic coupling shows the extent to which the documents share the same citations. The network map in Figure 9 shows a total of nine clusters with a minimum of nine citations in one document, as the lowest number of citations among the leading documents listed above is nine. In each of the nine clusters, Forceville (2002) in Cluster #1, van Mulken et al. (2014) in Cluster #2, Hlawatsch et al. (2011) in Cluster #3, van Mulken et al. (2010) in Cluster #4, Wise (1999) in Cluster #5, Tsakona (2009) in Cluster #6, Stark (2011) in Cluster #7, Ng and Koller (2013) in Cluster #8 and Lee (2007) in Cluster #9.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. VOSviewer network map showing the bibliographic coupling of documents (min number of citations of a document = 9; visualization by weight of total link strength).

3.2. Research domain of co-occurrence: key word plus

The purpose of co-occurrence keyword analysis is to look into the relationship between keywords in a set of publications to uncover the topical issues and help scholars better grasp current research concerns. A total of 549 keywords were investigated, 137 of which appeared more than two times. Figure 10 shows the visual network map of keyword co-occurrence. There are in total 137 items, 11 clusters. The total link strength is 803. The different colored nodes represent different domains of interests in multimodal metaphor. The size of a node implies the occurrence of keywords. The closeness of the relationship between any two items is shown by the thickness of the connection lines.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. VOSviewer network map showing the co-occurrence of key word plus (mini occurrences of a key word = 2; visualization by weight of occurrences).

According to the results of cluster analysis, 11 key areas of research are found: Cluster #1 is related to the visual metaphor comprehension ability of the individuals with intellectual disability. For example, Shnitzer-Meirovich et al. (2018) conduct a program to enhance their analogical thinking and find they can recruit the ability required for visual metaphor comprehension.

Cluster #2 concerns the visualization of the attitudes and the framework for multimodal metaphor comprehension. One of the representative works is Forceville’s (2009) model of multimodal metaphor within a cognitive framework which becomes the mainstream in this field.

Cluster #3 is about the information design and the exploration of the images. For instance, Ojha and Indurkhya (2020) apply perception theory to analyze the design of visual metaphors.

Cluster #4 reveals a perspective from discourse approach to the metaphor representing various emotions. Feng and O’Halloran (2013) , integrating social semiotic theory and cognitive linguistic theory, proposes a multimodal discourse approach to explore the structural features of the visual metaphor mapping various emotions.

Cluster #5 concerns the appreciation of visual metaphor in people with old age-related diseases. One of the representative works explores those people’s humor preferences, which finds that they enjoy simple and familiar ones ( Kmita et al., 2022 ).

Cluster #6 is related to the contribution of metonymy to multimodal metaphor or visual metaphor. Those studies demonstrate the unignorable function of metonymy in understanding multimodal metaphor and propose the types of the interaction of multimodal metaphor and metonymy (e.g., Forceville and Urios-Aparisi, 2009 ).

Cluster #7 focuses on the impact of the persuasion of visual metaphors. Such studies prove that the persuasive effect could be more positive if the message designed in the form of visual metaphor (e.g., Meijers et al., 2019 ).

Cluster #8 is about space-time metaphor system. In this cluster, the issue is related to use visual metaphor method to understand space-time accessibility. For example, Jiang et al. (2022) use visual metaphor to understand the space-time accessibility of the Hong Kong-Zhuhai-Macao Belt.

Cluster #9 reveals a relation to the contribution of visual metaphor. For example, one of the studies show that the advertisement metaphorically representing the product with personification, appears to lead to more positive brand liking ( Marjorie et al., 2011 ).

Cluster #10 is about the function of visual metaphor in representing the evolution of the eubacteria. Those studies demonstrate the appropriateness of visual metaphor to depict the formation of phenotypic variants of bacterial cells ( Sánchez-Romero and Casadesús, 2021 ).

Cluster #11 concerns the understanding of the art of the images. In this cluster, a representative work is by Poppi et al. (2020) , who point out the current interpretations of the metaphorical structures do not always work within the domain of art cognition and thus propose a participant-based framework.

According to the clustering topics, we find that the main issues of multimodal metaphor studies concern what framework to use, what model to interpret, and how to interpret metaphors in different genres, such as advertisements, films, etc. The co-occurrence of the keyword analysis also shows that the effect of multimodal metaphor is one of the hot topics. Those hot topics can be concluded as six top keywords. They are “visual metaphor” with 24 occurrences. “pictures” with 15 occurrences, “persuasion” with 14 occurrences, “model” with 12 occurrences, as well as “impact” and “multimodal metaphor” with 11 occurrences, respectively.

The result implies the current research trend of multimodal metaphor is still in trying to develop an applicable model and in understanding their impacts.

4. Discussion

The co-occurrence of the keyword analysis displays that the hot topics related to multimodal metaphor studies could be classified into two research aims. One is related to the framework or the models for the exploration of visual or multimodal metaphor, the other is related to the impact or the power of the multimodal metaphor. The three major approaches driven by the interaction, the conceptual and the blending theories have been used widely and further developed by scholars to study different types of multimodal discourse. The major concern is on the impact of visual metaphor ( van Mulken et al., 2010 , 33), such as the persuasion of advertisements, cartoons or political discourse ( Teng and Sun, 2002 ; Tsakona, 2009 ; Hidalgo Downing and Kraljevic Mujic, 2011 ; Indurkhya and Ojha, 2013 ). The following sections discuss the research trend and possible future directions in multimodal metaphor studies.

4.1. Research direction driven by cognitive linguistic theory

The conceptual multimodal metaphor initiated by Forceville (1996 , 2006) has been integrated with social semiotic theory and further developed by Feng and O’Halloran (2013) . Feng and O’Halloran (2013) explored the structural features of visual images and models the visual representation of metaphor with respect to the representational, interactive and compositional metafunctions. Their social semiotic model provides a comprehensive account of the visual realization of both creative and conventional metaphors. The model has been widely applied in studies with a social semiotic background and has been used to address the impact of multimodal metaphor in different types of multimodal texts, such as advertisements (e.g., Liu and Zhang, 2020 ). Nonetheless, more empirical studies concerning more different genres are needed to demonstrate its usefulness. Further, how multimodal metaphor works and how to interpret its power is a research direction in the current research landscape.

4.2. Research direction driven by the theory of pragmatics

Within the interaction theory, Ojha and Indurkhya (2016) proposed an improved model for metaphor processing based on the perception theories ( O’Regan and Noe, 2001 ; Zimbardo and Gerrig, 2002 ) and integrated model of text and image processing ( Schnotz, 2002 ). The model comprises top-down and bottom-up mechanisms, which allow the conceptual and the perceptual features to stimulate each other. Such metaphor features are seen as emergent features. In their model, context is a key role in identifying the source and the target of a visual metaphor. The model has provided implications for the analysis on the design of visual metaphors (e.g., Ojha and Indurkhya, 2020 ), which would continue to be a direction worth further study.

In the blending theory, Fauconnier and Turner (2002) developed the model into a more systematic, mature and adaptive theory. The three dimensions for the interpretation: composition, completion and elaboration, have been further expanded. The generated emergent structure in the three dimensions is called “running the blend” and is seen as a dynamic and complex cognitive process highly related to the social world. This model thus concerns the pragmatic and sociological interpretation. This model has been applied by Li and Dai (2020) to explore the hidden ideology of print advertisement. It has also been used in the science education field, such as Fredriksson and Pelger (2020) . They use it to help students verbalize and visualize abstract phenomena and concepts. Their study demonstrates that the model could support students to understand science matters and their learning process and suggests research directions both in exploiting the use of multimodal metaphors in the education field and in evaluating the use of multimodal metaphors.

4.3. Research direction driven by visual/multimodal rhetoric theory

While the above three approaches have illuminated the major directions of multimodal metaphor studies, recent research shows there is another emerging new direction in multimodal metaphor studies. Such studies tend to use the term visual metaphor and draw on rhetoric, pragmatics and argumentation theories, aiming to analyze the impact of multimodal metaphors in, particularly, multimodal arguments. The researchers apply the visual rhetorical theory driven by Barthes’s rhetoric of images, to understand what rhetorical effect of the use of visual metaphor or other rhetorical devices for the reconstruction of argumentation ( Kjeldsen, 2018 ; Tseronis, 2021 ). The study implies a research tendency of combining multimodal rhetoric theory and argumentation theory to investigate the power of multimodal metaphor in multimodal arguments. The exploration in this field could make clear how multimodal argumentation works as well as how to evaluate them.

In all, the studies within cognitive linguistic theory provide an access to understanding the operation of multimodal metaphor mechanism. The studies within pragmatics accentuate the role of social cultural context and the pragmatic functions of multimodal metaphor. Further, the studies from a visual rhetoric theory are enlightening in integrating conceptual metaphor theory, pragmatics and argumentation theory to explore the rhetorical effect. Those studies also show an interdisciplinary approach to multimodal metaphor research.

To conclude, based on the literature review, we have found that cognitive topics are still the common trend in multimodal metaphor studies. Second, most of the multimodal metaphor studies taking advertisements as data. Further research may include more types of multimodal discourse to understand multimodal metaphor more systematically and therefore to further improve current modes for multimodal metaphor interpretations. Third, the impact of multimodal metaphors in multimodal arguments is a new field worth further exploration. Last, more works should be done using multimodal corpus analysis and empirical approaches to prove the applicability of various models.

5. Conclusion

Multimodal metaphor including metaphor constructed in non-verbal expressions, i.e., pictorial/visual metaphor is an important concept as it is based on theoretical mechanisms of interaction, blended space and conceptual blending and is applicable in many practical scenarios such as advertisement and animation. While multimodal metaphor has been a prospering concept that has been increasingly investigated, there remain few studies that address its research trend through a bibliometric analysis.

Based on the 397 articles obtained from the Web of Science (WoS) Core Collection Database in all relevant editions, the current study reveals significant patterns in publication features including publication years, country/region participation, journal publication, participating authors, citation and research domain of concurrence. It is found that there has been a general rising trend in the research on multimodal metaphor with 2020 the most fruitful year. USA is the country with the highest number of documents and collaboration links. Journal of Advertising and Journal of Pragmatics are two most influential sources in terms of the number of citations and co-citations. With minimal co-authorship with others, Charles Forceville is the most impactful author as far as the total link strength and the total number of citations are concerned. This has been confirmed by citation analysis of most cited reference and bibliographic coupling. Co-occurrence in key word plus suggests 11 research domains related to what framework to use, what model to interpret, and how to interpret metaphors in different genres.

In addition to the two research aims concluded from the most frequent hot topics, we have discussed three research directions driven by three different strains of theories, while they are all cognitive in nature. Our findings suggest that the research trends in existing research on multimodal metaphor lie in the types of multimodal discourses, the investigation in multimodal arguments and the use of multimodal corpus analysis.

This study can be potentially useful for those attempting to contribute to the existing line of research, as it provides a detailed account of the entire landscape of literature on multimodal metaphor and sheds light on the possible research directions for further exploration. However, there are limitations that should be noted. First, only one source of data is considered. Future research can involve other sources of data such as Google Scholar and Scopus to generate a more extending view. Second, we have only used VOS viewer as the only bibliometric tool, which means the aspects discussed on the scholarship collected are restricted. It is possible to employ other similar tools such as CiteSpace and Network Workbench to look into more features of the publications.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

ZZ compiled the data and did the statistics and was devoted to chapters 2, 3.1 and conclusion. SW was devoted to chapters 1, 3.2, and 4. SC participated in the discussion and proofread the whole manuscript. All authors annotated the data, contributed to the article, and approved the submitted version.

This work was supported by Project of Humanities and Social Sciences of Ministry of Education in China (22YJA740004), Guangzhou Philosophy and Social Science Planning 2021 Project (2021GZGJ201), and Guangdong Social Science Planning 2021 Foreign Language Subject Special Project (GD21WZX01-02).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Barthes, R. (1977). “Rhetoric of the image,” in Image Music Text , ed. R. Barthes (London: Fontana).

Google Scholar

Bounegru, L., and Forceville, C. (2011). Metaphors in editorial cartoons representing the global financial crisis. Visual Commun. 10, 209–229. doi: 10.1177/1470357211398446

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brika, S. K. M., Chergui, K., Algamdi, A., Musa, A. A., and Zouaghi, R. (2022). E-learning research trends in higher education in light of COVID-19: a bibliometric analysis. Front. Psychol. 12:762819. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.762819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danado, J., and Paternò, F. (2014). Puzzle: a mobile application development environment using a jigsaw metaphor. J. Visual Languages Comput. 25, 297–315. doi: 10.1016/j.jvlc.2014.03.005

Delbaere, M., McQuarrie, E. F., and Phillips, B. J. (2011). Personification in advertising. J. Adv. 40, 121–130. doi: 10.2753/JOA0091-3367400108

Eggertsson, G. T., and Forceville, C. (2009). “Multimodal expressions of the human victim is animal metaphor in horror films,” in Multimodal Metaphor , eds C. Forceville and E. Urios-Aparisi (New York: Mouton de Gruyter).

Fauconnier, G., and Turner, M. (2002). The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind’s Hidden Complexities. New York: Basic Books.

Feng, D., and O’Halloran, K. (2013). The visual representation of metaphor. Rev. Cogn. Linguist. 11, 320–335. doi: 10.1075/rcl.11.2.07fen

Forceville. (2009). “Non-verbal and multimodal metaphor in a cognitivist framework: agendas for research,” in Multimodal Metaphor , eds C. Forceville and E. Urios-Aparisi (New York, NY: Mouton de Gruyter).

Forceville, C. (1996). Pictorial Metaphor in Advertising. New York, NY: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203272305

Forceville, C. (2002). The identification of target and source in pictorial metaphors. J. Pragmatics 34, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/S0378-2166(01)00007-8

Forceville, C. (2006). “Non-verbal and multimodal metaphor in a cognitivist framework: agendas for research,” in Cognitive Linguistics: Current Applications and Future Perspectives , eds G. Kristiansen, M. Achard, R. Dirven, and F. J. Ruiz de Mendoza Ibanez (Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter), 379–402.

Forceville, C., and Urios-Aparisi, E. (2009). Multimodal Metaphor. New York, NY: Mouton de Gruyter. doi: 10.1515/9783110215366

Fredriksson, A., and Pelger, S. (2020). Conceptual blending monitoring students’ use of metaphorical concepts to further the learning of science. Res. Sci. Educ. 50, 917–940. doi: 10.1007/s11165-018-9717-8

Hart, C. (2017). Metaphor and intertextuality in media framings of the (1984–1985) British miners’ strike: a multimodal analysis. Discourse Commun. 11, 3–30. doi: 10.1177/1750481316683291

Hidalgo Downing, L., and Kraljevic Mujic, B. (2011). Multimodal metonymy and metaphor as complex discourse resources for creativity in ICT advertising discourse. Rev. Cogn. Linguistics 9, 153–178. doi: 10.1075/rcl.9.1.08hid

Hlawatsch, M., Leube, P., Nowak, W., and Weiskopf, D. (2011). Flow radar glyphs—static visualization of unsteady flow with uncertainty. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graphics 17, 1949–1958. doi: 10.1109/TVCG.2011.203

Indurkhya, B. (1992). Metaphor and Cognition. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. doi: 10.1007/978-94-017-2252-0

Indurkhya, B., and Ojha, A. (2013). An experimental study on the role of perceptual similarity in visual metaphors. Metaphor Symb. 28, 233–253. doi: 10.1080/10926488.2013.826554

Jiang, B., An, X., and Chen, Z. (2022). Understand space–time accessibility using a visual metaphor: a case study in Hong Kong–Zhuhai–Macao belt. Environ. Earth Sci. 81, 130. doi: 10.1007/s12665-022-10243-w

Johns, B. (1984). Visual metaphor: lost and found. Semiotica 52, 291–333. doi: 10.1515/semi.1984.52.3-4.291

Kjeldsen, J. E. (2018). Visual rhetorical argumentation. Semiotica 220, 69–94. doi: 10.1515/sem-2015-0136

Kmita, M., Lindner-Pawlowicz, K., and Libura, A. (2022). Who does not find metaphors funny? Humor preferences in geriatric patients. Commun. Sci. Disord. 27, 330–348. doi: 10.12963/csd.21885

Kogan, N., Connor, K., Gross, A., and Fava, D. (1980). Understanding visual metaphor: developmental and individual differences. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 45:85. doi: 10.2307/1165832

Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors We Live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Lee, J. (2007). The effects of visual metaphor and cognitive style for mental modeling in a hypermedia-based environment. Interact. Comput. 19, 614–629. doi: 10.1016/j.intcom.2007.05.005

Li, J., and Dai, G. (2020). Multimodal metaphor analysis of print advertisements based on the conceptual blending theory: exploring the hidden ideology-sign-consumption. Int. Bus. Res. 13, 31–45. doi: 10.5539/ibr.v13n5p31

Liu, Y., and Zhang, W. (2020). A study of national images in the economy-themed front covers of Beijing review from the perspective of multimodal metaphor theory. Foreign Lang. Res. 6, 30–35.

Marjorie, D., McQuarrie, E. F., and Phillips, B. J. (2011). Personification in advertising. J. Adv. 40, 121–130.

Meijers, M. H. C., Remmelswaal, P., and Wonneberger, A. (2019). Using visual impact metaphors to stimulate environmentally friendly behavior: the roles of response efficacy and evaluative persuasion knowledge. Environ. Commun. J. Nat. Cult. 13, 995–1008. doi: 10.1080/17524032.2018.1544160

Ng, C. J. W., and Koller, V. (2013). Deliberate conventional metaphor in images: the case of corporate branding discourse. Metaphor Symb. 28, 131–147. doi: 10.1080/10926488.2013.797807

Ojha, A., and Indurkhya, B. (2016). “On the role of perceptual features in metaphor comprehension,” in Metaphor and Communication , eds E. Gola and F. Ervas (Amsterdam: JohnBenjamins), 147–170. doi: 10.1075/milcc.5.08ojh

Ojha, A., and Indurkhya, B. (2020). “On the role of perceptual similarity in producing visual metaphor,” in Producing Figurative Expression , eds J. Barnden and A. Gargett (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), 105–126. doi: 10.1075/ftl.10.05ojh

O’Regan, K. J., and Noe, A. (2001). A sensory motor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behav. Brain Sci. 24, 939–1031. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01000115

Ortiz, M. J. (2011). Primary metaphors and monomodal visual metaphors. J. Pragmat. 43, 1568–1580. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2010.12.003

Poppi, F. I. M., Marianna, B., and Ojha, A. (2020). Imago dei: metaphorical conceptualization of pictorial artworks within a participant-based framework. Semiotica 2020, 349–376. doi: 10.1515/sem-2018-0077

Sánchez-Romero, M. A., and Casadesús, J. (2021). Waddington’s landscapes in the bacterial world. Front. Microbiol. 12:685080. doi: 10.3389/fmicb.2021.685080

Schnotz, W. (2002). Towards an integrated view of learning from text and visual displays. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 14, 101–120. doi: 10.1023/A:1013136727916

Shnitzer-Meirovich, S., Lifshitz, H., and Mashal, N. (2018). Enhancing the comprehension of visual metaphors in individuals with intellectual disability with or without down syndrome. Res. Dev. Disabil. 74, 113–123. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.01.010

Sperber, D., and Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and Cognition , 2nd Edn. Oxford: Blackwell.

Stark, M. (2011). The sandpile model: optimal stress and hormesis. Dose Response 10, 66–74. doi: 10.2203/dose-response.11-010.Stark

Teng, N. Y., and Sun, S. (2002). Grouping, simile, and oxymoron in pictures: a design-based cognitive approach. Metaphor Symb. 17, 295–316. doi: 10.1207/S15327868MS1704_3

Tsakona, V. (2009). Language and image interaction in cartoons: towards a multimodal theory of humor. J. Pragmat. 41, 1171–1188. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2008.12.003

Tseronis, A. (2021). From visual rhetoric to multimodal argumentation: exploring the rhetorical and argumentative relevance of multimodal figures on the covers of the economist. Vis. Commun. 20, 374–396. doi: 10.1177/14703572211005498

van Mulken, M., le Pair, R., and Forceville, C. (2010). The impact of perceived complexity, deviation and comprehension on the appreciation of visual metaphor in advertising across three European countries. J. Pragmat. 42, 3418–3430. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2010.04.030

van Mulken, M., van Hooft, A., and Nederstigt, U. (2014). Finding the tipping point: visual metaphor and conceptual complexity in advertising. J. Adv. 43, 333–343. doi: 10.1080/00913367.2014.920283

Wise, A. J. (1999). The ecological approach to text visualization. J. Am. Soc. Inf. Sci. 50, 1224–1233. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4571(1999)50:13<1224::AID-ASI8>3.0.CO;2-4

Yu, N. (2011). Beijing Olympics and Beijing opera: a multimodal metaphor in a CCTV Olympics commercial. Cogn. Linguistics 22, 595–628. doi: 10.1515/cogl.2011.023

Zimbardo, P. G., and Gerrig, J. R. (2002). “Perception,” in Foundations of Cognitive Psychology , ed. D. J. Levitin (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).

Keywords : multimodal metaphor, visual metaphor, pictorial metaphor, bibliometric analysis, web of science (WoS) database, trends

Citation: Zhong Z, Wen S and Chen S (2023) Research trends in multimodal metaphor: a bibliometric analysis. Front. Psychol. 14:1144725. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1144725

Received: 15 January 2023; Accepted: 31 March 2023; Published: 17 April 2023.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2023 Zhong, Wen and Chen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Suijun Wen, [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

  • DOI: 10.54097/zw1t3y94
  • Corpus ID: 272032426

Analysis of Multimodal Metaphor and Values Representation in Children’s Picture Books

  • Published in International Journal of… 20 August 2024
  • Education, Linguistics

7 References

Reading images: the grammar of visual design, metaphors we live by, conceptual integration networks, comparative analysis of chinese and british children’s death education picture books from multimodal critical analysis—a case study of death in a nut and grandma’s amulet, related papers.

Showing 1 through 3 of 0 Related Papers

Information

  • Author Services

Initiatives

You are accessing a machine-readable page. In order to be human-readable, please install an RSS reader.

All articles published by MDPI are made immediately available worldwide under an open access license. No special permission is required to reuse all or part of the article published by MDPI, including figures and tables. For articles published under an open access Creative Common CC BY license, any part of the article may be reused without permission provided that the original article is clearly cited. For more information, please refer to https://www.mdpi.com/openaccess .

Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or approaches, provides an outlook for future research directions and describes possible research applications.

Feature papers are submitted upon individual invitation or recommendation by the scientific editors and must receive positive feedback from the reviewers.

Editor’s Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area. The aim is to provide a snapshot of some of the most exciting work published in the various research areas of the journal.

Original Submission Date Received: .

  • Active Journals
  • Find a Journal
  • Proceedings Series
  • For Authors
  • For Reviewers
  • For Editors
  • For Librarians
  • For Publishers
  • For Societies
  • For Conference Organizers
  • Open Access Policy
  • Institutional Open Access Program
  • Special Issues Guidelines
  • Editorial Process
  • Research and Publication Ethics
  • Article Processing Charges
  • Testimonials
  • Preprints.org
  • SciProfiles
  • Encyclopedia

admsci-logo

Article Menu

  • Subscribe SciFeed
  • Recommended Articles
  • Author Biographies
  • Google Scholar
  • on Google Scholar
  • Table of Contents

Find support for a specific problem in the support section of our website.

Please let us know what you think of our products and services.

Visit our dedicated information section to learn more about MDPI.

JSmol Viewer

How ethical behavior is considered in different contexts: a bibliometric analysis of global research trends.

research on metaphor analysis

1. Introduction

2. literature review, 2.1. ethical behavior, 2.2. bibliometric, 3. methodology, 4.1. countries and their concerns about ethical behavior, 4.2. key themes in research terms, 4.3. bibliographic coupling analysis, 4.3.1. journals, 4.3.2. authors, 4.4. co-citation analysis, 4.4.1. publications, 4.4.2. journals, 4.4.3. authors, 5. discussion, 5.1. ethical behavior in consumption, 5.2. ethical behavior in leadership, 5.2.1. social learning theory (slt), 5.2.2. social exchange theory (set), transformational leadership, authentic leadership, spiritual leadership, 5.3. ethical behavior in business.

  • Focus on social responsibility;
  • Emphasis on honesty and fairness;
  • Focus on “Golden Rules”;
  • Values that are consistent with a person’s behavior or religious beliefs;
  • Obligations, responsibilities, and rights towards dedicated or enlightened work;
  • Philosophy of good or bad;
  • Ability to clarify issues in decision making;
  • Focus on personal conscience;
  • Systems or theories of justice that question the quality of one’s relationships;
  • The relationship of the means to ends;
  • Concern with integrity, what should be, habits, logic, and principles of Aristotle;
  • Emphasis on virtue, leadership, confidentiality, judgment of others, putting God first, topicality, and publicity.

Values, Business Ethics, and Corporate Social Responsibility (CSR)

5.4. ethical behavior in the medical context, 5.4.1. autonomy, 5.4.2. beneficence, 5.4.3. non-maleficence, 5.4.4. fairness, 5.5. ethical behaviour in education, 5.5.1. violation of school/university regulation, 5.5.2. selfishness, 5.5.3. cheating, 5.5.4. computer ethics, 5.6. ethical context in organization, 5.6.1. context of organizational ethical climate, 5.6.2. context of organizational ethical culture, 6. conclusions, 7. limitations and future research, author contributions, institutional review board statement, informed consent statement, data availability statement, conflicts of interest.

ClusterAuthorBaseConcept
Cluster 1: Ethical Behavior in Organization and BusinessAndreas ChatzidakisRoyal Holloway UniversityEthical consumption
John PelozaKentucky UniversityResponsibility
Sean ValentineLouisiana Tech UniversityEthical business, human management, and behavior in an organization
Linda TreviñoPennsylvania State UniversityBehavior in organizations and ethics, behavior in organizations and ethical business
Gary R. WeaverDelaware UniversityMoral awareness, ethical behavior in organizations
Cluster 2: Ethical Behavior in LeadershipBruce AvolioWashington UniversityEthical communication of leadership, strategic leadership from individual to global
Deanne N. Den HartogAmsterdam UniversityLeadership behavior in the organization, dynamic, international management
Jennifer J. Kish-GephartMassachusetts—Amherst UniversityBehavioral ethics, diversity, social inequality, behavior, business ethics
Fred O. WalumbwaArizona State University’s W.P.Authentic leadership
Cluster 3: Nervous, Deep Brain Stimulation, and DepressionLaura B. DunnStanford UniversityScientific and ethical issues related to deep brain stimulation for mood, behavioral, and thought disorders, ethics of schizophrenia, treatment of depression
Benjamin D. GreenbergBrown UniversityPsychiatry, neuroscience, anxiety-related features, deep brain stimulation, treatment-resistant depression
Joseph J. FinRockefeller University, Weill Cornell Medical CollegeConsciousness disorders, deep brain stimulation, neurotechnology, neuroethics
Thomas E. SchlaepferThe Johns Hopkins UniversityDeep brain stimulation, depression, anxiety, neurobiology
Cluster 4: Ethical CultureMarcus Dickson WayneState UniversityUnderlying leadership theories generalizing culture and multiculturalism, the influence of culture on leadership and organizations
Mary A. Keating Trinity College DublinMulticultural management, ethics, human resource management
Gillian S. MartinCollege DublinLeadership culture change
Christian ResickDrexel UniversityTeamwork, personality, organizational culture and conformity, ethical leadership, and ethical-related organizational environment
Cluster 5: Moral PsychologyMichael C. Gottieb and Mitchell M. HandelsmanThe University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center & University of KansasThe Ethical Dilemma in Psychotherapy, Ethical Psychologist Training: A Self-Awareness Question for Effective Psychotherapists: Helping Good Psychotherapists Become Even Better, APA Handbook of Ethics in Psychology
Samuel L. KnappDartmouth CollegePhysiological sustainability
Cluster 6: Ethical issues in health care, especially concerned with the knowledge of nursesJang, In-sunSungshin Women’s UniversityEthical decision-making model for nurses, nursing students, telehealth technology, research topics on family care between Korea and other countries
Park, Eun-junSejong UniversityNursing students, beliefs in knowledge and health, Korean nursing students, nurses’ organizational culture, health-related behavior
ClusterRepresentative AuthorBaseConcept
Cluster 1: Psychology, TPB, theory of the stages of moral development, the development of behavior in the context of makeupIcek AjzenMassachusetts Amherst UniversityTPB
Shelby D. HuntTexas Technology UniversityMarketing research
O.C. FerrellAuburn UniversityEthical marketing, social responsibility
Scott J. VitellMississippi UniversityBusiness administration, social psychology, marketing, management
Lawrence Kohlberg Theory of the stages of moral development
AnusornSinghapakdiOld Dominion University, Mississippi UniversityMarketing with subfields in consumer behavior and econometrics
Cluster 2: Social cognitive theory, ethical behavior in leadershipAlbert BanduraStanford UniversityBehaviorism and cognitive psychology, social learning theory originator, theoretical structure of self-efficacy
Michael E. BrownSam and Irene Black School of Business Penn State-Erie, The Behrend CollegeBehavioral leadership, ethics, ethical leadership, moral conflict
David M. MayerMichigan UniversityBehavioral ethics, leadership ethics, organizational behavior
Philip PodsakoffFlorida UniversityCitizen organization, behavioral organization, research methods leadership
Cluster 3: Psychological, emotional, and unethical behaviorFrancesca GinoHarvard Business SchoolUnethical, dishonest behavior
Jonathan HaidtNYU-SternEthical psychology, political psychology, positive psychology, business ethics
Ann E. TenbrunselNotre Dame UniversityPsychology of ethical decision making and the ethical infrastructure in organizations, examining why employees, leaders, and students behave unethically, despite of their best intention
Karl AquinoBritish Columbia UniversityEthics, forgiveness, victims, emotions.
Cluster 4: Ethical behavior in business and organizationTheresa Jones Ecological light pollution, chemical communication, immune function, history features, mating
Linda TreviñoPennsylvania State UniversityOrganizational behavior and business ethics
Gary R. WeaverDelaware UniversityBehavioral ethics in organizations
Bart VictorVanderbilt UniversityThe organizational basis of an ethical work environment
  • Agrell, Anders, and Roland Gustafson. 1994. The Team Climate Inventory (TCI) and group innovation: A psychometric test on a Swedish sample of work groups. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology 67: 143–51. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Aguinis, Herman. 2011. Organizational responsibility: Doing good and doing well. In APA Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology . Edited by Sheldon Zedeck. Maintaining, Expanding, and Contracting the Organization. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, vol. 3, pp. 855–79. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ahmed, Mohamed M., Kun Young Chung, and John W. Eichenseher. 2003. Business students’ perception of ethics and moral judgment: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Business Ethics 43: 89–102. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ajzen, Icek. 1991. The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 50: 179–211. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Allam, Zafrul, Muzaffar Asad, Nasir Ali, and Azam Malik. 2022. Bibliometric analysis of research visualizations of knowledge aspects on burnout among teachers from 2012 to January 2022. Paper presented at the 2022 International Conference on Decision aid Sciences and Applications (DASA), Chiangrai, Thailand, March 23–25; pp. 126–31. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • American Library Association (ALA). 1983. ALA Glossary of Library and Information Science . Chicago: American Library Association. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Aquino, Karl, and Americus Reed, II. 2002. The self-importance of moral identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83: 1423. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Arefeen, Sirajul, Mohammad E. Mohyuddin, and Mohammad Aktaruzzaman Khan. 2020. An exploration of unethical behavior attitude of tertiary level students of Bangladesh. Global Journal of Management and Business Research 20: 39–47. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Arman, Saleh Md, and Cecilia Mark-Herbert. 2022. Ethical Pro-Environmental Self-Identity Practice: The Case of Second-Hand Products. Sustainability 14: 2154. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Armstrong, Mary Beth, J. Edward Ketz, and Dwight Owsen. 2003. Ethics education in accounting: Moving toward ethical motivation and ethical behavior. Journal of Accounting Education 21: 1–16. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Arnaud, Anke, and Marshall Schminke. 2012. The ethical climate and context of organizations: A comprehensive model. Organization Science 23: 1767–80. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ashforth, Blake, Dennis A. Gioia, Sandra L. Robinson, and Linda K. Treviño. 2008. Re-viewing organizational corruption. Academy of Management Review 33: 670–84. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Asif, Muhammad, Miao Qing, Jinsoo Hwang, and Hao Shi. 2019. Ethical leadership, affective commitment, work engagement, and creativity: Testing a multiple mediation approach. Sustainability 11: 4489. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Aslam, Hassan Danial, Sorinel Căpușneanu, Tasawar Javed, Ileana-Sorina Rakos, and Cristian-Marian Barbu. 2024. The mediating role of attitudes towards performing well between ethical leadership, technological innovation, and innovative performance. Administrative Sciences 14: 62. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Avolio, Bruce J., and William L. Gardner. 2005. Authentic leadership development: Getting to the root of positive forms of leadership. The Leadership Quarterly 16: 315–38. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Avolio, Bruce J., William L. Gardner, Fred O. Walumbwa, Fred Luthans, and Douglas R. May. 2004. Unlocking the mask: A look at the process by which authentic leaders impact follower attitudes and behaviors. The Leadership Quarterly 15: 801–23. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Babin, Barry J., James S. Boles, and Donald P. Robin. 2000. Representing the perceived ethical work climate among marketing employees. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 28: 345–58. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bai, Yuntao, Li Lin, and Joseph T. Liu. 2019. Leveraging the employee voice: A multi-level social learning perspective of ethical leadership. The International Journal of Human Resource Management 30: 1869–901. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bandura, Albert. 1977. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review 84: 191–215. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bandura, Albert. 1999. Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. Personality and Social Psychology Review 3: 193–209. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Barnett, Tim, and Cheryl Vaicys. 2000. The moderating effect of individuals’ perceptions of ethical work climate on ethical judgments and behavioral intentions. Journal of Business Ethics 27: 351–62. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Barry, Vincent. 1979. Moral Issues in Business . Belmont: Wadsworth Publishing Company. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bass, Bernard M. 1985. Leadership and Performance Beyond Expectations . New York: Free Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bauman, Christopher W., and Linda J. Skitka. 2012. Corporate social responsibility as a source of employee satisfaction. Research in Organizational Behavior 32: 63–86. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Baur, Vivianne, Inge van Nistelrooij, and Linus Vanlaere. 2017. The sensible health care professional: A care ethical perspective on the role of caregivers in emotionally turbulent practices. Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy 20: 483–93. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Beauchamp, Tom L., and James F. Childress. 2001. Principles of Biomedical Ethics . New York: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Beauchamp, Tom L., and Norman E. Bowie. 1983. Ethical Theory and Business , 2nd ed. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Beaudoin, Cathy A., Anna M. Cianci, Sean T. Hannah, and George T. Tsakumis. 2019. Bolstering managers’ resistance to temptation via the firm’s commitment to corporate social responsibility. Journal of Business Ethics 157: 303–18. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Beciu, Silviu, Georgiana Armenița Arghiroiu, and Maria Bobeică. 2024. From Origins to Trends: A Bibliometric Examination of Ethical Food Consumption. Foods 13: 2048. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bedi, Akanksha, Can M. Alpaslan, and Sandy Green. 2016. A meta-analytic review of ethical leadership outcomes and moderators. Journal of Business Ethics 139: 517–36. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Benckendorff, Pierre, and Anita Zehrer. 2013. A network analysis of tourism research. Annals of Tourism Research 43: 121–49. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bhatti, Sabeen Hussain, Saifullah Khalid Kiyani, Scott B. Dust, and Ramsha Zakariya. 2021. The impact of ethical leadership on project success: The mediating role of trust and knowledge sharing. International Journal of Managing Projects in Business 14: 982–98. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bing, Mark N., H. Kristl Davison, Scott J. Vitell, Anthony P. Ammeter, Bart L. Garner, and Milorad M. Novicevic. 2012. An experimental investigation of an interactive model of academic cheating among business school students. Academy of Management Learning & Education 11: 28–48. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Blau, Peter M. 1964. Exchange and Power in Social Life . New York: John Wiley & Sons. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Broadus, Robert N. 1987. Toward a definition of “bibliometrics”. Scientometrics 12: 373–79. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Brown, Michael E., and Linda K. Treviño. 2006. Ethical leadership: A review and future directions. The leadership Quarterly 17: 595–616. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Brown, Michael E., and Marie S. Mitchell. 2010. Ethical and unethical leadership: Exploring new avenues for future research. Business Ethics Quarterly 20: 583–616. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Brown, Michael E., Linda K. Treviño, and David A. Harrison. 2005. Ethical leadership: A social learning perspective for construct development and testing. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 97: 117–34. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Bunn, Douglas N., Steven B. Caudill, and Daniel M. Gropper. 1992. Crime in the classroom: An economic analysis of undergraduate student cheating behavior. The Journal of Economic Education 23: 197–207. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Burns, James MacGregor. 1978. Leadership . New York: Harper & Row. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Camilleri, Mark Anthony. 2021. Strategic attributions of corporate social responsibility and environmental management: The business case for doing well by doing good! Sustainable Development 30: 409–22. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Carrington, Michal J., Ben Neville, and Robin Canniford. 2015. Unmanageable multiplicity: Consumer transformation towards moral self coherence. European Journal of Marketing 49: 1300–25. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Carroll, Archie B. 1991. The pyramid of corporate social responsibility: Toward the moral management of organizational stakeholders. Business Horizons 34: 39–48. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Carroll, Archie B. 2016. Carroll’s pyramid of CSR: Taking another look. International Journal of Corporate Social Responsibility 1: 3. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Cauffman, Elizabeth, S. Shirley Feldman, Lene Arnett Jensen, and Jeffrey Jensen Arnett. 2000. The (un) acceptability of violence against peers and dates. Journal of Adolescent Research 15: 652–73. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Chabowski, Brian R., Jeannette A. Mena, and Tracy L. Gonzalez-Padron. 2011. The structure of sustainability research in marketing, 1958–2008: A basis for future research opportunities. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 39: 55–70. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Chang, Yu-Wei, Mu-Hsuan Huang, and Chiao-Wen Lin. 2015. Evolution of research subjects in library and information science based on keyword, bibliographical coupling, and co-citation analyses. Scientometrics 105: 2071–87. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Chapman, Kenneth J., Richard Davis, Daniel Toy, and Lauren Wright. 2004. Academic integrity in the business school environment: I’ll get by with a little help from my friends. Journal of Marketing Education 26: 236–49. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Chatters, Linda M. 2000. Religion and health: Public health research and practice. Annual Review of Public Health 21: 335–67. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Chatzidakis, Andreas, Pauline Maclaran, and Alan Bradshaw. 2012. Heterotopian space and the utopics of ethical and green consumption. Journal of Marketing Management 28: 494–515. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Chen, Wei-Fen, Xue Wang, Haiyan Gao, and Ying-Yi Hong. 2019. Understanding consumer ethics in China’s demographic shift and social reforms. Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics 31: 627–46. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Chen, Ziguang, Wing Lam, and Jian An Zhong. 2007. Leader-member exchange and member performance: A new look at individual-level negative feedback-seeking behavior and team-level empowerment climate. Journal of Applied Psychology 92: 202. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Cheung, Erick H., and Joseph M. Pierre. 2015. The medical ethics of cognitive neuroenhancement. AIMS Neuroscience 2: 102–22. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Clark, Duncan, and Richie Unterberger. 2007. The Rough Guide to Shopping with a Conscience . New York: Rough Guides. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Çollaku, Lum, Arbana Sahiti Ramushi, and Muhamet Aliu. 2024. Fraud intention and the relationship with selfishness: The mediating role of moral justification in the accounting profession. International Journal of Ethics and Systems , ahead-of-print . [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Comoli, Maurizio, Patrizia Tettamanzi, and Michael Murgolo. 2023. Accounting for ‘ESG’ under Disruptions: A Systematic Literature Network Analysis. Sustainability 15: 6633. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Condon, Paul. 2019. Meditation in context: Factors that facilitate prosocial behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology 28: 15–19. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Cooper-Martin, Elizabeth, and Morris B. Holbrook. 1993. Ethical consumption experiences and ethical space. Advances in Consumer Research 20: 113–18. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Daradkeh, Mohammad. 2023. Navigating the complexity of entrepreneurial ethics: A Systematic review and future research agenda. Sustainability 15: 11099. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Dasborough, Marie T., Sean T. Hannah, and Weichun Zhu. 2020. The generation and function of moral emotions in teams: An integrative review. Journal of Applied Psychology 105: 433–52. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • De Hoogh, Annebel H. B., and Deanne N. Den Hartog. 2008. Ethical and despotic leadership, relationships with leader’s social responsibility, top management team effectiveness and subordinates’ optimism: A multi-method study. The Leadership Quarterly 19: 297–311. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • De la Hoz-Correa, Andrea, Francisco Muñoz-Leiva, and Márta Bakucz. 2018. Past themes and future trends in medical tourism research: A co-word analysis. Tourism Management 65: 200–11. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • DeConinck, James B. 2011. The effects of ethical climate on organizational identification, supervisory trust, and turnover among salespeople. Journal of Business Research 64: 617–24. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Di Nardo, Matteo, Anna Dalle Ore, Giuseppina Testa, Gail Annich, Edoardo Piervincenzi, Giorgio Zampini, Gabriella Bottari, Corrado Cecchetti, Antonio Amodeo, Roberto Lorusso, and et al. 2019. Principlism and personalism. Comparing two ethical models applied clinically in neonates undergoing extracorporeal membrane oxygenation support. Frontiers in Pediatrics 7: 312. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Dietz, Joerg, Sandra L. Robinson, Robert Folger, Robert A. Baron, and Martin Schulz. 2003. The impact of community violence and an organization’s procedural justice climate on workplace aggression. Academy of Management Journal 46: 317–26. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Dufresne, Ronald L. 2004. An action learning perspective on effective implementation of academic honor codes. Group & Organization Management 29: 201–18. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Duggan, Patrick S., Gail Geller, Lisa A. Cooper, and Mary Catherine Beach. 2006. The moral nature of patient-centeredness: Is it “just the right thing to do”. Patient Education and Counseling 62: 271–6. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Ellemers, Naomi, Stefano Pagliaro, Manuela Barreto, and Colin Wayne Leach. 2008. Is it better to be moral than smart? The effects of morality and competence norms on the decision to work at group status improvement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95: 1397–410. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Engelen, Bart. 2019. Ethical criteria for health-promoting nudges: A case-by-case analysis. The American Journal of Bioethics 19: 48–59. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Erokhin, Vasilii, Kamel Mouloudj, Ahmed C. Bouarar, Smail Mouloudj, and Tianming Gao. 2024. Investigating Farmers’ Intentions to Reduce Water Waste through Water-Smart Farming Technologies. Sustainability 16: 4638. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Fan, Guanhua, Zhenhua Lin, Yizhen Luo, Maohuai Chen, and Liping Li. 2019. Role of community health service programs in navigating the medical ethical slippery slope—A 10-year retrospective study among medical students from southern China. BMC Medical Education 19: 240. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ferreira, Karine Araújo, Larissa Almeida Flávio, and Lasara Fabrícia Rodrigues. 2018. Postponement: Bibliometric analysis and systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Logistics Systems and Management 30: 69–94. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ferrell, Odies C., and Larry G. Gresham. 1985. A contingency framework for understanding ethical decision making in marketing. Journal of Marketing 49: 87–96. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Field, R. H. George, and Michael A. Abelson. 1982. Climate: A reconceptualization and proposed model. Human Relations 35: 181–201. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Fishbein, Martin, and Icek Ajzen. 1977. Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Philosophy and Rhetoric 10: 130–32. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fornell, Claes, and David F. Larcker. 1981. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research 18: 39–50. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Frederick, William C. 1960. The growing concern over business responsibility. California Management Review 2: 54–61. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Friend, Scott B., Fernando Jaramillo, and Jeff S. Johnson. 2020. Ethical climate at the frontline: A meta-analytic evaluation. Journal of Service Research 23: 116–38. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Fry, Louis W. 2003. Toward a theory of spiritual leadership. The Leadership Quarterly 14: 693–727. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Gamarra, María Pilar, and Michele Girotto. 2022. Ethical behavior in leadership: A bibliometric review of the last three decades. Ethics & Behavior 32: 124–46. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Gatersleben, Birgitta, Niamh Murtagh, Megan Cherry, and Megan Watkins. 2019. Moral, wasteful, frugal, or thrifty? Identifying consumer identities to understand and manage pro-environmental behavior. Environment and Behavior 51: 24–49. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Goebel, Sebastian, and Barbara E. Weißenberger. 2017. The relationship between informal controls, ethical work climates, and organizational performance. Journal of Business Ethics 141: 505–28. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Graham, Melody A. 1994. Cheating at small colleges: An examination of student and faculty attitudes and behaviors. Journal of College Student Development 35: 255–60. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gram-Hanssen, Kirsten. 2021. Conceptualising ethical consumption within theories of practice. Journal of Consumer Culture 21: 432–49. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Greene, Joshua D., Brian Sommerville, Leigh E. Nystrom, John M. Darley, and Jonathan D. Cohen. 2001. An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293: 2105–8. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Greenleaf, Robert K. 2002. Servant Leadership: A Journey into the Nature of Legitimate Power and Greatness . Mahwah: Paulist Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gregory-Smith, Diana, Danae Manika, and Pelin Demirel. 2017. Green intentions under the blue flag: Exploring differences in EU consumers’ willingness to pay more for environmentally-friendly products. Business Ethics: A European Review 26: 205–22. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Grobler, Sonja, and Anton Grobler. 2021. Ethical leadership, person-organizational fit, and productive energy: A South African sectoral comparative study. Ethics & Behavior 31: 21–37. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Haidt, Jonathan. 2001. The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review 108: 814–34. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Haines, Russell, and Lori N. K. Leonard. 2007. Individual characteristics and ethical decision-making in an IT context. Industrial Management & Data Systems 107: 5–20. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Harding, Trevor S., Matthew J. Mayhew, Cynthia J. Finelli, and Donald D. Carpenter. 2007. The Theory of Planned Behavior as a Model of Academic Dishonesty in Engineering and Humanities Undergraduates. Ethics & Behavior 17: 255–79. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Harrison, Rob, Terry Newholm, and Deirdre Shaw. 2005. The Ethical Consumer . New York: SAGE Publications. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Harvey, Paul, Mark J. Martinko, and Nancy Borkowski. 2017. Justifying deviant behavior: The role of attributions and moral emotions. Journal of Business Ethics 141: 779–95. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hassan, Louise M., Edward Shiu, and Deirdre Shaw. 2016. Who says there is an intention–behaviour gap? Assessing the empirical evidence of an intention–behaviour gap in ethical consumption. Journal of Business Ethics 136: 219–36. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hirth-Goebel, Tabea Franziska, and Barbara E. Weißenberger. 2019. Management accountants and ethical dilemmas: How to promote ethical intention? Journal of Management Control 30: 287–322. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hofmann, David A., and Barbara Mark. 2006. An investigation of the relationship between safety climate and medication errors as well as other nurse and patient outcomes. Personnel Psychology 59: 847–69. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hong, Eun-Sil, and Hyo-Yeon Shin. 2010. Ethical consumption and related variables of college students. Korean Journal of Family Management 13: 1–25. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hong, Yeon Geum, and Insook Song. 2008. A study of cases of ethical consumption in the analysis of purchasing motives of environmentally-friendly agriculture products. Journal of Consumption Culture 11: 23–42. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Huang, Wen-yeh, Ching-Yun Huang, and Alan J. Dubinsky. 2014. The impact of guanxi on ethical perceptions: The case of Taiwanese salespeople. Journal of Business-to-Business Marketing 21: 1–17. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hunt, Shelby D., and Scott Vitell. 1986. A general theory of marketing ethics. Journal of Macromarketing 6: 5–16. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Husser, Jocelyn, Jean-Marc Andre, and Véronique Lespinet-Najib. 2019. The impact of locus of control, moral intensity, and the microsocial ethical environment on purchasing-related ethical reasoning. Journal of Business Ethics 154: 243–61. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Johnson, Kevin J., Joé T. Martineau, Saouré Kouamé, Gokhan Turgut, and Serge Poisson-de-Haro. 2018. On the unethical use of privileged information in strategic decision-making: The effects of peers’ ethicality, perceived cohesion, and team performance. Journal of Business Ethics 152: 917–29. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Johnson, Olivia, and Veena Chattaraman. 2019. Conceptualization and measurement of millennial’s social signaling and self-signaling for socially responsible consumption. Journal of Consumer Behaviour 18: 32–42. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Jones, Thomas M. 1991. Ethical decision making by individuals in organizations: An issue-contingent model. Academy of Management Review 16: 366–95. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kacmar, Kacmar, K. Michele, Daniel G. Bachrach, Kenneth J. Harris, and Suzanne Zivnuska. 2011. Fostering good citizenship through ethical leadership: Exploring the moderating role of gender and organizational politics. Journal of Applied Psychology 96: 633–42. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Kang, Jee-Won, and Young Namkung. 2018. The effect of corporate social responsibility on brand equity and the moderating role of ethical consumerism: The case of Starbucks. Journal of Hospitality & Tourism Research 42: 1130–51. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kanungo, Rabindra N. 2001. Ethical values of transactional and transformational leaders. Canadian Journal of Administrative Sciences 18: 257–65. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kautish, Pradeep, Justin Paul, and Rajesh Sharma. 2019. The moderating influence of environmental consciousness and recycling intentions on green purchase behavior. Journal of Cleaner Production 228: 1425–36. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Keck, Natalija, Steffen R. Giessner, Niels Van Quaquebeke, and Erica Kruijff. 2020. When do followers perceive their leaders as ethical? A relational models perspective of normatively appropriate conduct. Journal of Business Ethics 164: 477–93. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kelly, Paul, Simon J. Marshall, Hannah Badland, Jacqueline Kerr, Melody Oliver, Aiden R. Doherty, and Charlie Foster. 2013. An ethical framework for automated, wearable cameras in health behavior research. American journal of Preventive Medicine 44: 314–19. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kelman, Herbert C., and V. Lee Hamilton. 1989. Crimes of Obedience: Toward a Social Psychology of Authority and Responsibility . New Haven: Yale University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kerse, Gökhan. 2021. A leader indeed is a leader in deed: The relationship of ethical leadership, person–organization fit, organizational trust, and extra-role service behavior. Journal of Management & Organization 27: 601–20. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kim, Hyelin Lina, Yinyoung Rhou, Muzaffer Uysal, and Nakyung Kwon. 2017. An examination of the links between corporate social responsibility (CSR) and its internal consequences. International Journal of Hospitality Management 61: 26–34. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kim, Junghwan, and Mei-Po Kwan. 2021. The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on people’s mobility: A longitudinal study of the US from March to September of 2020. Journal of Transport Geography 93: 103039. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kim, Jungsun Sunny, Hak Jun Song, and Choong-Ki Lee. 2016. Effects of corporate social responsibility and internal marketing on organizational commitment and turnover intentions. International Journal of Hospitality Management 55: 25–32. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kirsch, Roxanne E., Corrine R. Balit, Franco A. Carnevale, Jos M. Latour, and Victor Larcher. 2018. Ethical, cultural, social, and individual considerations prior to transition to limitation or withdrawal of life-sustaining therapies. Pediatric Critical Care Medicine 19: 10–18. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Kirsch, Roxanne, and David Munson. 2018. Ethical and end of life considerations for neonates requiring ECMO support. Seminars in Perinatology 42: 129–37. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Klein, Katherine J., and Joann Speer Sorra. 1996. The challenge of innovation implementation. Academy of Management Review 21: 1055–80. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kohlberg, Lawrence. 1976. Moral stages and moralization: The cognitive-development approach. In Moral Development and Behavior: Theory and Research and Social Issues . Edited by Thomas Lickona. New York: Holt, Rienhart, and Winston, pp. 31–53. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kohlberg, Lawrence. 1981. The Philosophy of Moral Development: Moral Stages and the Idea of Justice. New York: Harper & Row. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Köseoglu, Mehmet Ali, Yasin Sehitoglu, Gary Ross, and John A. Parnell. 2016. The evolution of business ethics research in the realm of tourism and hospitality: A bibliometric analysis. International Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Management 28: 1598–621. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Laratta, Rosario. 2011. Ethical climate and accountability in nonprofit organizations: A comparative study between Japan and the UK. Public Management Review 13: 43–63. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Lawrence, Dana J. 2007. The four principles of biomedical ethics: A foundation for current bioethical debate. Journal of Chiropractic Humanities 14: 34–40. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Leung, Xi Y., Jie Sun, and Billy Bai. 2017. Bibliometrics of social media research: A co-citation and co-word analysis. International Journal of Hospitality Management 66: 35–45. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Lewis, Phillip V. 1985. Defining ‘business ethics’: Like nailing jello to a wall. Journal of Business Ethics 4: 377–83. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Liao, Hui, and Deborah E. Rupp. 2005. The impact of justice climate and justice orientation on work outcomes: A cross-level multifoci framework. Journal of Applied Psychology 90: 242–56. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Liu, Yongdan, Matthew Tingchi Liu, Andrea Pérez, Wilco Chan, Jesús Collado, and Ziying Mo. 2020. The importance of knowledge and trust for ethical fashion consumption. Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics 33: 1154–94. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Luthans, Fred, and Bruce J. Avolio. 2003. Authentic leadership: A positive developmental approach. In Positive Organizational Scholarship . Edited by Kim S. Cameron, Jane E. Dutton and Robert E. Quinn. San Francisco: Barrett-Koehler, pp. 241–61. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Machlup, Fritz, and Una Mansfield. 1983. Cultural Diversity in Studies of Information. In The Study of Information: Interdisciplinary Messages . Edited by F. Machlup and U. Mansfield. New York: John Wiley and Sons, pp. 3–59. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Martin, Sean R., Jennifer J. Kish-Gephart, and James R. Detert. 2014. Blind forces: Ethical infrastructures and moral disengagement in organizations. Organizational Psychology Review 4: 295–325. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Mas-Tur, Alicia, Norat Roig-Tierno, Shikhar Sarin, Christophe Haon, Trina Sego, Mustapha Belkhouja, Alan Porter, and José M. Merigó. 2021. Co-citation, bibliographic coupling and leading authors, institutions and countries in the 50 years of Technological Forecasting and Social Change. Technological Forecasting and Social Change 165: 120487. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • May, Douglas R., Young K. Chang, and Ruodan Shao. 2015. Does ethical membership matter? Moral identification and its organizational implications. Journal of Applied Psychology 100: 681–94. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Mayer, David M. 2014. A review of the literature on ethical climate and culture. In The Oxford Handbook of Organizational Climate and Culture . Edited by Benjamin Schneider and Karen M. Barbera. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 415–40. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Mayer, David M., Maribeth Kuenzi, Rebecca Greenbaum, Mary Bardes, and Rommel (Bombie) Salvador. 2009. How low does ethical leadership flow? Test of a trickle-down model. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 108: 1–13. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Mazar, Nina, On Amir, and Dan Ariely. 2008. The dishonesty of honest people: A theory of self-concept maintenance. Journal of Marketing Research 45: 633–44. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • McCain, Katherine W. 1990. Mapping authors in intellectual space: A technical overview. Journal of the American Society for Information Science (1986–1996) 41: 433–43. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • McGuire, Joseph W. 1963. Factors Affecting the Growth of Manufacturing Firms . Seattle: University of Washington, Bureau of Business Research. [ Google Scholar ]
  • McKay, Patrick F., Derek R. Avery, and Mark A. Morris. 2009. “A tale of two climates: Diversity climate from subordinates” and managers’ perspectives and their role in store unit sales performance. Personnel Psychology 62: 767–91. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • McMurtry, Kim. 2001. E-cheating: Combating a 21st Century challenge. T.H.E. Journal 29: 36–38. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mihelič, Katarina Katja, and Barbara Culiberg. 2014. Turning a blind eye: A study of peer reporting in a business school setting. Ethics & Behavior 24: 364–81. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Mitchell, Marie S., Scott J. Reynolds, and Linda K. Treviño. 2020. The study of behavioral ethics within organizations: A special issue introduction. Personnel Psychology 73: 5–17. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Mohi Ud Din, Qaiser, and Li Zhang. 2023. Unveiling the mechanisms through which leader integrity shapes ethical leadership behavior: Theory of planned behavior perspective. Behavioral Sciences 13: 928. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Moore, Celia, David M. Mayer, Flora F. T. Chiang, Craig Crossley, Matthew J. Karlesky, and Thomas A. Birtch. 2019. Leaders matter morally: The role of ethical leadership in shaping employee moral cognition and misconduct. Journal of Applied Psychology 104: 123–45. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Mouloudj, Kamel, and Ahmed Chemseddine Bouarar. 2023. Investigating predictors of medical students’ intentions to engagement in volunteering during the health crisis. African Journal of Economic and Management Studies 14: 205–22. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Mouloudj, Kamel, Anuli Njoku, Dachel Martínez Asanza, Ahmed Chemseddine Bouarar, Marian A. Evans, Smail Mouloudj, and Achouak Bouarar. 2023. Modeling Predictors of Medication Waste Reduction Intention in Algeria: Extending the Theory of Planned Behavior. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 20: 6584. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Mulki, Jay Prakash, Fernando Jaramillo, Shavin Malhotra, and William B. Locander. 2012. Reluctant employees and felt stress: The moderating impact of manager decisiveness. Journal of Business Research 65: 77–83. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Mumford, Michael D., Ginamarie M Scott, Blaine Gaddis, and Jill M Strange. 2002. Leading creative people: Orchestrating expertise and relationships. The Leadership Quarterly 13: 705–50. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Murrell, Vicki S. 2014. The failure of medical education to develop moral reasoning in medical students. International Journal of Medical Education 5: 219–25. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Nathanson, Craig, Delroy L. Paulhus, and Kevin M. Williams. 2006. Predictors of a behavioral measure of scholastic cheating: Personality and competence but not demographics. Contemporary Educational Psychology 31: 97–122. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Nga, Joyce K. H., and Evelyn W. S. Lum. 2013. An investigation into unethical behavior intentions among undergraduate students: A Malaysian study. Journal of Academic Ethics 11: 45–71. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Njoku, Anuli, Kamel Mouloudj, Ahmed Chemseddine Bouarar, Marian A. Evans, Dachel Martínez Asanza, Smail Mouloudj, and Achouak Bouarar. 2024. Intentions to create green start-ups for collection of unwanted drugs: An empirical study. Sustainability 16: 2797. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Noh, Yoon Goo, and Se Young Kim. 2024. Factors of hospital ethical climate among hospital nurses in Korea: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Healthcare 12: 372. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Onesti, Gianni. 2023. Exploring the impact of leadership styles, ethical behavior, and organizational identification on workers’ well-being. Administrative Sciences 13: 149. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Orellano, Anabel, Carmen Valor, and Emilio Chuvieco. 2020. The Influence of Religion on Sustainable Consumption: A Systematic Review and Future Research Agenda. Sustainability 12: 7901. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Overall, Jeffrey, and Steven Gedeon. 2023. Rational Egoism virtue-based ethical beliefs and subjective happiness: An empirical investigation. Philosophy of Management 22: 51–72. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Pan, Yue, and John R. Sparks. 2012. Predictors, consequence, and measurement of ethical judgments: Review and meta-analysis. Journal of Business Research 65: 84–91. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Pepper, Miriam, Tim Jackson, and David Uzzell. 2009. An examination of the values that motivate socially conscious and frugal consumer behaviours. International Journal of Consumer Studies 33: 126–36. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Petrovskaya, Irina, and Fazli Haleem. 2021. Socially responsible consumption in Russia: Testing the theory of planned behavior and the moderating role of trust. Business Ethics, the Environment & Responsibility 30: 38–53. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Piccolo, Ronald F., Rebecca Greenbaum, Deanne N. den Hartog, and Robert Folger. 2010. The relationship between ethical leadership and core job characteristics. Journal of Organizational Behavior 31: 259–78. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Podsakoff, Philip M., Scott B. MacKenzie, Jeong-Yeon Lee, and Nathan P. Podsakoff. 2003. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology 88: 879–903. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Potter, William Gray. 1981. Introduction to bibliometrics. Library Trends 30: 5–7. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rasool, Shahid, Roberto Cerchione, and Jari Salo. 2020. Assessing ethical consumer behavior for sustainable development: The mediating role of brand attachment. Sustainable Development 28: 1620–31. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Reave, Laura. 2005. Spiritual values and practices related to leadership effectiveness. The Leadership Quarterly 16: 655–87. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Resick, Christian J., Gillian S. Martin, Mary A. Keating, Marcus W. Dickson, Ho Kwong Kwan, and Chunyan Peng. 2011. What ethical leadership means to me: Asian, American, and European perspectives. Journal of Business Ethics 101: 435–57. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Rest, James R. 1986. Moral Development: Advances in Research and Theory . New York: Praeger. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rey-Martí, Andrea, Domingo Ribeiro-Soriano, and Daniel Palacios-Marqués. 2016. A bibliometric analysis of social entrepreneurship. Journal of Business Research 69: 1651–55. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Richardson, Hettie A., and Robert J. Vandenberg. 2005. Integrating managerial perceptions and transformational leadership into a work-unit level model of employee involvement. Journal of Organizational Behavior 26: 561–89. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Roberts, James A. 1993. Sex differences in socially responsible consumers’ behavior. Psychological Reports 73: 139–48. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Robertson, Christopher J. 2008. An analysis of 10 years of business ethics research in strategic management journal: 1996–2005. Journal of Business Ethics 80: 745–53. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Rodriguez-Rad, Carlos. J., and Encarnacion Ramos-Hidalgo. 2018. Spirituality, consumer ethics, and sustainability: The mediating role of moral identity. Journal of Consumer Marketing 35: 51–63. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Romani, Simona, Silvia Grappi, and Richard P. Bagozzi. 2016. Corporate socially responsible initiatives and their effects on consumption of green products. Journal of Business Ethics 135: 253–64. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ross, Michael W. 2005. Typing, doing, and being: Sexuality and the Internet. Journal of Sex Research 42: 342–52. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Roy, Achinto, Alexander Newman, Heather Round, and Sukanto Bhattacharya. 2024. Ethical culture in organizations: A review and agenda for future research. Business Ethics Quarterly 34: 97–138. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Runes, Dagobert D. 1964. Dictionary of Philosophy . Patterson: Littlefields, Adams and Co. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Saini, Supreet. 2013. Academic ethics at the undergraduate level: Case study from the formative years of the institute. Journal of Academic Ethics 11: 35–44. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Sánchez-González, Irene, Irene Gil-Saura, and María Eugenia Ruiz-Molina. 2020. Ethically Minded Consumer Behavior, Retailers’ Commitment to Sustainable Development, and Store Equity in Hypermarkets. Sustainability 12: 8041. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Schminke, Marshall, Anke Arnaud, and Maribeth Kuenzi. 2007. The power of ethical work climates. Organizational Dynamics 36: 171–86. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Schwepker, Charles H., Jr. 2001. Ethical climate’s relationship to job satisfaction, organizational commitment, and turnover intention in the salesforce. Journal of Business Research 54: 39–52. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Şendağ, Serkan, Mesut Duran, and M. Robert Fraser. 2012. Surveying the extent of involvement in online academic dishonesty (e-dishonesty) related practices among university students and the rationale students provide: One university’s experience. Computers in Human Behavior 28: 849–60. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Shapira-Lishchinsky, Orly, and Zehava Rosenblatt. 2010. School ethical climate and teachers’ voluntary absence. Journal of Educational Administration 48: 164–81. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Singhapakdi, Anusorn. 1993. Ethical perceptions of marketers: The interaction effects of Machiavellianism and organizational ethical culture. Journal of Business Ethics 12: 407–18. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Smith, Samantha, and Angela Paladino. 2010. Eating clean and green? Investigating consumer motivations towards the purchase of organic food. Australasian Marketing Journal 18: 93–104. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Snider, Jamie, Ronald Paul Hill, and Diane Martin. 2003. Corporate social responsibility in the 21st century: A view from the world’s most successful firms. Journal of Business Ethics 48: 175–87. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Soler-Costa, Rebeca, Pablo Lafarga-Ostáriz, Marta Mauri-Medrano, and Antonio-José Moreno-Guerrero. 2021. Netiquette: Ethic, Education, and Behavior on Internet—A Systematic Literature Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18: 1212. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Soltani, Bahram. 2014. The anatomy of corporate fraud: A comparative analysis of high profile American and European corporate scandals. Journal of Business Ethics 120: 251–74. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Starr, Martha A. 2009. The social economics of ethical consumption: Theoretical considerations and empirical evidence. The Journal of Socio-Economics 38: 916–25. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Stern, Paul C., Thomas Dietz, and Gregory A. Guagnano. 1995. The new ecological paradigm in social-psychological context. Environment and Behavior 27: 723–43. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Su, Hsin-Ning, and Pei-Chun Lee. 2010. Mapping knowledge structure by keyword co-occurrence: A first look at journal papers in Technology Foresight. Scientometrics 85: 65–79. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Sumanth, John J., and Sean T. Hannah. 2014. An Integration and Exploration of Ethical and Authentic Leadership Antecedents. In Advances in Authentic and Ethical Leadership . Edited by Linda L. Neider and Chester A. Schriesheim. Charlotte: Information Age Publishing, pp. 25–74. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tafolli, Festim, and Sonja Grabner-Kräuter. 2020. Employee perceptions of corporate social responsibility and organizational corruption: Empirical evidence from Kosovo. Corporate Governance 20: 1349–70. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Tenbrunsel, Ann E., and Kristin Smith-Crowe. 2008. 13 ethical decision making: Where we’ve been and where we’re going. Academy of Management Annals 2: 545–607. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Treviño, Linda Klebe. 1986. Ethical decision making in organizations: A person-situation interactionist model. Academy of Management Review 11: 601–17. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Treviño, Linda Klebe, and Gary R. Weaver. 2001. Organizational justice and ethics program “follow-through”: Influences on employees’ harmful and helpful behavior. Business Ethics Quarterly 11: 651–71. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Treviño, Linda Klebe, and Katherine A. Nelson. 2021. Managing Business Ethics: Straight Talk about How to Do it Right . Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Treviño, Linda Klebe, and Michael Brown. 2004. Managing to be ethical: Debunking five business ethics myths. Academy of Management Perspectives 18: 69–81. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Treviño, Linda Klebe, and Stuart A. Youngblood. 1990. Bad apples in bad barrels: A causal analysis of ethical decision-making behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology 75: 378–85. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Treviño, Linda Klebe, Gary R. Weaver, and Scott J. Reynolds. 2006. Behavioral ethics in organizations: A review. Journal of Management 32: 951–90. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Treviño, Linda Klebe, Kenneth D. Butterfield, and Donald L. McCabe. 1998. The ethical context in organizations: Influences on employee attitudes and behaviors. Business Ethics Quarterly 8: 447–76. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Treviño, Linda Klebe, Laura Pincus Hartman, and Michael Brown. 2000. Moral person and moral manager: How executives develop a reputation for ethical leadership. California Management Review 42: 128–42. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Treviño, Linda Klebe, Michael Brown, and Laura Pincus Hartman. 2003. A qualitative investigation of perceived executive ethical leadership: Perceptions from inside and outside the executive suite. Human Relations 56: 5–37. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Treviño, Linda Klebe, Niki A. Den Nieuwenboer, and Jennifer J. Kish-Gephart. 2014. (Un) ethical behavior in organizations. Annual Review of Psychology 65: 635–60. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Tsai, Feng-Chiao, Yu-Tsung Huang, Luan-Yin Chang, and Jin-Town Wang. 2008. Pyogenic liver abscess as endemic disease, Taiwan. Emerging Infectious Diseases 14: 1592–600. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Tsalikis, John, and David J. Fritzsche. 2013. Business Ethics: A Literature Review with a Focus on Marketing Ethics. In Citation Classics from the Journal of Business Ethics . Edited by Alex C. Michalos and Deborah C. Poff. Advances in Business Ethics Research. Dordrecht: Springer, vol. 2, pp. 337–404. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ülkü, Tugay, and Musa Said Döven. 2021. The Moderator role of ethical climate upon the effect between health personnel’s Machiavellian tendencies and whistleblowing intention: The case of Eskişehir. Is AhlakıDergisi 14: 125–66. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Underwood, Jean, and Attila Szabo. 2003. Academic offences and e-learning: Individual propensities in cheating. British Journal of Educational Technology 34: 467–77. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Uusitalo, Outi, and Reetta Oksanen. 2004. Ethical consumerism: A view from Finland. International Journal of Consumer Studies 28: 214–21. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Valentine, Sean, and Tim Barnett. 2007. Perceived organizational ethics and the ethical decisions of sales and marketing personnel. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management 27: 373–88. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Val-Laillet, David, Esther Aarts, Bernhard M. Weber, Marco M. D. Ferrari, Valentina Quaresima, Luke Edward Stoeckel, Miguel Alonso-Alonso, Michel Albert Audette, Charles Henri Malbert, and Eric M. Stice. 2015. Neuroimaging and neuromodulation approaches to study eating behavior and prevent and treat eating disorders and obesity. NeuroImage: Clinical 8: 1–31. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Vallaster, Christine, Sascha Kraus, José M. Merigó Lindahl, and Annika Nielsen. 2019. Ethics and entrepreneurship: A bibliometric study and literature review. Journal of Business Research 99: 226–37. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Van Eck, Nees, and Ludo Waltman. 2010. Software survey: VOSviewer, a computer program for bibliometric mapping. Scientometrics 84: 523–38. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Van Quaquebeke, Niels, Jan U. Becker, Niko Goretzki, and Christian Barrot. 2019. Perceived ethical leadership affects customer purchasing intentions beyond ethical marketing in advertising due to moral identity self-congruence concerns. Journal of Business Ethics 156: 357–76. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Vardaman, James M., Maria B. Gondo, and David G. Allen. 2014. Ethical climate and pro-social rule breaking in the workplace. Human Resource Management Review 24: 108–18. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Victor, Bart, and John B. Cullen. 1988. The organizational bases of ethical work climates. Administrative Science Quarterly 33: 101–25. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Vitell, Scott J., Robert Allen King, Katharine Howie, Jean-François Toti, Lumina Albert, Encarnación Ramos Hidalgo, and Omneya Yacout. 2016. Spirituality, moral identity, and consumer ethics: A multi-cultural study. Journal of Business Ethics 139: 147–60. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Vošner, Helena Blažun, Samo Bobek, Simona Sternad Zabukovšek, and Peter Kokol. 2017. Openness and information technology: A bibliometric analysis of literature production. Kybernetes 46: 750–66. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Walker, Margaret Urban. 2007. Moral Understandings: A Feminist Study in Ethics . New York: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wang, Yau-De, and Hui-Hsien Hsieh. 2012. Toward a better understanding of the link between ethical climate and job satisfaction: A multilevel analysis. Journal of Business Ethics 105: 535–45. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Wartick, Steven L., and Philip L. Cochran. 1985. The evolution of the corporate social performance model. Academy of ManagementReview 10: 758–69. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Wiegmann, Alex, and Michael R. Waldmann. 2014. Transfer effects between moral dilemmas: A causal model theory. Cognition 131: 28–43. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Wood, Donna J. 1991. Corporate social performance revisited. Academy of Management Review 16: 691–718. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Yadav, Rambalak. 2016. Altruistic or egoistic: Which value promotes organic food consumption among young consumers? A study in the context of a developing nation. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 33: 92–97. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Yardley, Jennifer, Melanie Domenech Rodríguez, Scott C. Bates, and Johnathan Nelson. 2009. True confessions?: Alumni’s retrospective reports on undergraduate cheating behaviors. Ethics & Behavior 19: 1–14. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ye, Nan, Tung-Boon Kueh, Lisong Hou, Yongxin Liu, and Hang Yu. 2020. A bibliometric analysis of corporate social responsibility in sustainable development. Journal of Cleaner Production 272: 122679. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Yukl, Gary. 2006. Leadership in Organizations , 6th ed. Upper Saddle River: Pearson Prentice Hall. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zandi, GholamReza, Imran Shahzad, Muhammad Farrukh, and Sebastian Kot. 2020. Supporting Role of Society and Firms to COVID-19 Management among Medical Practitioners. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17: 7961. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Zemigala, Marcin. 2019. Tendencies in research on sustainable development in management sciences. Journal of Cleaner Production 218: 796–809. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zhang, Guangxi, Jianan Zhong, and Muammer Ozer. 2020. Status threat and ethical leadership: A power-dependence perspective. Journal of Business Ethics 161: 665–85. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zhang, Qian, Bee Lan Oo, and Benson Teck Heng Lim. 2019. Drivers, motivations, and barriers to the implementation of corporate social responsibility practices by construction enterprises: A review. Journal of Cleaner Production 210: 563–84. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zohar, Dov. 2010. Thirty years of safety climate research: Reflections and future directions. Accident Analysis & Prevention 42: 1517–22. [ Google Scholar ]

Click here to enlarge figure

ObjectivesMethod
CountryBibliographic coupling
KeywordCo-occurrence
PublicationBibliographic coupling and Co-citation
JournalBibliographic coupling and Co-citation
AuthorBibliographic coupling and Co-citation
Cluster (Number of Keywords)The Theme of Research about Ethical Behavior in the ContextContextKeywords
1 (146)Concerns about health problemsMedicalCare; health; depression; cancer; medicine; stress; quality-of-life; risk; burnout; children; COVID-19; vulnerability; care; human-rights; psychology, life, family; HIV; suicide; bioethics; health-care; nurse
2 (75)Management work of leadersLeadershipPerformance; ethical leadership; model; ethical decision-making; job-satisfaction; ethical climate; employee voice; work; transformational leadership; abusive supervision
3 (54)Consumer behavior toward products of a socially responsible firmConsumption Corporate social-responsibility; corporate social responsibility; planned behavior; consumers; intentions; consumption; green; consumer behavior; product; welfare; welfare animal; responsibility; sustainability
4 (51)Understand the process of making an ethical decisionEthical decisionmaking Ethics; judgment; decision making; power; empathy; morality; emotion; dilemmas; psychologists, dynamics, intuition, negotiation, willingness
5 (37)Student’s behavior in educationAcademic Education; students; organization; managers; depletion; misconduct; integrity; cheating; academic dishonesty; unethical behavior
6 (30)Activities in corporate (business, management)Corporate Behavior; business ethics; codes; management; entrepreneurship; work climate; financial performance; human resource management; stakeholder theory
7 (23)The concept of factors mentioned when marketingMarketing Marketing ethics; consumer ethics; religiosity; collectivism; decision-making; idealism; social responsibility; culture; strategy
8 (6)Spirituality and virtue affect ethical behavior in Indian firmsSpiritual Firms; India; philosophy; spirituality; virtue; workplace spirituality
Cluster Representative Publications
Cluster 1 (435 publications)
Medical Context
( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( )
Cluster 2 (131 Publications)
Ethical Behavior in Consumption
( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( )
Cluster 3 (129 Publications)
Moral Development, Ethical Perception, Moral Judgment, and Ethical Decision Making
( ); ( ); ( ); ( )
Cluster 4 (119 Publications)
Ethical Behavior in Leadership
( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( )
Cluster 5 (78 Publications)
Ethical Behavior in Business: Corporate Social Responsibility
( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( )
Cluster 6 (64 Publications)
(Un)Ethical Behavior in Organizational Context
( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( )
Cluster 7 (27 Publications)
(Un)Ethical Behavior in Educational Context
( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( )
Cluster 8 (16 Publications)
Ethical Climate in Organizational Context
( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( )
JournalCountryPublicationsSJR 2021Quartile
Journal of Business Ethics (1982)Netherlands1432.44Q1
Journal of Applied Psychology (1917)UK246.45Q1
Ethics and Behavior (1991)USA170.44Q2
Sustainability (2009)Switzerland170.66Q1
Science and Engineering Ethics (1995)Netherlands151.07Q1
Frontiers in Psychology (2010)Switzerland100.87Q1
Academic Medicine (1964)USA101.66Q1
Business Ethics Quarterly (1996)UK91.54Q1
Journal of Business Research (1973)USA92.32Q1
Personnel Review (1971)UK50.89Q2
Business Ethics (1992)UK50.93Q1
Cluster Representative Research
Cluster 1 (37 publications)
Ethical Decision Making
( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( )
Cluster 2 (34 publications)
Ethical Leadership
( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( )
Cluster 3 (23 publications)
Ethical Judgment, Moral Development, and Ethical Behavior in an Organization
( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( ); ( )
Cluster 4 (6 publications)
Ethical Climate
( ); ( ); ( )
JournalCountryCitationSJR 2021Quartile
Journal of Business Ethics (1982)Netherlands47752.44Q1
Journal of Applied Psychology (1917)USA13266.45Q1
Academy of Management Review (1978)USA10067.62Q1
Academy of Management Journal (1975)USA90810.87Q1
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology (1965)USA8953.7Q1
Leadership Quarterly (1990)USA6394.91Q1
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes (1985)USA5772.83Q1
Journal of Business Research (1973)USA5382.32Q1
Journal of Management (1975)USA5252.12Q1
Journal of Marketing (1969)USA5227.46Q1
Science (1880)USA37514.59Q1
Business Ethics (1992)UK3580.93Q1
A. Bibliographic Coupling AnalysisB. Co-Citation AnalysisC. Key Context
Cluster 2 (131 Publications)
Ethical Behavior in Consumption
Cluster 1 (37 publications) Ethical Decision MakingConsumption
Cluster 4 (119 Publications)
Ethical Behavior in Leadership
Cluster 2 (34 publications) Ethical LeadershipLeadership
Cluster 3 (129 Publications)
Moral Development, Ethical Perception, Moral Judgment, and Ethical Decision Making
Cluster 3 (23 publications) Ethical Judgment, Moral Development, and Ethical Behavior in OrganizationsBusiness
Cluster 5 (78 Publications)
Ethical Behavior in Business: Corporate Social Responsibility
Cluster 6 (64 Publications)
(Un)Ethical Behavior in Organizational Contexts
Cluster 4 (6 publications) Ethical ClimateOrganization
Cluster 8 (16 Publications)
Ethical Climate in Organizational Contexts
Cluster 1 (435 publications)
Medical Contexts
Medical
Cluster 7 (27 Publications)
(Un)Ethical Behavior in Educational Contexts
Education
Main ConceptExplanationAuthors
Altruistic consumptionCustomers choose forms of consumption that are not environmentally friendly ( ); ( )
Exchanging behaviorUsing the ethical values of the exchange product ( ); ( )
Fair trade (FT) practiceThese include (1) willingness to pay more, (2) guidance by universalism, benevolence, self-direction and stimulation, (3) self-identity, (4) emphasis on brand fair trade in products, and (5) cultural influences ( ); ( )
Frugal consumptionCustomers are less interested in shopping, more physical repair and product reuse, longer product life ( ); ( )
Green consumptionCustomers drive communities and practices at the national level, which forces manufacturers to adhere to environmentally friendly products ( ); ( )
Socially conscious consumption behaviorConsider equity between environmental issues (e.g., use of used products), health (e.g., building low-waste communities) and social issues (e.g., donate unused products) ( ); ( )
Socially responsible consumption behaviorThese include buying behavior (e.g., buying used products), non-buying behavior (e.g., discouraging purchasing products using raw materials), and post-purchase behavior (e.g., sell fully functional used products at lower market prices) ( ); ( )
Spiritual and moral consumptionConsumer spiritual practices promote ethical consumption ( ); ( )
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

Vu Lan Oanh, L.; Tettamanzi, P.; Tien Minh, D.; Comoli, M.; Mouloudj, K.; Murgolo, M.; Dang Thu Hien, M. How Ethical Behavior Is Considered in Different Contexts: A Bibliometric Analysis of Global Research Trends. Adm. Sci. 2024 , 14 , 200. https://doi.org/10.3390/admsci14090200

Vu Lan Oanh L, Tettamanzi P, Tien Minh D, Comoli M, Mouloudj K, Murgolo M, Dang Thu Hien M. How Ethical Behavior Is Considered in Different Contexts: A Bibliometric Analysis of Global Research Trends. Administrative Sciences . 2024; 14(9):200. https://doi.org/10.3390/admsci14090200

Vu Lan Oanh, Le, Patrizia Tettamanzi, Dinh Tien Minh, Maurizio Comoli, Kamel Mouloudj, Michael Murgolo, and Mai Dang Thu Hien. 2024. "How Ethical Behavior Is Considered in Different Contexts: A Bibliometric Analysis of Global Research Trends" Administrative Sciences 14, no. 9: 200. https://doi.org/10.3390/admsci14090200

Article Metrics

Article access statistics, further information, mdpi initiatives, follow mdpi.

MDPI

Subscribe to receive issue release notifications and newsletters from MDPI journals

  • Do Not Sell My Personal Info

Register For Free

  •  ⋅ 
  • Content Strategy

11 Ways To Do SEO Content Research Beyond Competitor Analysis

Take your SEO strategy to the next level with content research beyond competitor analysis. Discover how to develop unique niche strategies for long-term success.

research on metaphor analysis

Early SEO milestones might be easy, but scaling the results needs an upgraded approach.

What could that look like?

Like startups that come up with a solid niche idea and compete significantly with larger companies, we SEO pros and content strategists need to work harder to develop unique, fresh, niche strategies.

However, whenever we think of creating strategies, we start looking at what competitors are doing . We start feeling that we can win this game by outperforming our competitors.

Remember: we win when our focus is on winning the game and not on how to make our competitors lose.

So, here comes an upgraded approach to our SEO strategy – going beyond competitor analysis.

However, since our SEO strategies heavily rely on content, we’ll discuss content research beyond competitor analysis in this blog.

Now, What Is Content Research Beyond Competitor Analysis?

Most of us analyze our competitors to develop content ideas. It’s easy and quick.

But…

What if your competitors are ranking in the top positions but are not serving users’ intent?

What if your competitors might not be yielding enough traffic despite better rankings?

What if your competitors are driving massive organic traffic but not enough conversions?

Also, there may be some competitors that are doing extremely well regarding content KPIs serving SEO growth.

You may feel that if the competitors can achieve such results in one year, you can achieve them in six months by copying their strategies.

But that’s where you limit yourself in growth. Your competitors’ SEO and content teams might also be struggling; who knows?

This is why your content research must go beyond competitor analysis.

In this approach, we don’t look at what content competitors have written.

We don’t want to copy them or repeat their mistakes. We want to work in ways that truly resonate with our target audiences, geographies, business models, and industries.

So, the “ content research beyond competitor analysis” approach helps us bring unique and fresh perspectives to our content research, creating incredible value for our audience and clients and scaling our SEO results extensively.

11 Ways Of Content Research Beyond Competitor Analysis To Scale SEO ROI

We have 11 ways to use this approach. Let’s uncover them one by one with step-by-step processes and examples.

1. Use Semrush

This is our basic step of content research since most of our initial goal is driving organic traffic.

And because Semrush is handy for most of our team members at Missive Digital, we log in immediately to start our content research instead of looking at competitors.

We put seed, actual, long-tail, and more keywords to do our content research, depending on the search volume, keyword difficulty, and search intent.

For example, we have put “diamond jewelry” into Semrush and will add the filters according to our SEO strategy.

Screenshot from Semrush

Another content research feature of Semrush that we use extensively is Topic Research. We choose the content topics based on which ones relate directly or indirectly to our website.

Screenshot from Semrush

2. Use Ahrefs

To do the content research on Ahrefs, we follow the same steps as Semrush, but here, we also use Content Explorer.

We filter based on the Page Traffic and reference domains to identify queries that can bring us traffic and conversions.

Screenshot from Ahrefs 2024

Then, we also examine the frequency of republishing, which gives our team an idea of when to schedule it next for content optimization, considering the performance.

Screenshot from Ahrefs, August 2024

3. Use Google News

While auditing the content, if we realize that the client is already writing a lot of content, we try researching content ideas through Google News.

Also, for some D2C industries like jewelry, the trend also comes from celebrities wearing them – so we keep a close eye on Google News.

research on metaphor analysis

For example, the screenshot below shows a ‘B’ necklace worn by Selena Gomez in reference to her boyfriend.

Screenshot from search for [diamond necklace]

Bingo! Now, we have to discuss with the client’s team for our next content piece.

4. Use People Also Ask, AlsoAsked

Since most B2B IT and SaaS clients are highly technical, we sometimes struggle to understand the topic and create a content strategy.

People Also Ask on Google Search and AlsoAsked.com by Mark Williams-Cook works like a savior during our content research.

Screenshot from search for [kubernetes architecture]

5. Check Google Trends

No matter what industry you are in , you’ve got something or the other trending.

In our SEO industry, SearchGPT is trending.

Google Trends

So it’s worth writing about it to take the early advantage and grab the traffic share.

See, a lot of people are writing about it:

Screenshot from search for [searchgpt]

So, it’s worth constantly watching what’s trending via Google Trends.

6. Hop On ChatGPT Or Gemini

Remember, we are here to do content research on ChatGPT or Gemini, not to choose the titles they suggest.

Here is a sample content research prompt that we have put for a contact center software company on ChatGPT:

Screenshot from ChatGPT

And here are the responses below:

Screenshot from ChatGPT, results for blog posts

Since the topics are not up to the mark considering the audience (“BPO” in this case), based on the above content ideas, we’ll pick up the seed keywords or topics such as:

  • Lessons from a Legacy Contact Center Software Company.
  • The Contact Center Software Market In The BPO Segment.
  • Optimizing Your Contact Center Operations.
  • How to Drive Innovation in Your Customer Support Department?

7. Monitor Social Media

Yes, we are all active on social media, so we can use it for our content research. Still, we are not considering competitors on social media at the moment.

This viral X thread inspired us to write a blog:

X tweet

Similarly, this article on content research was inspired by my recent post on LinkedIn :

LinkedIn

These are examples of self-created social media content that can be turned into blogs.

However, you can keep monitoring the types of content that get the most visibility and engagement on social media – be it LinkedIn, Instagram, X, or any other platform.

Turn them into your blogs or webinars, but don’t forget to mention them since it’s their original content idea.

8. Dive Into Industry-specific Research Studies

The most unique way to research content ideas is to read your industry-specific research studies extensively. And there’s no one way to do it.

For example, for one of the ecommerce consulting companies, we can get various content ideas from HBR’s eCommerce pricing test :

  • Why Should Ecommerce Brands Stop Offering Free Shipping?
  • X Benefits of No Free Shipping or Conditional Shipping.
  • Free Shipping vs. Conditional Shipping.

Don't just give away free shipping: infographic from HBR

In the below study by Broadridge on Digital Transformation , the below can become the topic clusters, and each can have its own spoke-like content topics.

Image from study by Broadridge on Digital Transformation

For example, if we take Unleashing Artificial Intelligence, we can pick up so many topics out of just one graphic:

Priority areas for AI investments

9. Check Industry-Specific Forums/Communities

Most of our clientele includes IT companies, and we have used IT forums and communities like StackOverflow for content research.

For example, we can come up with the below topic clusters when covering Flutter for the non-technical and technical target audiences:

  • Flutter animation widgets.
  • Flutter dependency management.
  • Why add Firebase to your Flutter app?

Stack Overflow

Similarly, there will be many such forums or communities of your client or employers available to peek into for such content ideas, except for competitive analysis.

10. Google site:reddit.com “my topic”

One such unique idea by Kunjal Chawhan is to Google site:reddit.com “my topic ,”  and let’s see what content ideas look like for a couple of topics:

Looking at the above screenshot, below are the topics that we can definitely create:

  • X Most Popular Social Media Platforms for Ecommerce.
  • How to Use Video Podcasts to Drive Ecommerce Sales?
  • How to Boost Ecommerce Sales When Digital Marketing Seems Expensive?

So yes, Kunjal’s way of content research is amazing, and from that, you can similarly Google:

site:“your industry’s leading site” “topic”

For example:

  • site:searchenginejournal.com “ai content”
  • site:quora.com “ai content”
  • site:practicalecommerce.com “sales”

Let’s move on to the last but not the least method of content research, except for looking at competitors.

11. See What Competing Sites Have NOT Covered

Now you might wonder, “Weren’t the above content research ways except for competitors analysis?”

Yes, they are the ways to research content ideas except for what competitors have written.

But here, I’m trying to make a point where you have to see exactly what indirect competing sites are NOT writing about despite targeting the same industry, keyword clusters, and audience.

What is an indirect competing site?

An indirect competing site is a website that ranks for the industry and search queries of your target audience but is not exactly your product/service competitor. This can be a marketplace, publishing site, or product review site.

Let’s take a website, “leadsquared.com,” for indirect competitive analysis and pick the queries that rank after 50th positions and have a keyword difficulty of less than 29.

Pick those queries and search on Google: site:leadsquared.com “sales funnel vs sales pipeline” .

site:leadsquared.com

In short, you can cover the below topics:

  • Sales funnel vs. sales pipeline.
  • Sales funnel vs. marketing funnel.
  • Sales funnel vs. flywheel.

Just ensure these content topics align with your offerings to bring maximum ROI.

How Will Content Research Beyond Competitor Analysis Contribute To SEO Efforts?

When you go beyond competitor analysis for content research, you discover a few benefits:

  • You innovate – With innovative content ideas, you can experiment and build better strategies that can bring unbelievable results. Also, with AI taking the space predominantly, businesses are looking for innovation in their business and marketing. So when you innovate, you may get better attention and even resources.
  • You get niche opportunities – Instead of just focusing on what competitors are doing, you go deeper into understanding your target audience and explore new content ideas that your competitors might have missed. In such scenarios, you get better results since competition is reduced.
  • You create unique, audience-specific content – My LinkedIn post saw great engagement because it resonated with its audience. This opened us to something unique and specific to the pain point of SEOs and content strategists: content ideation to scale SEO results with a not-so-usual approach. Such content helps us build authority in the market, which is essential today to becoming market leaders.
  • You capitalize on emerging trends – Being an early adopter of something has huge potential for success. When you create your content strategy focused on what’s new or trending in your industry before it becomes mainstream, you get the most eyes right from the beginning and even repeat eyes going forward.
  • You build better engagement and loyalty – You can extend beyond blogs, a traditional way of driving SEO results. Videos, whitepapers, case studies, user-generated content, and many more content formats can take the lead in building user engagement and brand loyalty through SEO.
  • You earn backlinks – Yes, such unique content may require less effort to build backlinks since it can earn them.

Stop looking at competitors for content research; try using these fresh and unique ways to drive better content ROI.

Just remember two things: Competitors are not always right, and you are not necessarily required to look upon them when developing your SEO content strategies.

You can copy and paste your competitors’ strategies to achieve certain SEO milestones, but creating history requires an upgraded approach. What say?

More resources: 

  • SEO Competitive Analysis: The Definitive Guide
  • Leveraging Generative AI Tools For SEO
  • Perfectly Optimized Content From Start To Finish

Featured Image: Natalya Kosarevich /Shutterstock

Himani is the Founder of Missive Digital, an organic marketing agency that focuses on enhancing the brand positioning of the ...

Numbers, Facts and Trends Shaping Your World

Read our research on:

Full Topic List

Regions & Countries

  • Publications
  • Our Methods
  • Short Reads
  • Tools & Resources

Read Our Research On:

Key things to know about U.S. election polling in 2024

Conceptual image of an oversized voting ballot box in a large crowd of people with shallow depth of field

Confidence in U.S. public opinion polling was shaken by errors in 2016 and 2020. In both years’ general elections, many polls underestimated the strength of Republican candidates, including Donald Trump. These errors laid bare some real limitations of polling.

In the midterms that followed those elections, polling performed better . But many Americans remain skeptical that it can paint an accurate portrait of the public’s political preferences.

Restoring people’s confidence in polling is an important goal, because robust and independent public polling has a critical role to play in a democratic society. It gathers and publishes information about the well-being of the public and about citizens’ views on major issues. And it provides an important counterweight to people in power, or those seeking power, when they make claims about “what the people want.”

The challenges facing polling are undeniable. In addition to the longstanding issues of rising nonresponse and cost, summer 2024 brought extraordinary events that transformed the presidential race . The good news is that people with deep knowledge of polling are working hard to fix the problems exposed in 2016 and 2020, experimenting with more data sources and interview approaches than ever before. Still, polls are more useful to the public if people have realistic expectations about what surveys can do well – and what they cannot.

With that in mind, here are some key points to know about polling heading into this year’s presidential election.

Probability sampling (or “random sampling”). This refers to a polling method in which survey participants are recruited using random sampling from a database or list that includes nearly everyone in the population. The pollster selects the sample. The survey is not open for anyone who wants to sign up.

Online opt-in polling (or “nonprobability sampling”). These polls are recruited using a variety of methods that are sometimes referred to as “convenience sampling.” Respondents come from a variety of online sources such as ads on social media or search engines, websites offering rewards in exchange for survey participation, or self-enrollment. Unlike surveys with probability samples, people can volunteer to participate in opt-in surveys.

Nonresponse and nonresponse bias. Nonresponse is when someone sampled for a survey does not participate. Nonresponse bias occurs when the pattern of nonresponse leads to error in a poll estimate. For example, college graduates are more likely than those without a degree to participate in surveys, leading to the potential that the share of college graduates in the resulting sample will be too high.

Mode of interview. This refers to the format in which respondents are presented with and respond to survey questions. The most common modes are online, live telephone, text message and paper. Some polls use more than one mode.

Weighting. This is a statistical procedure pollsters perform to make their survey align with the broader population on key characteristics like age, race, etc. For example, if a survey has too many college graduates compared with their share in the population, people without a college degree are “weighted up” to match the proper share.

How are election polls being conducted?

Pollsters are making changes in response to the problems in previous elections. As a result, polling is different today than in 2016. Most U.S. polling organizations that conducted and publicly released national surveys in both 2016 and 2022 (61%) used methods in 2022 that differed from what they used in 2016 . And change has continued since 2022.

A sand chart showing that, as the number of public pollsters in the U.S. has grown, survey methods have become more diverse.

One change is that the number of active polling organizations has grown significantly, indicating that there are fewer barriers to entry into the polling field. The number of organizations that conduct national election polls more than doubled between 2000 and 2022.

This growth has been driven largely by pollsters using inexpensive opt-in sampling methods. But previous Pew Research Center analyses have demonstrated how surveys that use nonprobability sampling may have errors twice as large , on average, as those that use probability sampling.

The second change is that many of the more prominent polling organizations that use probability sampling – including Pew Research Center – have shifted from conducting polls primarily by telephone to using online methods, or some combination of online, mail and telephone. The result is that polling methodologies are far more diverse now than in the past.

(For more about how public opinion polling works, including a chapter on election polls, read our short online course on public opinion polling basics .)

All good polling relies on statistical adjustment called “weighting,” which makes sure that the survey sample aligns with the broader population on key characteristics. Historically, public opinion researchers have adjusted their data using a core set of demographic variables to correct imbalances between the survey sample and the population.

But there is a growing realization among survey researchers that weighting a poll on just a few variables like age, race and gender is insufficient for getting accurate results. Some groups of people – such as older adults and college graduates – are more likely to take surveys, which can lead to errors that are too sizable for a simple three- or four-variable adjustment to work well. Adjusting on more variables produces more accurate results, according to Center studies in 2016 and 2018 .

A number of pollsters have taken this lesson to heart. For example, recent high-quality polls by Gallup and The New York Times/Siena College adjusted on eight and 12 variables, respectively. Our own polls typically adjust on 12 variables . In a perfect world, it wouldn’t be necessary to have that much intervention by the pollster. But the real world of survey research is not perfect.

research on metaphor analysis

Predicting who will vote is critical – and difficult. Preelection polls face one crucial challenge that routine opinion polls do not: determining who of the people surveyed will actually cast a ballot.

Roughly a third of eligible Americans do not vote in presidential elections , despite the enormous attention paid to these contests. Determining who will abstain is difficult because people can’t perfectly predict their future behavior – and because many people feel social pressure to say they’ll vote even if it’s unlikely.

No one knows the profile of voters ahead of Election Day. We can’t know for sure whether young people will turn out in greater numbers than usual, or whether key racial or ethnic groups will do so. This means pollsters are left to make educated guesses about turnout, often using a mix of historical data and current measures of voting enthusiasm. This is very different from routine opinion polls, which mostly do not ask about people’s future intentions.

When major news breaks, a poll’s timing can matter. Public opinion on most issues is remarkably stable, so you don’t necessarily need a recent poll about an issue to get a sense of what people think about it. But dramatic events can and do change public opinion , especially when people are first learning about a new topic. For example, polls this summer saw notable changes in voter attitudes following Joe Biden’s withdrawal from the presidential race. Polls taken immediately after a major event may pick up a shift in public opinion, but those shifts are sometimes short-lived. Polls fielded weeks or months later are what allow us to see whether an event has had a long-term impact on the public’s psyche.

How accurate are polls?

The answer to this question depends on what you want polls to do. Polls are used for all kinds of purposes in addition to showing who’s ahead and who’s behind in a campaign. Fair or not, however, the accuracy of election polling is usually judged by how closely the polls matched the outcome of the election.

A diverging bar chart showing polling errors in U.S. presidential elections.

By this standard, polling in 2016 and 2020 performed poorly. In both years, state polling was characterized by serious errors. National polling did reasonably well in 2016 but faltered in 2020.

In 2020, a post-election review of polling by the American Association for Public Opinion Research (AAPOR) found that “the 2020 polls featured polling error of an unusual magnitude: It was the highest in 40 years for the national popular vote and the highest in at least 20 years for state-level estimates of the vote in presidential, senatorial, and gubernatorial contests.”

How big were the errors? Polls conducted in the last two weeks before the election suggested that Biden’s margin over Trump was nearly twice as large as it ended up being in the final national vote tally.

Errors of this size make it difficult to be confident about who is leading if the election is closely contested, as many U.S. elections are .

Pollsters are rightly working to improve the accuracy of their polls. But even an error of 4 or 5 percentage points isn’t too concerning if the purpose of the poll is to describe whether the public has favorable or unfavorable opinions about candidates , or to show which issues matter to which voters. And on questions that gauge where people stand on issues, we usually want to know broadly where the public stands. We don’t necessarily need to know the precise share of Americans who say, for example, that climate change is mostly caused by human activity. Even judged by its performance in recent elections, polling can still provide a faithful picture of public sentiment on the important issues of the day.

The 2022 midterms saw generally accurate polling, despite a wave of partisan polls predicting a broad Republican victory. In fact, FiveThirtyEight found that “polls were more accurate in 2022 than in any cycle since at least 1998, with almost no bias toward either party.” Moreover, a handful of contrarian polls that predicted a 2022 “red wave” largely washed out when the votes were tallied. In sum, if we focus on polling in the most recent national election, there’s plenty of reason to be encouraged.

Compared with other elections in the past 20 years, polls have been less accurate when Donald Trump is on the ballot. Preelection surveys suffered from large errors – especially at the state level – in 2016 and 2020, when Trump was standing for election. But they performed reasonably well in the 2018 and 2022 midterms, when he was not.

Pew Research Center illustration

During the 2016 campaign, observers speculated about the possibility that Trump supporters might be less willing to express their support to a pollster – a phenomenon sometimes described as the “shy Trump effect.” But a committee of polling experts evaluated five different tests of the “shy Trump” theory and turned up little to no evidence for each one . Later, Pew Research Center and, in a separate test, a researcher from Yale also found little to no evidence in support of the claim.

Instead, two other explanations are more likely. One is about the difficulty of estimating who will turn out to vote. Research has found that Trump is popular among people who tend to sit out midterms but turn out for him in presidential election years. Since pollsters often use past turnout to predict who will vote, it can be difficult to anticipate when irregular voters will actually show up.

The other explanation is that Republicans in the Trump era have become a little less likely than Democrats to participate in polls . Pollsters call this “partisan nonresponse bias.” Surprisingly, polls historically have not shown any particular pattern of favoring one side or the other. The errors that favored Democratic candidates in the past eight years may be a result of the growth of political polarization, along with declining trust among conservatives in news organizations and other institutions that conduct polls.

Whatever the cause, the fact that Trump is again the nominee of the Republican Party means that pollsters must be especially careful to make sure all segments of the population are properly represented in surveys.

The real margin of error is often about double the one reported. A typical election poll sample of about 1,000 people has a margin of sampling error that’s about plus or minus 3 percentage points. That number expresses the uncertainty that results from taking a sample of the population rather than interviewing everyone . Random samples are likely to differ a little from the population just by chance, in the same way that the quality of your hand in a card game varies from one deal to the next.

A table showing that sampling error is not the only kind of polling error.

The problem is that sampling error is not the only kind of error that affects a poll. Those other kinds of error, in fact, can be as large or larger than sampling error. Consequently, the reported margin of error can lead people to think that polls are more accurate than they really are.

There are three other, equally important sources of error in polling: noncoverage error , where not all the target population has a chance of being sampled; nonresponse error, where certain groups of people may be less likely to participate; and measurement error, where people may not properly understand the questions or misreport their opinions. Not only does the margin of error fail to account for those other sources of potential error, putting a number only on sampling error implies to the public that other kinds of error do not exist.

Several recent studies show that the average total error in a poll estimate may be closer to twice as large as that implied by a typical margin of sampling error. This hidden error underscores the fact that polls may not be precise enough to call the winner in a close election.

Other important things to remember

Transparency in how a poll was conducted is associated with better accuracy . The polling industry has several platforms and initiatives aimed at promoting transparency in survey methodology. These include AAPOR’s transparency initiative and the Roper Center archive . Polling organizations that participate in these organizations have less error, on average, than those that don’t participate, an analysis by FiveThirtyEight found .

Participation in these transparency efforts does not guarantee that a poll is rigorous, but it is undoubtedly a positive signal. Transparency in polling means disclosing essential information, including the poll’s sponsor, the data collection firm, where and how participants were selected, modes of interview, field dates, sample size, question wording, and weighting procedures.

There is evidence that when the public is told that a candidate is extremely likely to win, some people may be less likely to vote . Following the 2016 election, many people wondered whether the pervasive forecasts that seemed to all but guarantee a Hillary Clinton victory – two modelers put her chances at 99% – led some would-be voters to conclude that the race was effectively over and that their vote would not make a difference. There is scientific research to back up that claim: A team of researchers found experimental evidence that when people have high confidence that one candidate will win, they are less likely to vote. This helps explain why some polling analysts say elections should be covered using traditional polling estimates and margins of error rather than speculative win probabilities (also known as “probabilistic forecasts”).

National polls tell us what the entire public thinks about the presidential candidates, but the outcome of the election is determined state by state in the Electoral College . The 2000 and 2016 presidential elections demonstrated a difficult truth: The candidate with the largest share of support among all voters in the United States sometimes loses the election. In those two elections, the national popular vote winners (Al Gore and Hillary Clinton) lost the election in the Electoral College (to George W. Bush and Donald Trump). In recent years, analysts have shown that Republican candidates do somewhat better in the Electoral College than in the popular vote because every state gets three electoral votes regardless of population – and many less-populated states are rural and more Republican.

For some, this raises the question: What is the use of national polls if they don’t tell us who is likely to win the presidency? In fact, national polls try to gauge the opinions of all Americans, regardless of whether they live in a battleground state like Pennsylvania, a reliably red state like Idaho or a reliably blue state like Rhode Island. In short, national polls tell us what the entire citizenry is thinking. Polls that focus only on the competitive states run the risk of giving too little attention to the needs and views of the vast majority of Americans who live in uncompetitive states – about 80%.

Fortunately, this is not how most pollsters view the world . As the noted political scientist Sidney Verba explained, “Surveys produce just what democracy is supposed to produce – equal representation of all citizens.”

  • Survey Methods
  • Trust, Facts & Democracy
  • Voter Files

Download Scott Keeter's photo

Scott Keeter is a senior survey advisor at Pew Research Center .

Download Courtney Kennedy's photo

Courtney Kennedy is Vice President of Methods and Innovation at Pew Research Center .

How do people in the U.S. take Pew Research Center surveys, anyway?

How public polling has changed in the 21st century, what 2020’s election poll errors tell us about the accuracy of issue polling, a field guide to polling: election 2020 edition, methods 101: how is polling done around the world, most popular.

901 E St. NW, Suite 300 Washington, DC 20004 USA (+1) 202-419-4300 | Main (+1) 202-857-8562 | Fax (+1) 202-419-4372 |  Media Inquiries

Research Topics

  • Email Newsletters

ABOUT PEW RESEARCH CENTER  Pew Research Center is a nonpartisan fact tank that informs the public about the issues, attitudes and trends shaping the world. It conducts public opinion polling, demographic research, media content analysis and other empirical social science research. Pew Research Center does not take policy positions. It is a subsidiary of  The Pew Charitable Trusts .

© 2024 Pew Research Center

‘This could change everything!’ Nous Research unveils new tool to train powerful AI models with 10,000x efficiency

  • Share on Facebook
  • Share on LinkedIn

Join our daily and weekly newsletters for the latest updates and exclusive content on industry-leading AI coverage. Learn More

Nous Research turned heads earlier this month with the release of its permissive, open-source Llama 3.1 variant Hermes 3 .

Now, the small research team dedicated to making “personalized, unrestricted AI” models has announced another seemingly massive breakthrough: DisTrO (Distributed Training Over-the-Internet), a new optimizer that reduces the amount of information that must be sent between various GPUs (graphics processing units) during each step of training an AI model.

Nous’s DisTrO optimizer means powerful AI models can now be trained outside of big companies, across the open web on consumer-grade connections, potentially by individuals or institutions working together from around the world.

DisTrO has already been tested and shown in a Nous Research technical paper to yield an 857 times efficiency increase compared to one popular existing training algorithm, All-Reduce , as well as a massive reduction in the amount of information transmitted during each step of the training process (86.8 megabytes compared to 74.4 gigabytes) while only suffering a slight loss in overall performance. See the results in the table below from the Nous Research technical paper:

research on metaphor analysis

Ultimately, the DisTrO method could open the door to many more people being able to train massively powerful AI models as they see fit.

As the firm wrote in a post on X yesterday : “Without relying on a single company to manage and control the training process, researchers and institutions can have more freedom to collaborate and experiment with new techniques, algorithms, and models. This increased competition fosters innovation, drives progress, and ultimately benefits society as a whole.”

What if you could use all the computing power in the world to train a shared, open source AI model? Preliminary report: https://t.co/b1XgJylsnV Nous Research is proud to release a preliminary report on DisTrO (Distributed Training Over-the-Internet) a family of… pic.twitter.com/h2gQJ4m7lB — Nous Research (@NousResearch) August 26, 2024

The problem with AI training: steep hardware requirements

As covered on VentureBeat previously, Nvidia’s GPUs in particular are in high demand in the generative AI era, as the expensive graphics cards’ powerful parallel processing capabilities are needed to train AI models efficiently and (relatively) quickly. This blog post at APNic describes the process well.

A big part of the AI training process relies on GPU clusters — multiple GPUs — exchanging information with one another about the model and the information “learned” within training data sets.

However, this “inter-GPU communication” requires that GPU clusters be architected, or set up, in a precise way in controlled conditions, minimizing latency and maximizing throughput. Hence why companies such as Elon Musk’s Tesla are investing heavily in setting up physical “superclusters” with many thousands (or hundreds of thousands) of GPUs sitting physically side-by-side in the same location — typically a massive airplane hangar-sized warehouse or facility.

Because of these requirements, training generative AI — especially the largest and most powerful models — is typically an extremely capital-heavy endeavor, one that only some of the most well-funded companies can engage in, such as Tesla, Meta, OpenAI, Microsoft, Google, and Anthropic.

The training process for each of these companies looks a little different, of course. But they all follow the same basic steps and use the same basic hardware components. Each of these companies tightly controls its own AI model training processes, and it can be difficult for incumbents, much less laypeople outside of them, to even think of competing by training their own similarly-sized (in terms of parameters, or the settings under the hood) models.

But Nous Research, whose whole approach is essentially the opposite — making the most powerful and capable AI it can on the cheap, openly, freely, for anyone to use and customize as they see fit without many guardrails — has found an alternative.

What DisTrO does differently

While traditional methods of AI training require synchronizing full gradients across all GPUs and rely on extremely high bandwidth connections, DisTrO reduces this communication overhead by four to five orders of magnitude.

The paper authors haven’t fully revealed how their algorithms reduce the amount of information at each step of training while retaining overall model performance, but plan to release more on this soon.

The reduction was achieved without relying on amortized analysis or compromising the convergence rate of the training, allowing large-scale models to be trained over much slower internet connections — 100Mbps download and 10Mbps upload, speeds available to many consumers around the world.

The authors tested DisTrO using the Meta Llama 2, 1.2 billion large language model (LLM) architecture and achieved comparable training performance to conventional methods with significantly less communication overhead.

They note that this is the smallest-size model that worked well with the DisTrO method, and they “do not yet know whether the ratio of bandwidth reduction scales up, down, or stays constant as model size increases.”

Yet, the authors also say that “our preliminary tests indicate that it is possible to get a bandwidth requirements reduction of up to 1000x to 3000x during the pre-training,” phase of LLMs, and “for post-training and fine-tuning, we can achieve up to 10000x without any noticeable degradation in loss.”

They further hypothesize that the research, while initially conducted on LLMs, could be used to train large diffusion models (LDMs) as well: think the Stable Diffusion open source image generation model and popular image generation services derived from it such as Midjourney .

Still need good GPUs

To be clear: DisTrO still relies on GPUs — only instead of clustering them all together in the same location, now they can be spread out across the world and communicate over the consumer internet.

Specifically, DisTrO was evaluated using 32x H100 GPUs, operating under the Distributed Data Parallelism (DDP) strategy, where each GPU had the entire model loaded in VRAM .

This setup allowed the team to rigorously test DisTrO’s capabilities and demonstrate that it can match the convergence rates of AdamW+All-Reduce despite drastically reduced communication requirements.

This result suggests that DisTrO can potentially replace existing training methods without sacrificing model quality, offering a scalable and efficient solution for large-scale distributed training.

By reducing the need for high-speed interconnects DisTrO could enable collaborative model training across decentralized networks, even with participants using consumer-grade internet connections.

The report also explores the implications of DisTrO for various applications, including federated learning and decentralized training.

Additionally, DisTrO’s efficiency could help mitigate the environmental impact of AI training by optimizing the use of existing infrastructure and reducing the need for massive data centers.

Moreover, the breakthroughs could lead to a shift in how large-scale models are trained, moving away from centralized, resource-intensive data centers towards more distributed, collaborative approaches that leverage diverse and geographically dispersed computing resources.

What’s next for the Nous Research team and DisTrO?

The research team invites others to join them in exploring the potential of DisTrO. The preliminary report and supporting materials are available on GitHub , and the team is actively seeking collaborators to help refine and expand this groundbreaking technology.

Already, some AI influencers such as @kimmonismus on X (aka chubby) have praised the research as a huge breakthrough in the field, writing, “This could change everything!”

Wow, amazing! This could change everything! https://t.co/2f0PDSaTSm — Chubby♨️ (@kimmonismus) August 27, 2024

With DisTrO, Nous Research is not only advancing the technical capabilities of AI training but also promoting a more inclusive and resilient research ecosystem that has the potential to unlock unprecedented advancements in AI.

Stay in the know! Get the latest news in your inbox daily

By subscribing, you agree to VentureBeat's Terms of Service.

Thanks for subscribing. Check out more VB newsletters here .

An error occured.

IMAGES

  1. Which Best Describes the Author's Purpose for Using a Metaphor

    research on metaphor analysis

  2. 1: Metaphor analysis procedure (Cameron, 2006)

    research on metaphor analysis

  3. 1: Metaphor analysis procedure (Cameron, 2006)

    research on metaphor analysis

  4. The Process of Great Chain of Metaphor Analysis

    research on metaphor analysis

  5. (PDF) Systematic Metaphor Analysis as a Method of Qualitative Research

    research on metaphor analysis

  6. Metaphor Examples: Understanding Definition, Types, and Purpose

    research on metaphor analysis

VIDEO

  1. MetaphorsㅣDefinition, Usage, and ExamplesㅣFigurative LanguageㅣEnglish

  2. MetaNet: Deep semantic automatic metaphor analysis

  3. Paradox and metaphor

  4. A Taxonomy of Visualization Research Cases of Sentiment Analysis Data based on Visual Metaphors

  5. Introduction to the Metaphor in end-of-life care project by Sheila Payne

  6. Literary Analysis The Metaphor

COMMENTS

  1. Critical Metaphor Analysis: A Systematic Step-by-step Guideline

    Metaphor analysis is a growing field of research and has particularly gained popularity in discourse and critical discourse studies over the last two decades. This has led to various analytical ...

  2. Research on metaphor processing during the past five decades: a

    Through the aforementioned document co-citation analysis, co-occurring word analysis, and cluster visualization which were done by CiteSpace, this study showed that research on metaphor processing ...

  3. Metaphor Analysis

    The purpose of metaphor analysis for qualitative research is to develop a better understanding of complex structures and lexical units. "Systematic metaphor analysis attempts to reconstruct models of thought, language and action" (Schmitt, 2005, p. 386) to make language more accessible.Furthermore, metaphor analysis outlines metaphorical patterns and can shed light into the frame of ...

  4. (PDF) Metaphor Analysis

    Subsequently, metaphor analysis has become a popular tool for understanding social phenomena including how people conceptualize organizational change, education, health issues, relationships ...

  5. PDF Metaphor Comprehension: A Critical Review of Theories and Evidence

    metaphors implies an eye for resemblances" (see Levin, 1982, for a discussion of Aristotle's views). Over the ensuing centuries, metaphor has proved to be a perennial source of fascination for philosophers, poets, linguists, computer scientists, and psycholo-gists. In recent years metaphor and related cognitive processes

  6. Systematic Metaphor Analysis as a Method of Qualitative Research

    first draft of the systematic metaphor analys is (Schmitt, 1995) relied upon quality criteria. such as those formulated by Mayring (1983) for "qualitative content analysis" and. conformed to ...

  7. Metaphor Analysis: Research Practice in Applied Linguistics, Social

    Metaphor Analysis: Research Practice in Applied Linguistics, Social Sciences and the Humanities by Lynn Cameron and Robert Maslen (Eds.) London, UK: Equinox, 2010. 298 pages, $34 (paperback), ISBN 9781845534479. Kathleen Ahrens Language Centre, Hong Kong Baptist University Correspondence [email protected].

  8. Metaphor research as a research strategy in social sciences and

    Metaphors have so far inspired many researchers to explain complex concepts or new theorizing. But there is no clear instruction for metaphor-based research and its validation principles. Here, we first locate metaphor research in social sciences and humanities (SSH) and classify different types of its use. Then we describe the basics of metaphor including its concept, types, components and ...

  9. Metaphor Research in the 21st Century: A Bibliographic Analysis

    has been done in bibliographical analysis of metaphor research. This paper exam-ines the advancements in metaphor research from 2000 to 2017. Using data re-trieved from Microsoft Academic Graph and Web of Science, this paper makes a macro analysis of metaphor research, and expounds the underlying patterns of its development.

  10. The Subjectivity Problem: Improving Triangulation Approaches in

    In this article, we argue that qualitative approaches to metaphor analysis are most effective when an intentional plan for triangulation is built into the research design. To enhance a researcher's interpretation of linguistic metaphors and their entailments, it is necessary to have a system in place for verification (Schmitt, 2005).

  11. PDF Metaphor Analysis in the Educational Discourse: A Critical Review

    Metaphor analysis is based on the belief that metaphor is a powerful linguistic device, because it extends and encapsulates knowledge about the familiarity and unfamiliarity. Metaphor analysis has been adopted in the educational discourse. The paper categorizes the previous relevant research into 3: interactions between

  12. Using Metaphors to Make Research Findings Meaningful

    However, while the right metaphor can be extremely useful, it also is important to understand that using metaphors to translate research results is not just a new way of offering information. ... Systematic metaphor analysis as a method of qualitative research. The Qualitative Report, 10 (2), 358-394. 10.46743/2160-3715/2005.1854 ...

  13. Current Approaches to Metaphor Analysis in Discourse

    This volume takes up the challenge of surveying the present state of a variety of approaches to the identification, analysis and interpretation of metaphor across communication channels, situational contexts, genres and social spheres. It reflects three foremost trends of present metaphor research, namely the communicative approach, the cognitive modelling approach and the multimodality ...

  14. Using Metaphors to Make Research Findings Meaningful

    In qualitative research, metaphors can help simplify complex and/or multidimensional concepts through connecting one familiar concept to another familiar one, resulting in the comparison between the two concepts opening up new possibilities and perspectives (Schmitt, 2005). Metaphors provide structure to data and aid understanding of a familiar ...

  15. Metaphor Analysis : Research Practice in Applied Linguistics, Social

    Lynne Cameron 2 A Selective Survey of Research Practice in Published Studies Using Metaphor Analysis Zazie Todd, University of Leeds, and Graham Low, University of York Section 2 Contemporary Theories of Metaphor 3 The Cognitive View of Metaphor: Conceptual Metaphor Theory Alice Deignan, University of Leeds 4 Between Mind and Language: A ...

  16. Systematic Metaphor Analysis as a Method of Qualitative Research

    Words: Metaphor Analysis, Subjectivity, Hermeneutics, and Qualitative Research "But one must know how to invent metaphors, which is not something for a rustic like me..." (Umberto Eco, 1996, p. 90) Metaphor, Qualitative Research and Lakoff and Johnson's "Cognitive Linguistics" For Anglo-American readers, this paper documents a return.

  17. Research trends in multimodal metaphor: a bibliometric analysis

    The concept of multimodal metaphor has generated a growing body of literature over the past decades. However, a systemic review of the domain seems to be lacking in relevant literature. This study, therefore, is an attempt to conduct a bibliometric analysis of the field of multimodal metaphor during 1977-2022, with a focus on 397 relevant ...

  18. [PDF] Analysis of Multimodal Metaphor and Values Representation in

    Children's picture books, as a form of text interwoven with vision and language, carry rich cultural information and educational functions. The purpose of this article is to explore the phenomenon of multimodal metaphor in children's picture books and reveal its influence on ideological construction. Firstly, the article compiles the research background and puts forward the research value ...

  19. (PDF) A bibliometric study of metaphor research and its implications

    journals in the WoS database from 2010 to 2020, and it is an exploratory study aided by a bibliometric. tool to analyse the research output in the field of metaphor studies, which was never been ...

  20. Reading meatphors in DNA (and RNA): a bio-rhetorical view of genetic

    1 Lakoff and Johnson's (Citation 2003) characterization of metaphor relies on a distinction between what a word 'actually' means and applications of that word outside its 'real' meaning.This is a practical limitation. Because the 'actual' meaning of a word shifts across time and discourse communities so that a usage may be metaphorical in one location and literal in another ...

  21. How Ethical Behavior Is Considered in Different Contexts: A ...

    The co-occurrence analysis of keywords provided diverse perspectives on the research topic, including focus areas, research contexts, and potential future directions. Third, our use of bibliographic coupling and co-citation techniques revealed clusters of related publications, offering a valuable resource for theoretical synthesis and analysis ...

  22. 11 Ways To Do SEO Content Research Beyond Competitor Analysis

    11 Ways Of Content Research Beyond Competitor Analysis To Scale SEO ROI. We have 11 ways to use this approach. Let's uncover them one by one with step-by-step processes and examples. 1. Use Semrush

  23. PDF Deloitte US

    Deloitte US | Audit, Consulting, Advisory, and Tax Services

  24. Key things to know about election polls in the U.S.

    ABOUT PEW RESEARCH CENTER Pew Research Center is a nonpartisan fact tank that informs the public about the issues, attitudes and trends shaping the world. It conducts public opinion polling, demographic research, media content analysis and other empirical social science research. Pew Research Center does not take policy positions.

  25. Beware the Unpatchable: Corona Mirai Botnet Spreads via Zero-Day

    Kyle Lefton is a security research intern on Akamai's Security Intelligence Response Team. Formerly an intelligence analyst for the Department of Defense, Kyle has experience in cyber defense, threat research, and counter-intelligence, spanning several years. ... 2024, but analysis showed activity for this variant as early as December 2023. The ...

  26. Time trends in the epidemiology of food allergy in England: an

    IgE-mediated food allergy is the most common cause of anaphylaxis in children and adults, and can be life-threatening. 1 Data suggest that in many regions, food allergy is becoming more common. 2,3 Estimated prevalence is between 0·1% and 9·3% in children, with geographical variations that suggest a larger disease burden in higher-income countries. 3 These estimates vary considerably ...

  27. Metaphor Analysis: Research Practice in Applied Linguistics, Social

    Depending on the research question, metaphor analysts may be interested in chunks of language situated above the procedure's standardized level of analysis (i.e., the lexical unit or lexeme ...

  28. Nous Research unveils powerful new AI training optimizer DisTrO

    Nous Research unveils new tool to train powerful AI models with 10,000x efficiency ... The reduction was achieved without relying on amortized analysis or compromising the convergence rate of the ...

  29. Can You Guess Which Brands Republicans and Democrats Love?

    Many mainstream brands and items have a customer base that leans red or blue, a WSJ analysis finds. Research shows that everything from your cereal to your sneakers can lean red or blue.

  30. (PDF) Metaphor Analysis in Psychology—Method, Theory ...

    The analysis of metaphors is a classical research theme in linguistics, but has received very little attention in psychological research so far. Metaphor analysis—as conceptualized in cognitive ...