Image

  • Presidential Message
  • Nominations/Elections
  • Past Presidents
  • Member Spotlight
  • Fellow List
  • Fellowship Nominations
  • Current Awardees
  • Award Nominations
  • SCP Mentoring Program
  • SCP Book Club
  • Pub Fee & Certificate Purchases
  • LEAD Program
  • Introduction
  • Find a Treatment
  • Submit Proposal
  • Dissemination and Implementation
  • Diversity Resources
  • Graduate School Applicants
  • Student Resources
  • Early Career Resources
  • Principles for Training in Evidence-Based Psychology
  • Advances in Psychotherapy
  • Announcements
  • Submit Blog
  • Student Blog
  • The Clinical Psychologist
  • CP:SP Journal
  • APA Convention
  • SCP Conference

CASE STUDY Jen (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder)

Case study details.

Jen is a 29 year-old woman who presents to your clinic in distress. In the interview she fidgets and has a hard time sitting still. She opens up by telling you she is about to be fired from her job. In addition, she tearfully tells you that she is in a major fight with her husband of 1 year because he is ready to have children but she fears that she is “too disorganized to be a good mother.” As you break down some of the processes that have led to her current crises, you learn that she has a hard time with time management and tends to be disorganized. She chronically misplaces everyday objects like her keys and runs late to appointments. Although she wants her work to be perfect, she is prone to making careless mistakes. The struggle for perfection makes starting a new task feel very stressful, leading her to procrastinate starting in the first place. As a consequence, she has recently received a number of warnings from her boss related to missing deadlines for assignments and errors in her work, which has led to her acute fear of being fired. As her performance at work has plummeted and she has grown increasingly anxious and doubting of herself, she has grown more pessimistic about starting a family. You learn that she received extra time for test taking in school as a child but never had any formal neuropsychological testing.  With Jen’s permission, you conduct additional structured assessments, including collecting collateral information from her fiancé, and conclude that she has adult ADHD.

  • Concentration Difficulties
  • Impulsivity

Diagnoses and Related Treatments

1. attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (adults).

Thank you for supporting the Society of Clinical Psychology. To enhance our membership benefits, we are requesting that members fill out their profile in full. This will help the Division to gather appropriate data to provide you with the best benefits. You will be able to access all other areas of the website, once your profile is submitted. Thank you and please feel free to email the Central Office at  [email protected] if you have any questions

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts

Logo of nihpa

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

Contributors

Associated Data

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), like other psychiatric disorders, represents an evolving construct that has been refined and developed over the past several decades in response to research into its clinical nature and structure. The clinical presentation and course of the disorder have been extensively characterised. Efficacious medication-based treatments are available and widely used, often alongside complementary psychosocial approaches. However, their effectiveness has been questioned because they might not address the broader clinical needs of many individuals with ADHD, especially over the longer term. Non-pharmacological approaches to treatment have proven less effective than previously thought, whereas scientific and clinical studies are starting to fundamentally challenge current conceptions of the causes of ADHD in ways that might have the potential to alter clinical approaches in the future. In view of this, we first provide an account of the diagnosis, epidemiology, and treatment of ADHD from the perspective of both the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders and the eleventh edition of the International Classification of Diseases. Second, we review the progress in our understanding of the causes and pathophysiology of ADHD on the basis of science over the past decade or so. Finally, using these discoveries, we explore some of the key challenges to both the current models and the treatment of ADHD, and the ways in which these findings can promote new perspectives.

Introduction

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), like other psychiatric syndromes, has been refined and developed over the past 50 years, from its first contemporary description in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (second edition; DSM-II) as a hyperkinetic reaction of childhood to its current inclusion in DSM-5 1 as a lifespan neurodevelopmental condition with specific criteria for children and adults, a change reflected in its counterpart, the International Classification of Diseases (11th revision; ICD-11). 2 This process of diagnostic evolution has been the result of periodic review and reformulation shaped by both research and clinical drivers. From a research perspective, the ADHD diagnostic formulation can be considered a part of a larger working hypothesis about the nature and structure of the disorder. 3 As such, this diagnostic formulation is tested against empirical evidence so that it represents an increasingly accurate approximation of nosological reality as reflected in established research findings. Because the primary purpose of diagnostic systems is to provide intuitive and implementable guides for clinical decision making, the threshold for diagnostic innovation is set high and the pace of diagnostic evolution has been incremental in nature. 4 Furthermore, as diagnostic systems in psychiatry have adopted a descriptive or phenomenological approach, considerations of the underlying causes of ADHD have been excluded from this process of re-evaluation and refinement. However, this diagnostic framework might be set to change. Progress in the aetiology and pathophysiology of ADHD challenges our current ways of thinking about the condition, while raising the prospect of new and potentially more effective clinical approaches.

Developing a broader range of more effective clinical approaches for people with ADHD, through the use of scientific discoveries, represents an important goal for the field.

ADHD is a prevalent, impairing condition that is frequently comorbid with other psychiatric disorders and creates a substantial burden for the individual, their family, and the community. 5 Medication-based treatment strategies have proven efficacious and cost-effective in the short term and a number of compounds are available, recommended, and widely used. 6 , 7 However, the long-term effectiveness of these treatments on key educational, vocational, and social outcomes remains uncertain. 8 , 9 Furthermore, such effects are compounded by low adherence, especially after extended use in adolescence. 10 These limitations are probably the result of both biological and psychosocial processes (eg, the build up of medication tolerance, ADHD-related stigma, and social resistance to medication). 8 , 11 Clearly, there is a pressing need for better long-term treatments for ADHD. By changing the way the field thinks about the causes of ADHD, scientific progress might help stimulate the development of new strategies for increasing the effectiveness of current treatments or the evolution of new alternatives. This Seminar will explore the issue of long-term treatment in three sections. The first section provides an account of the consensus about the clinical condition of ADHD, its diagnosis, epidemiology, developmental course, and treatment. The second section presents an up-to-date overview of ADHD science, focusing on advancements in aetiology and pathophysiology. The final section briefly explores how some of the most important scientific discoveries are beginning to challenge conceptions of ADHD in specific ways and examines the prospect that they will encourage new clinical perspectives and approaches.

Clinical consensus

ADHD is a clinical diagnosis requiring a detailed evaluation of current and previous symptoms and functional impairment. A full family, gestational, and developmental history should be taken. 12 The American Psychiatric Association’s DSM-5 defines ADHD in children (younger than age 17 years) as the presence of six or more symptoms in either the inattentive or hyperactive and impulsive domains, or both ( panel 1 ). Fewer symptoms (ie, at least five symptoms in either domain) are required to meet the adult diagnostic criteria. The age of symptom onset was modified from before age 7 years in DSM-IV to before age 12 years in DSM-5 to permit greater flexibility when diagnosing adults. Additionally, whereas DSM-IV divided ADHD into three subtypes on the basis of the predominant symptomatology (inattentive, hyperactive and impulsive, or combined), DSM-5 replaced the term “subtype” with “presentation” to emphasise that symptom clusters can change as patients mature and develop. 14 The ICD has updated its diagnostic formulation to bring it into line with DSM-5, moving ADHD from the disruptive to the neurodevelopmental disorder domain, exchanging the label hyperkinetic disorder with ADHD, and including inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive presentations of symptoms. 15 Distinct from DSM-5 and ICD-10, ICD-11 describes the essential features of the disorder, without giving a precise age of onset, duration, or number of symptoms. 15 We reviewed the diagnostic challenges, common comorbidities, and the role of neuropsychological tests ( appendix pp 1–3).

Criteria for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

The fifth edition of the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders criteria, inattention.

  • Often cannot give close attention to details or makes careless mistakes in schoolwork, at work, or with other activities
  • Often has trouble holding attention on tasks or play activities
  • Often does not seem to listen when spoken to directly
  • Often does not follow through on instructions and does not finish schoolwork, chores, or duties in the workplace (eg, loses focus, or is side-tracked)
  • Often has trouble organising tasks and activities
  • Often avoids, dislikes, or is reluctant to do tasks that require mental effort over a long period of time (such as schoolwork or homework)
  • Often loses things necessary for tasks and activities (eg, school materials, pencils, books, tools, wallets, keys, paperwork, eyeglasses, and mobile telephones)
  • Is often easily distracted
  • Is often forgetful in daily activities

Hyperactivity and impulsivity

  • Often fidgets with or taps hands or feet, or squirms in seat
  • Often leaves seat in situations when remaining seated is expected
  • Often runs about or climbs in situations where it is not appropriate (adolescents or adults might be limited to feeling restless)
  • Often unable to play or take part in leisure activities quietly
  • Is often “on the go” or acting as if “driven by a motor”
  • Often talks excessively
  • Often blurts out an answer before a question has been completed
  • Often has trouble waiting their turn
  • Often interrupts or intrudes on others (eg, conversations or games)

The eleventh edition of the International Classification of Diseases criteria

  • Persistent pattern (at least 6 months) of inattention,* hyperactivity-impulsivity,* or both
  • Onset typically in early to mid-childhood
  • Symptoms interfere with academic, occupational, or social functioning

*Additional descriptors of inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity are provided in the International Classification of Diseases, 11th revision. 13

Although ADHD is chronic in nature, and treatment is typically provided over several years, the course of the disorder can vary from one patient to the next. Longitudinal studies suggest the possibility of at least four developmental trajectories: early onset (preschool ADHD [3–5 years]), middle childhood (6–14 years) onset with a persistent course, middle childhood onset with adolescent offset, and adolescent or adult onset (16 years and older). 16 – 18 Treatment approaches and particular medications overlap substantially across these trajectories (as we discuss later), but prognosis might differ and understanding these disease courses might aid in treatment planning (eg, whether a child with ADHD no longer needs medication when they reach adolescence). Efforts are underway to predict the onset and course of ADHD across the lifespan. For example, on the basis of four longitudinal cohorts, Caye and colleagues 19 developed a risk calculator of childhood characteristics, such as intelligence quotient (IQ) and childhood maltreatment, that collectively estimates risk for adult ADHD. Establishing of robust predictors of clinical course would aid treatment decisions, informing, for instance, the duration of interventions and periods of elevated risk.

Epidemiology

Accurately estimating the number of individuals affected by ADHD in a population is essential to health service planning. This estimate allows the burden associated with the disorder to be approximated and then the required investment to be made. Furthermore, accurate epidemiological data across time and countries can help test the validity of the ADHD diagnosis, and might provide indications about its causes and pathophysiology. 20 Initial studies in the 1970s and 1980s provided a wide range of prevalence estimates, which, when interpreted in the context of the rapidly increasing number of children treated with medication at the time, raised societal concerns about inconsistent and excessive diagnosis. These studies also created disputes about the validity of the diagnosis. Some suggested the disorder was nothing more than a cultural product of competitive developed societies, its increasing use spurred on by the influence of the pharmaceutical industries trying to build market share. Meta-analytic studies investigating the factors responsible for this variability in prevalence were important in resolving such disputes. Two studies were especially influential and aggregated 102 studies 21 (and then an additional 41 studies) 20 involving community samples of children and adolescents from 35 countries in six continents worldwide and estimated the prevalence of ADHD as 5·29% (95% CI 5·01–5·56). 21 Follow-up meta-regression analyses indicated that the variability in previously reported ADHD prevalence was, in fact, attributable to methodological differences between studies, specifically in the diagnostic criteria, sources of information, and the requirement of functional impairment for diagnosis factors that varied between regions and countries. 21 , 22 Point prevalence from Europe, Oceania, South America, Asia, Africa, and the Middle East did not differ from those from North America, nor did the prevalence change across time from 1985 to 2012. 21 , 22 These data suggest that ADHD prevalence worldwide is stable when study methods are consistent. In general, studies done in the same populations with equivalent rating scales do not detect temporal changes in the number or severity of symptoms, 23 supporting the meta-analytical data, with only some studies pointing to decreasing 24 and fewer to increasing rates of subthreshold symptoms. 25

Meta-analyses have estimated the prevalence of ADHD in adults aged 19–45 years at 2·5% (95% CI 2·1–3·1). 26 Longitudinal studies following clinical samples of children with ADHD document a general decline of symptoms; meta-analytic data estimate that 15% of patients will persist with full diagnostic criteria in adulthood and 40–60% of patients will be classified as partial remitters. 27 Prevalence in adults is higher than would be expected by the persistence rates from children, suggesting the emergence of new cases during development. In fact, studies of prospectively followed up, representative community samples showed the emergence of ADHD during adolescence and adulthood, indicating a new developmental subtype of the disorder, 28 a finding discussed further in subsequent sections of this Seminar.

ADHD seldom affects only one functional domain but impacts many aspects of an individual’s wellbeing, including physical health, and academic, social, and occupational functioning. Often arising in childhood, ADHD can also be chronic in nature, frequently continuing through adolescence and beyond, at least at the level of impairment. Evaluation of treatment outcomes therefore should incorporate multiple components, such as psychoeducation, learning and academic support, school accommodation, intervention for management of symptoms, parental practices, and assessment and treatment of associated disorders. Treatment approaches are also likely to evolve as a patient matures. For example, parental practices have a large role in the treatment of a child aged 6–12 years, whereas psychoeducation regarding the risk of substance abuse and motor vehicle accidents becomes more central when a patient reaches adolescence. In our proceeding discussion of different approaches to managing ADHD, the degree of unmet clinical need in many countries where only a small percentage of individuals with the condition receive any treatment should be acknowledged. 29

With respect to the management of ADHD symptoms, US, 30 Canadian, 31 Latin American, 32 and European 33 medical organisations all recommend the use of psychostimulant medications. Many of these ADHD resources and guidelines can be found online. 33 , 34 However, most of these organisations recommend beginning with psychoeducation and behavioural management, particularly for individuals with mild symptoms and impairment. 6 , 35 US guidelines differ and suggest that medication is considered with initial treatment. 30 For children younger than 6 years old, there is consensus that treatment should start with behaviour management in the form of parent training and that medication should be reserved for more severe or unresponsive cases. The National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) guidelines, 33 for example, recommend that medication management for children younger than 5 years be considered only when parent training has been attempted and a second opinion has been obtained from a provider with expertise in ADHD in young children.

Medications for ADHD are categorised into stimulants (or psychostimulants) and non-stimulants, with several different formulations, delivery systems, and pharmacokinetic profiles available ( table ). Importantly, the availability of medications varies worldwide, with very few options accessible in some countries.

Medications for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

Dose range (mg)Delivery
Methylphenidate (short; duration of 4 h)
 Methylphenidate, immediate release10–60Tablet
 Methylphenidate, oral solution10–60Liquid
 Dexmethylphenidate, immediate release  2·5–20Tablet
Methylphenidate (intermediate; duration of 6–8 h)
 Methylphenidate hydrochloride, sustained release10–60Tablet
 Methylphenidate, long-acting10–60Capsule; contents can be sprinkled onto soft food
Methylphenidate (long; duration of 8–12 h)
 Dexmethylphenidate, extended release  5–30Capsule; contents can be sprinkled onto soft food
 Methylphenidate, oral solution, extended release20–60Liquid or chewable tablet
 Methylphenidate, osmotic release18–54 for children; 18–72 for adultsTablet; osmotic-release oral system
 Methylphenidate, transdermal10–30Patch
 Methylphenidate hydrochloride, extended release10–60Capsule; contents can be sprinkled onto soft food
Amphetamine (short; duration of action 4–6 h)
 Dextroamphetamine  5–40Tablet and liquid
 Dextroamphetamine-amphetamine  5–30Tablet
Amphetamine (long; duration of action 8–12 h)
 Dextroamphetamine-amphetamine, extended release  5–30Capsule; contents can be sprinkled onto soft food
 Dextroamphetamine, sustained release  5–40Capsule
 Lisdexamfetamine10–70Capsule; contents can be dissolved in liquid
Atomoxetine0·5–1·4 mg/kg; maximum 100 mgCapsule
Guanfacine, extended release1–4Tablet
Clonidine, extended release0·1–0·4Tablet

First used in children in the 1930s, psychostimulants continue to be first-line medications for management of ADHD symptoms and consist of formulations of methylphenidate and amphetamine. Mechanisms of action for both are similar. Methylphenidate blocks presynaptic dopamine and norepinephrine transporters, thereby increasing catecholamine transmission; amphetamine also inhibits both transporters, but additionally increases the presynaptic efflux of dopamine. 36 The efficacy of psychostimulants in reducing ADHD symptoms in short-term treatment has been shown in numerous clinical trials of both children and adults with ADHD. For instance, a meta-analysis including more than 10000 children and adolescents (with trials lasting approximately 3 months) found that methylphenidate and amphetamine both had moderate-to-large effect sizes when symptom change was rated by clinicians (SD for methylphenidate 0·78; SD for amphetamine 1·02) and teachers (SD for methylphenidate 0·82; data not available for amphetamine). 37 A meta-analysis of 18 studies suggested that methylphenidate is also efficacious in adults, with an effect size of 0·6 based on self-reported and clinician-reported symptom change. 38 Moreover, another meta-analysis encompassing more than 8000 adult participants showed moderate effect sizes for both methylphenidate (0·49) and amphetamine (0·79). 37 An additional meta-analysis of children with ADHD across 22 trials and adolescents with ADHD across nine trials compared methylphenidate and amphetamine and found both interventions highly efficacious, with slightly larger effects sizes for amphetamine (0·99) relative to methylphenidate (0·72). 39 The side-effect profiles of these drugs are similar, with the most common side-effects being appetite suppression, insomnia, dry mouth, and nausea, but amphetamine might be somewhat more prone to side-effects. 40 Side-effects are generally similar for adults and children, but might be more common in young children (ie, aged 5 years and younger). 41 NICE guidelines 33 recommend that medication treatment should begin with methylphenidate for children older than 5 years, but to then switch to amphetamine if the response is inadequate. For adults aged 18 years and older, the NICE guidelines recommend starting with either methylphenidate or the amphetamine formulation, lisdexamfetamine.

Three additional areas of concern with stimulant medications merit further consideration. First, there are the effects of long-term stimulant treatment on growth, specifically height and weight velocity. Although studies have yielded mixed results, most suggest that consistent stimulant use over several years can affect growth trajectories. 42 , 43 Based on childhood growth models, consistent long-term stimulant use might lead to modest reductions in adulthood height (approximately 1–3 cm), but more substantial increases of weight and body-mass index (although initial treatment can cause modest weight loss). 41 These effects, however, are potentially attenuated by introducing so-called drug holidays (eg, not taking medication over holiday periods or summers) and are arguably outweighed by the benefits of treatment. 44 , 45 Second, given the potential euphorigenic effects of stimulants, concern that stimulant use might increase the likelihood of subsequent substance abuse and dependence has been considerable. 46 However, this concern is not reflected in longitudinal research as studies have either suggested that stimulant use has no effect on, or can even lower, substance abuse risk. 46 – 48 In many developed countries, stimulant diversion is a related concern, by which individuals without ADHD acquire stimulants, often with the goal of increasing academic or vacation productivity. 49 Further research is needed to understand the prevalence and impact of such use. Finally, a third concern is that stimulants, with their sympathomimetic properties, increase the likelihood of cardiovascular events. Although initial research from small, retrospective samples indicated the presence of associations between stimulant use and sudden cardiac death, 49 large scale registry studies have generally been reassuring, showing no relationship between serious cardiovascular events and stimulant use. 51 , 52

Relative to stimulants, non-stimulant medications have lower responses and effect sizes and thus are typically reserved for patients who respond poorly or have intolerable side-effects to trials of stimulant formulations. Non-stimulant medications include the norepinephrine transporter inhibitor, atomoxetine, and the α-2 agonists, guanfacine and clonidine ( table ). Most treatment guidelines deem non-stimulant medications second-line treatments to be considered if treatment with stimulants proves inadequate. NICE guidelines, 33 for example, suggest that children with ADHD are switched to atomoxetine or guanfacine if their response to methylphenidate or amphetamine is poor; for adults, the recommendation is to switch to atomoxetine, as there is less evidence for α-2 agonists in adult ADHD.

A meta-analysis of 25 trials of atomoxetine in children with ADHD indicated a moderate effect size (SD 0·64); however, a large portion of patients (approximately 40%) had persistent symptoms requiring additional clinical intervention. 53 Atomoxetine is also effective in adults, albeit with a more modest effect size (approximately 0·33) based on a meta-analysis of 12 trials. 54 Trials of long-acting α-2 agonist formulations (extended-release guanfacine and extended-release clonidine) in children indicated medium effect sizes (0·5–0·6). 55 , 56 These formulations are often used as adjuvants to stimulants in patients with inadequate response to monotherapy, or in patients with comorbid aggression, insomnia, or tic disorders. 57 A clinical trial of extended-release guanfacine suggested similar effect sizes for adults with ADHD, but additional research is still needed to confirm these results. 58 In summary, the efficacy of ADHD medications has been clearly shown in the short term, with effect sizes and side-effects generally similar for adults and children. However, non-medication interventions do often differ between the age groups. Cognitive behavioural therapy and occupational coaching have a more substantial role in adult management, whereas home-based and school-based behavioural treatments are recommended for children (as we discuss later).

In the context of well controlled randomised controlled trials (RCTs), medications undoubtedly show short-term efficacy; however, their more general clinical value has been questioned. For example, once out of the rigorous context of an RCT, adherence is generally low, especially in adolescence, which undermines the practical effectiveness of medication treatments. 59 Long-term, naturalistic follow-up studies also cast doubt on the persistence of treatment effects, perhaps because of the development of tolerance with chronic dosing. 60 Taking academic-related outcomes as an example, a meta-analysis including 34 trials found that methylphenidate had only small and inconsistent effects on academic performance, improving general productivity, and accuracy in mathematics, but not reading. 61 Furthermore, in a sample of 370 children with ADHD followed up prospectively, stimulants were observed to have no effect on the number of school dropouts. 62 Similarly, in an 8-year, naturalistic follow-up of the Multimodal Treatment of ADHD (MTA) study, 8 null effects of psychostimulant treatment were reported across several functional domains, including academic achievement, social function, and overall levels of impairment. Findings from national registries have been more encouraging. For example, a Swedish registry found a substantial lowering of criminality during periods of stimulant use, 63 a US-based insurance registry described a reduction in motor vehicle accidents, 64 and a Taiwanese registry suggested a lowering of depression risk. 65 Additional positive effects on shorter-term outcomes such as traumas and injuries notwithstanding, 66 long-term functional outcomes are an essential area for further attention in the clinical management of ADHD.

The limitations of medication treatment for ADHD highlight the importance of the continued search for new and improved approaches to its management. In this regard, several new compounds are being explored. For example, the association between ADHD and de novo mutations in genes related to the expression of metabotropic glutamate receptors 67 has led to trials of glutamate modulators (eg, fasoracetam) in children with ADHD. 68 Trials are also underway to examine the effects of agents targeting nicotinic acetylcholine receptors, because of the putative role of acetylcholine in regulating arousal and attention. 69 Small studies of transdermal nicotine in adults and children with ADHD have shown promising initial results, but still require confirmation with larger RCTs. 70 , 71

Psychosocial and non-pharmacological approaches

Non-pharmacological approaches have either been adapted from other clinical areas or newly developed to complement medication treatment, and are recommended as part of the multimodal approach. 33 Access to effective non-pharmacological approaches is especially important for children aged 3–5 years for whom medication is not recommended, when there is a preference against medication expressed by families, or when there is resistance to medication use from clinicians and national organisations. 29 The MTA 72 study has provided important evidence about the value of generic psychosocial treatments when applied to ADHD. The MTA was a US-based, multisite study of young children aged 7–9·9 years with ADHD that allowed for comorbid conditions other than bipolar, autism, or psychosis, as long as the comorbidity did not require treatment incompatible with the study treatments. The MTA study found that treatment with either methylphenidate alone or in combination with psychosocial treatment (including home-based and school-based behavioural treatments and a summer school-based treatment) provided similar effects on ADHD symptoms after 14 months. However, combined treatment was superior to methylphenidate alone in improving some other functional outcomes, such as academic performance, parent–child relations, and social skills. 72 Analogous findings from other studies have led to recommendations that combined treatment (a stimulant and parent training) is preferable to medication alone for most children with ADHD. 35 Conversely, the benefits of parent training as the sole intervention are less clear. One meta-analysis suggested that symptom improvement with parent training is minimal when assessments are based on raters probably masked to treatment allocation (eg, teachers). 73 Similarly, neurofeedback, another popular non-pharmacological intervention, has shown promise in single arm or uncontrolled studies, but the effects do not separate from placebo with rigorous placebo control (eg, mock or sham neurofeedback) and masked raters. 74 Attentional and executive functioning training with interactive computer games (eg, Cogmed Working Memory Training) produces robust performance improvements on the training tasks. 75 However, the translation of these effects to improvements in ADHD symptoms has not been replicated, 75 , 76 despite promising findings from initial studies. 77 Placebo-controlled trials of dietary treatments (such as the exclusion of additives or supplements with free fatty acids) have shown value; 78 however, the effects are generally modest, although larger for exclusions when food intolerance is present. Interventions like physical exercise and meditation might have complementary benefits, but evidence for their short-term or long-term control of symptoms remains sparse. 79 , 80 Because of the equivocal effects of non-pharmacological interventions on ADHD symptoms to date, additional research is needed to show the efficacy of these interventions, and their combination, within the context of multimodal treatments. 81

Scientific progress in understanding the causes of ADHD

Having reviewed the clinical consensus about ADHD as represented in DSM-5 and ICD-11, we now provide an overview of scientific developments in our understanding of the pathogenesis, causes, and pathophysiology of ADHD. Through this overview, we want to convey the great strides made by researchers in understanding the disorder. These developments will create a platform for our exploration of the ways in which science is challenging our conceptions of ADHD and how these insights might stimulant new treatment approaches.

A more thorough review of the published literature on genetic influences in ADHD is provided in the appendix (pp 3–4) but, briefly, the heritability for ADHD is high and most estimates range between 70% and 80%. 82 Genome-wide association studies have successfully identified 12 genome-wide significant risk loci, yet these associations account for approximately 22% of the disorder’s heritability (as discussed later). Studies have also shown enrichment of certain copy-number variants in ADHD, 83 , 84 but these results require replication.

Numerous environmental exposures are associated with ADHD and have been suggested as putative causal factors. Nevertheless, given the complexity of the causal process (including the correlations between genetic and environmental exposures) and the reliance on observational (rather than experimental) study designs, most of these associations have yet to be shown as causal. In this context, many prenatal and perinatal risk factors, such as prematurity and low birthweight, have been more consistently associated with ADHD, with family studies suggesting that these effects cannot be explained by genetic confounding. 85 – 87 Intrauterine exposure to tobacco, and maternal stress and obesity during pregnancy, are also associated with ADHD, but they can be explained, at least in part, by confounding genetic factors. 88 Evidence is inconclusive or insufficient in relation to intrauterine exposure to alcohol and drugs and prenatal and perinatal birth-related complications.

Postnatal factors and social determinants have also been implicated in ADHD. For instance, experimental evidence shows that exposure to artificial food colourings and flavourings increases the severity of ADHD symptoms, but the effects are small. 89 The links between ADHD and exposure to pollutants and pesticides are largely correlational in nature. Innovative designs such as mendelian randomisation are starting to be used to test causal interpretations of these effects, albeit initial attempts have been disappointing. 90 Although associations between ADHD and parenting style have been noted, they are likely to be due to an evocative gene–environment correlation whereby a child’s behaviour elicits harsh and unsupportive parenting, which leads to an escalation of problems and the development of coercive cycles within families. 91 Evidence that supports social determinants of ADHD comes from a naturally created experiment: the adoption of children exposed, from soon after birth, to extreme deprivation in state institutions before the fall of the Communist regime in Romania in the late 1980s. Following on from earlier studies showing an association between institutional neglect and ADHD, Kennedy and colleagues 92 reported a 7-times increase in ADHD in individuals who had experienced more than 6 months of deprivation as children compared with those who experienced less than 6 months. The magnitude of this effect, combined with the strength of the design (contrasting those with more and less than 6 months deprivation), means that this result is unlikely to be attributable to pre-existing genetic or intrauterine risk. 93 However, the severity of the adversity experienced by these children is an extraordinary, or rare, environmental variant and the effect of less extreme exposures on ADHD risk is not as clear.

The mismatch between twin-based estimates of ADHD heritability (approximately 70–80%) and the estimates of genetic contribution based on the aforementioned genome-wide association study (about 22%) challenges researchers to account for the approximate 50% gap in the familial transmission of ADHD. Clearly, this gap cannot be explained by rare de novo mutations, which, by definition, are not inherited. To answer this question, some have looked to the study of gene–environment interactions. The plausibility of this hypothesis (ie, that gene–environment interactions account for the gap between heritability estimates and genetic contributions based on the genome-wide association study), in part, rests on the fact that the standard twin heritability model pools genetic main effects with gene–environment interactions into a single heritability term. To differentiate gene–environment interactions from genetic main effects, studies have combined specific risk alleles (mainly relating to neurotransmitter genes) and specific exposures (eg, social stressors such as intrauterine exposure to substances and psychosocial risk factors). Despite some studies finding suggestive results, to date convincing replications are absent. 94 Another approach focuses on epigenetic modifications in individuals with ADHD. Although this approach cannot, by itself, account for missing heritability, it has so far indicated differential DNA methylation in genes related to monoaminergic and GABAergic systems, and also in genes involved in neurodevelopmental processes. 95 However, partly because of their reliance on the harvesting of genetic material from peripheral tissues (which might not reflect changes in the brain), the actual extent to which these modifications are causal remains to be seen. 96 To improve our understanding of gene–environment interactions, and presumably ADHD causality, large-scale prospective studies such as the UK Biobank and the US-based ECHO and ABCD studies might prove helpful. These studies include biospecimens for genome-wide sequencing, detailed measures of phenotypic variance, and longitudinal assessments of environmental exposures, with adequate power to detect interactions and small effects. Over the next 5–10 years, we anticipate that these efforts will yield good results.

Pathophysiology

ADHD is associated with deficits across a range of cognitive domains ( panel 2 ). Global cognitive effects, as reflected in IQ below population norms, are well documented, 97 as are more circumscribed cognitive and motivational correlates. For example, laboratory studies have found replicated evidence of deficits in executive functions such as behavioural inhibition, working memory, set-shifting, and planning and organisation in groups of individuals with ADHD compared with non-affected controls. 98 , 99 However, within such groups, the specific pattern of individual executive function impairment varies dramatically from one individual with ADHD to another. This variation means that, although some individuals will show a pervasive pattern of impairment across different executive functions, others will display profound impairment in a particular executive function (eg, working memory) but be unaffected in other areas (eg, the ability to inhibit). Some patients will show no executive function impairment at all. This further emphasises the importance of issues of heterogeneity for current conceptions of ADHD. 100 , 101 Moreover, deficits in executive functions are by no means specific to ADHD and are similarly reported in many other psychiatric conditions. 102 – 104

Cognitive domains associated with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

  • Arousal: preparedness for action or behavioural activation
  • Executive functions: a broad range of higher-order cognitive processes, including decision making, planning, and working memory
  • Behavioural inhibition: capacity to restrain one response, or urge, in favour of another, often more automatic, response
  • Motivation: pursuing outcomes of potential value on the basis of subjective determinations
  • Set shifting: changing behavioural patterns to meet new environmental demands (eg, adjusting to new rules or expectations)
  • Working memory: short-term memory that allows for mental operation of information (eg, mental arithmetic) and often considered one type of executive function

Other cognitive domains, separable from (although potentially interacting with) executive functions, have also been implicated in ADHD. These include difficulties in regulating one’s psychological state in response to changing environmental circumstances by, for instance, trying to moderate one’s degree of arousal (so-called cognitive energetic factors). Clinically, this means that ADHD symptoms are exacerbated during lengthy and seemingly mundane tasks. 105 Furthermore, some people with ADHD show altered patterns of motivation 106 and respond differently to positive and negative reinforcement. 107 , 108 In this regard, difficulties in delaying gratification or waiting for important outcomes are motivational hallmarks for many with ADHD. These could be the result of either an inability to curb behavioural urges (ie, behavioural inhibition), or atypical responses to the expectation of future rewards, which often present as a heighted aversion to delay. 109 Once again, there is great heterogeneity in the expression of these motivational and cognitive impairments. 100

By highlighting the context-specific and dynamic nature of cognitive deficits in ADHD, the growing evidence of cognitive–energetic and motivational problems in ADHD has changed the way the pathophysiology of ADHD is considered. An especially important emergence has been the idea that ADHD is not always the result of a fixed deficit that affects performance across all settings, but rather it varies considerably from setting to setting. 110 , 111 For instance, ADHD symptoms and associated cognitive problems are much more common on tasks that are long and repetitive (with a low amount of stimulation) than they are on interesting tasks, in which there are frequent and engaging things happening. The most robust laboratory indication of the power of context, or setting, to shape the performance of people with ADHD is seen in how reaction time and accuracy change as the rate of stimulus presentation is varied. Patients with ADHD make more mistakes than controls only under very fast and very slow conditions. 112

Regarding brain structure and function, findings from neuroimaging research complement the cognitive and motivational profiles associated with ADHD. Because of its important role in executive functions, early neuroimaging research focused principally on the prefrontal cortex, showing functional and maturational abnormalities associated with ADHD. 113 , 114 For example, youth with ADHD show on average a 2–3 year delay in reaching peak thickness of much of the cerebrum, including the prefrontal cortex. 115 Over the last decade, neuroimaging research in ADHD, along with other psychiatric disorders, has shifted its focus of inquiry away from discrete neural substrates and towards the role of distributed neural circuits, recognising the importance of understanding the function, organisation, and development of interacting brain regions. 116 Emerging from this work, several large neural networks have been implicated in ADHD, including the default mode network (DMN), dorsal and ventral attentional networks, salience networks, and frontostriatal and mesocorticolimbic circuits (or the dopaminergic meso- limbic system). 5 Functional neuroimaging studies have shown reduced connectivity within the DMN of children aged 6–17 years with ADHD and a pattern suggestive of delayed DMN neuromaturation, 117 consistent with earlier structural MRI studies pointing to delayed maturation of the cerebral cortex. 115 The function of the DMN is hypothesised to underlie the normative capacity for mind-wandering and introspection, but in ADHD might reflect a tendency towards distractibility, potentially due to impaired regulation of attentional resources. 118 Similar research highlights atypical interactions between the DMN and the dorsal and ventral attentional networks, and the salience network, suggesting that the DMN, and its relationship to mindwandering, might interfere with, or disrupt, attentional networks’ capacity to maintain externally, focused attention. 5 , 118

In addition to the DMN and attentional networks, individuals with ADHD also manifest abnormalities within the dopaminergic mesolimbic system, a neural circuit associated with motivated behaviours, anticipated outcomes, and reinforced learning. 98 For example, relative to healthy controls, individuals with ADHD show reduced volumes of the nucleus accumbens (a key node within the mesolimbic system), 119 , 120 reduced activation of the mesolimbic system when anticipating rewarding outcomes, 121 and reduced fractional anisotropy (a diffusion MRI indicator of white matter organisation) within white matter tracts of the mesolimbic system. 122

Despite these advances in our understanding of the neurobiology of ADHD, several concerns persist. First, most neuroimaging studies of ADHD have been cross-sectional in design, and thus unable to reach causal interpretations. For example, adaptations to a disorder versus underlying causes cannot be disambiguated from cross-sectional, case-control designs. Combining neuroimaging and RCTs is a potentially fruitful approach to overcome the limitations of correlational neuroimaging research, identifying causal effects of interventions (although not causal factors that give rise to the disorder itself) on brain structure and function. 123 Second, neuroimaging studies of ADHD often have small samples and poor reproducibility. False findings might have arisen from inadequate control over multiple statistical comparisons, imaging confounds such as head motion and partial averaging, and inadequate clinical phenotyping (eg, relying solely upon parental reports for diagnostics). By contrast, one mega-regression analysis of structural MRI studies from more than 1700 youth with ADHD reported reduced volumes in multiple subcortical structures, including those associated with the aforementioned attentional and reward systems. 120 However, the effect sizes across all regions were small (Cohen’s d <0·2), highlighting the fact that many previous neuroimaging studies were underpowered and risked false-positive results. Finally, the small effect sizes reported by large mega-analyses, such as ENIGMA, suggest that neuroimaging is unlikely to have clinical use as a diagnostic tool, at least not until substantial methodological advances have been made.

What are the prospects for clinical advances in response to scientific advances?

In this final section, we briefly discuss four means by which scientific findings are challenging the way ADHD is conceptualised and explore the prospect that these can improve the diagnosis and treatment of ADHD in the future.

Dimensionality

All evidence confirms that ADHD is best understood as the extreme end of a continuum and that people with ADHD differ from those without ADHD by degree rather than in kind. Yet, both DSM-5 and ICD-11 continue to operationalise ADHD as a categorical diagnosis: a syndrome defined by symptom thresholds that suggest the existence of discrete boundaries between health and disorder, and between unaffected and impaired. However, there is no evidence that this definition of ADHD is correct, and so current clinical boundaries are somewhat arbitrary, built on clinical experience about the number and severity of symptoms or degree of impairment that warrants intervention. Are there viable alternatives to the current categorical models that better reflect the underlying reality of the condition? Despite concerns about clinical applicability, several different dimensional models have been proposed, but none have been judged to have the simplicity and immediacy of the current approaches. 124

Heterogeneity

As aforementioned, science leaves us in no doubt that ADHD is a complex and heterogeneous disorder. Individuals with the condition differ from one another in myriad ways and at multiple levels, such as in their genetic risks, environmental exposures, brain structures, and cognitive and motivational profiles. Recognising that ADHD is a heterogeneous disorder presents appreciable challenges to researchers (studies designed to explore this heterogeneity require large samples, multiple measures that capture the full range of deficits, and the use of sophisticated multivariate analytical approaches), but also offer potential clinical opportunities. Acknowledging heterogeneity might lead to the adoption of precision medicine: the tailoring of treatments to target an individual’s underlying cognitive and brain processes. 125 For instance, executive training might be efficacious for individuals with ADHD and executive dysfunctions, but not for those with motivational or cognitive energetic abnormalities. Neuropsychological subtyping of ADHD populations has been proposed to facilitate this sort of approach. 98 , 125 However, progress is hampered by a lack of consensus regarding the pathophysiological dimensions of greatest clinical relevance and whether individuals cluster along these dimensions to form identifiable subgroups. Addressing the challenges of heterogeneity is central to the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative of the US-based National Institute of Mental Health.

Development

ADHD is a lifespan disorder with roots in early childhood 126 and branches extending into adolescence and adulthood. 127 We’ve previously highlighted evidence that several different developmental forms of ADHD appear to exist, which are currently not distinguished in diagnostic approaches. The developmental core of the ADHD phenotype will remain marked by childhood onset and adulthood persistence, but four developmental types with potential clinical relevance are starting to emerge. However, much still needs to be learned about differential prognosis or treatment response of individuals with these distinct developmental phenotypes. Taking a developmental perspective focuses attention on the likely value of early intervention and prevention strategies. 128 Interventions aimed at delaying the initial onset or reducing the impact of ADHD continue to be hindered by a lack of understanding of the early cognitive and behavioural precursors and predictors of later ADHD that would allow for the cost-effective early identification of at-risk individuals. 129 We are also devoid of effective interventions that can be implemented during the first years of life, although novel approaches to strengthening underlying brain networks are being trialled. 130 Additional strategies, aimed at reducing the escalation and effect of the disorder, have effectively focused on the use of parent training, psychoeducation, and support to reduce the risk of emergence of comorbid conditions and associated impairment. 131

Overlapping causes

Our Seminar highlights a surprising degree of overlap between ADHD and other psychiatric conditions in terms of both aetiology and pathophysiology. This overlap, like evidence of dimensionality and heterogeneity, challenges current diagnostic systems, and lends conceptual support for approaches that attempt to identify common pathophysiological processes that cut across traditional diagnostic boundaries (eg, RDoC initiative), rather than those that diverge at the level of the clinical DSM or ICD phenotypes. 132 The assumption of a transdiagnostic framework, such as RDoC, is that shared vulnerability processes can be successfully targeted to treat different clinical conditions. For example, where similar executive deficits are present across a range of different neurodevelopmental conditions (ADHD, autism spectrum disorder, and psychosis), the same executive control training approaches could have clinical value. 133 Similarly, a transdiagnostic framework might provide some insight into why causal factors can give rise to ADHD, and other psychiatric conditions. For instance, gestational diabetes is associated with increased risk for ADHD in the offspring. 134 Although genetic confounding has not been excluded, preclinical research suggests that this observation might be attributable, at least in part, to the effects of maternal hyperinsulinaemia or adiposity on the development of the fetal mesolimbic system. 135 , 136 One could therefore hypothesise that this same prenatal exposure has transdiagnostic implications, increasing risk for other conditions related to mesolimbic dysfunction, such as Tourette’s syndrome and substance use disorders. 135 , 137 However, to date, evidence supporting treatment or causal implications of a transdiagnostic framework is largely absent.

Conclusions

ADHD is a common, highly heritable, and impairing condition. Efficacious treatments are available but limited in many ways. We believe that the enormous strides made over the past 10 years by scientists in understanding the nature and causes of ADHD challenge accepted models of ADHD and might have the potential to encourage new clinical improvement. However, this advancement will take both time and considerable investment to identify the specific processes and systems to target, develop new and innovative interventions to target these processes, and discover the best ways to tailor them to a patient’s individual needs.

Search strategy and selection criteria

We searched PubMed for articles published between Jan 1, 1980, and March 1, 2019, with an emphasis on the previous 10 years. English and non-English language publications were considered in our search. We included primary and review articles resulting from these searches, along with relevant references cited within those articles. Given the broad scope, yet restricted space, of our review, we occasionally cite review papers in place of primary reports. We used the search terms: [ISP check]“ADHD”, “neurobiolog*”, “neural circuits”, “brain imaging”, “genetics”, “endophenotypes”, “impulsivity”, and “psychostimulant”.

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgments.

Declaration of interests

JP received research support from Shire and Aevi Genomics. GVP received conference support, speaking fees, and consultancy fees from Takeda (formerly Shire), consultancy fees from Medice, and royalties from Editora Manole. ES-B received research funding and conference support from Shire, speaking fees from Janssen-Cilag, consultancy fees from Neurotech Solutions, Aarhus University, Copenhagen University, and Berhanderling, Skolerne, Copenhagen, KU Leuven, and royalties from Oxford University Press and Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

See Online for appendix

For more on Cogmed see https://www.cogmed.com

For more on the UK Biobank see https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk

For more on ECHO see https://www.nih.gov/echo

For more on ABCD see https://abcdstudy.org

For more on ENIGMA see http://enigma.ini.nsc.edu

adhd case study quizlet

Faculty and Disclosures

Disclosure of conflicts of interest, disclosure of unlabeled use.

adhd case study quizlet

Attention-deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Two Case Studies

  • Authors: Authors: Joseph Biederman, MD; Stephen V. Faraone, PhD
  • THIS ACTIVITY HAS EXPIRED FOR CREDIT

Target Audience and Goal Statement

This activity has been designed to meet the educational needs of pediatricians, family practitioners, child and adolescent psychiatrists, and general psychiatrists involved in the management of patients with ADHD.

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a chronic condition that affects 8% to 12% of school-aged children and contributes significantly to academic and social impairment. There is currently broad agreement on evidence-based best practices of ADHD identification and diagnosis, therapeutic approach, and monitoring. However, the increasing rate of diagnosis and treatment in the pediatric population has contributed to the significant public debate and misunderstanding of ADHD. Despite increased awareness, Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a chronic condition that affects 8% to 12% of school-aged children and contributes significantly to academic and social impairment. There is currently broad agreement on evidence-based best practices of ADHD identification and diagnosis, therapeutic approach, and monitoring. However, the increasing rate of diagnosis and treatment in the pediatric population has contributed to the significant public debate and misunderstanding of ADHD. Despite increased awareness, ADHD remains underrecognized and may be undertreated by a factor of 10 to 1 in the US population. In order to educate the public and ensure optimal outcomes for ADHD patients, this continuing education activity has been developed to provide physicians and other healthcare providers with the most current information available on assessing and treating ADHD.

Upon completion of this activity, participants should be able to:

  • Discuss the incidence of ADHD in adolescents and adults.
  • Identify DSM-IV criteria used to make the diagnosis of ADHD in each age group.
  • List important comorbidities of ADHD and identify distinguishing features between ADHD and other psychiatric diagnoses with similar manifestations.
  • Describe a pharmacologic approach to ADHD treatment, including treatment goals and choice of medication.
  • Enumerate self-management skills to be recommended when coaching ADHD patients on how to get along at school, at work, and at home.

Disclosures

Accreditation statements, for physicians.

This activity has been planned and implemented in accordance with the Essential Areas and Policies of the Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education (ACCME). The Postgraduate Institute for Medicine is accredited by the Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education (ACCME) to provide continuing medical education for physicians.

The Postgraduate Institute for Medicine designates this educational activity for a maximum of 1.0 Category 1 credit toward the AMA Physician's Recognition Award. Each physician should claim only those credits that he/she actually spent in the activity.

Contact This Provider

For questions regarding the content of this activity, contact the accredited provider for this CME/CE activity noted above. For technical assistance, contact [email protected]

Instructions for Participation and Credit

There are no fees for participating in or receiving credit for this online educational activity. For information on applicability and acceptance of continuing education credit for this activity, please consult your professional licensing board. This activity is designed to be completed within the time designated on the title page; physicians should claim only those credits that reflect the time actually spent in the activity. To successfully earn credit, participants must complete the activity online during the valid credit period that is noted on the title page. Follow these steps to earn CME/CE credit*:

  • Read the target audience, learning objectives, and author disclosures.
  • Study the educational content online or printed out.
  • Online, choose the best answer to each test question. To receive a certificate, you must receive a passing score as designated at the top of the test. In addition, you must complete the Activity Evaluation to provide feedback for future programming.

You may now view or print the certificate from your CME/CE Tracker. You may print the certificate but you cannot alter it. Credits will be tallied in your CME/CE Tracker and archived for 5 years; at any point within this time period you can print out the tally as well as the certificates by accessing "Edit Your Profile" at the top of your Medscape homepage. *The credit that you receive is based on your user profile.

Case 2: ADHD in an Adult

Case history.

A 42-year-old woman, mother of a son in his junior year of high school and a 20-year-old daughter, both living at home, comes seeking help because she feels her marriage is falling apart. This patient is the mother of the adolescent with ADHD in the previous case history. Her speech is rambling and a little impulsive. The physician manages to piece together the following history during her first visit, which was scheduled for 30 minutes but takes an hour because of her long, unfocused answers to the questions.

School had always been relatively hard for the patient, starting in grammar school. Though she had presented no behavior problems, teachers consistently complained of the patient's inattention and disorganization. With the help of summer school and tutors she managed to stay on course until she graduated from high school and even to get accepted to college. She was placed on probation after one semester at the university's business school, taking incompletes or "D's" and "F's" in each of her courses because she missed classes. She turned in assignments late, if at all; what she did hand in was sketchy and sloppy. Her advisor recommended that she switch her major from accounting to marketing, taking advantage of the patient's creative streak. With the help of tutoring and coaching in how to stay organized, the patient managed to earn her degree.

She was married for 6 months immediately after high school, but that marriage ended by mutual consent because, the patient says, "Neither of us had any idea what it meant to be in an adult relationship." The patient met her current husband, a graphic arts major, in a college advertising class. They were married in his senior year. It should have been her senior year too, but it took her an extra 1½ years to graduate. After they started dating they dreamed of developing their own business, combining her expertise in marketing with his in design. This was not to be. Not long after they started living together, her husband found that he could not count on his spouse to arrive at a meeting on time, remember to make an important phone call, or even to keep the checkbook in a consistent place. They decided to start a family together instead of a business. The husband worked in advertising while his wife stayed home with their 2 children.

As the children grew they had their own school problems and, later, social and legal troubles. (See previous case.) Their mother did her best to fulfill her role as stay-at-home caretaker, but it still fell on their father to pack their lunches, get them to the bus stop on time, and help them daily with homework. In spite of the patient's background in business, her husband took care of paying bills, balancing the checkbook, and preparing tax returns. The wife became a good cook. Her meals were quite creative but often served late because she had had to run out to the store, sometimes more than once, to purchase ingredients she had forgotten.

Once the children were in third and sixth grade, the patient found employment outside the home. Her husband took it upon himself to create a chore list for each family member. He was the only one to follow through consistently with his assigned tasks. Their home is no less messy than it was when the patient was at home full-time because even then, she was never organized enough to get ahead on the housework. Thanks to the patient, the home is decorated quite creatively. She rearranges the furniture and art work on the walls every few weeks because, she says, keeping things the same for too long makes her feel restless.

The patient's first job was in the marketing department of a local business. Within a few months she lost that position because of tardiness, absenteeism, unmet deadlines, and a general impression that she was not reliable or competent. She made few friends at work except for some smokers with whom she congregated regularly at the back door of the business.

She smokes 2 packs of cigarettes per day, a habit she has had since high school. She also drinks caffeinated coffee all day. The patient has recently cut down on her alcohol consumption after a near-miss on a second DUI and a confrontation with her husband over her escalating alcohol intake. She had tried other recreational drugs in college but did not continue using them after her marriage.

Losing that job as a marketer was the first of a succession of job losses. Subsequent reasons included the undependability shown at her first job, but she also impulsively quit when frustrated with working conditions and blurted out harsh criticism of the boss. Each new job brought lower pay and lower status than the previous one. The patient berates herself for her poor job performance, but though intelligent and educated enough, she can't seem to do any better. At the time of this interview she is working as the person on duty at 2 different laundromats for a 60+-hour work week. She likes how busy and active she is at this job; between servicing customers and machines she rarely sits down.

The home environment is messier and more chaotic than ever. It seems to her family that they are all perpetually being sent about the house in search of her glasses, keys, or wallet.

The patient has little energy for anything but her job. According to records received, her previous primary care doctor thought she might be depressed and tried a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, with little relief. The patient's answer to a direct question about whether she had taken the medication regularly is vague. It is not clear whether she did not trust the diagnosis of depression or could not remember to take her pills regularly, but the physician suspects that the patient did not have an adequate therapeutic trial of antidepressants. Progress notes in the previous doctor's record confirm the suspicion of noncompliance. That physician also tried clonidine, Vitamin B 6 , and various other measures without success to alleviate the severe PMS symptoms that had escalated over recent years. Still, she sleeps well and maintains a good appetite.

The patient describes herself as unpredictably irritable. She admits to picking fights with her husband and says she has completely lost interest in sex. Now, with concerns looming about both of their children compounded by the patient's being away from home so much of the time, her husband has threatened to leave. She feels like a failure in all realms: as a mother, a spouse, a homemaker, and a breadwinner.

Medscape Logo

IMAGES

  1. ADHD* Flashcards

    adhd case study quizlet

  2. Clinical- ADHD Diagram

    adhd case study quizlet

  3. Case 1

    adhd case study quizlet

  4. ADHD Flashcards

    adhd case study quizlet

  5. ADHD case study

    adhd case study quizlet

  6. ADHD Flashcards

    adhd case study quizlet

VIDEO

  1. Takeda Attention on ADHD: Case Study Video (André)

  2. ADHD

COMMENTS

  1. RN Hesi Case Study

    Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like Which information is important for the ED nurse to obtain about Jason's accidents? A. Explanation of previous accidents. B. Where was Jason at the time of the accidents? C. Precipitating events leading up to the accidents. D. Who was present at the time of the accident? E. If there is a family history of suicide., How should the ...

  2. ADHD Case Study Flashcards

    Preview. NR414 Exam 1. 94 terms. kj4134473. Preview. Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like What information is important for the RN to obtain about Joe's accidents?, What are HALLMARK characteristics of ADHD? SATA, The nurse recognizes which statement by Joe supports his diagnosis of ADHD? and more.

  3. ADHD Case Study Flashcards

    HESI Case Study ADHD. 25 terms. katie_do2. Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like What information is important for the RN to obtain about Joe's accidents?, When his laceration is treated he says he knows he is clumsy and wishes there was a pill for that.

  4. HESI Case Study ADHD Flashcards

    A. Nutritional status. B. Mood/anxiety disorders. E. Coping mechanisms. Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like What information is important for the RN to obtain about Joe's accidents?, What are hallmark characteristics of ADHD?, The nurse recognizes which statement by Joe supports his diagnosis of ADHD? and more.

  5. ADHD Case Study Flashcards

    Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like what do people with ADHD wish that the public knew?, what does ADHD stand for?, what is recommended to classrooms? and more.

  6. PSY203 CASE STUDY QUIZ: ADHD

    Quiz yourself with questions and answers for PSY203 CASE STUDY QUIZ: ADHD, so you can be ready for test day. Explore quizzes and practice tests created by teachers and students or create one from your course material.

  7. PDF Case Study 1

    Case Study 1 - Jack Jack is a 7 year old male Grade 1 student who lives in Toronto with his parents. He is the only child to two parents, both of whom have completed post-graduate education. There is an extended family history of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), mental health concerns as well as academic excellence.

  8. CASE STUDY Jen (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder)

    Case Study Details. Jen is a 29 year-old woman who presents to your clinic in distress. In the interview she fidgets and has a hard time sitting still. She opens up by telling you she is about to be fired from her job. In addition, she tearfully tells you that she is in a major fight with her husband of 1 year because he is ready to have ...

  9. A Case Study in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: An Innovative

    1.1. Evaluation of ADHD. The current diagnostic criteria for ADHD can be found in the DSM-5 [] and in the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, eleventh revision, from the World Health Organization [].Various evaluation instruments are used to identify ADHD, from general assessments via broad scales such as the Wechsler scale, to more specific tests ...

  10. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (Pediatric)

    Quizlet vati - Greenlight for Vati. Professional Nursing Practice. Lecture notes. 91% (11) 10. ... 2022 Case Study Topic: ( & Stand- Alone Trend) Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Author: Heidi Bresee, DNP, PPCNP-BC, RN Frederick Community College. Case Summary.

  11. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

    Meta-analyses have estimated the prevalence of ADHD in adults aged 19-45 years at 2·5% (95% CI 2·1-3·1). 26 Longitudinal studies following clinical samples of children with ADHD document a general decline of symptoms; meta-analytic data estimate that 15% of patients will persist with full diagnostic criteria in adulthood and 40-60% of ...

  12. Attention-deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Two Case Studies

    Despite increased awareness, Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a chronic condition that affects 8% to 12% of school-aged children and contributes significantly to academic and social impairment. There is currently broad agreement on evidence-based best practices of ADHD identification and diagnosis, therapeutic approach, and ...

  13. PDF CASE STUDY 10-year-old boy diagnosed with ADHD

    up the case study, names those activities in brief without the full details and explicit information each client-family receives in why and how to implement the program. Go to www.handle.org for more information. The HANDLE® Institute 7 Mt. Lassen Drive, Suite B110 San Rafael, CA 94903 415-479-1800

  14. ADHD case study Flashcards

    1 / 5. Study with Quizlet and memorize flashcards containing terms like What information is important for the RN to obtain about Joe's accident? SATA, What is the hallmark characteristic of ADHD. SATA, The nurse recognizes which statement by Joe supports his diagnosis of ADHD and more.

  15. Adhd Case Study Quizlet

    Adhd Case Study Quizlet - Andre Cardoso #30 in Global Rating Sharing Educational Goals. Our cheap essay service is a helping hand for those who want to reach academic success and have the perfect 4.0 GPA. Whatever kind of help you need, we will give it to you. It's your academic journey. ...

  16. Adhd Case Study Quizlet

    Adhd Case Study Quizlet, Purpose Of Literature Review In Research Report, Thesis Statement Analytical Essay, Cheap Resume Editor For Hire Ca, Article-based Doctoral Dissertation, Top University Essay Editor Service For Phd, Esl University Dissertation Conclusion Topics

  17. Adhd Case Study Quizlet

    Adhd Case Study Quizlet, Esl Blog Post Proofreading Website Au, Personal Statement Undergraduate Sample Essays, Undergraduate Thesis Archive, Link For Associates In Nursing To Bsn At Utrgv, Critical Critical Essay In Science Sociological Spirit, Corporate Resume Templates

  18. Adhd Case Study Quizlet

    Adhd Case Study Quizlet, Thesis About True Friendship, Essay On House In Kannada Language, Positive Effects Of Sports Essay, Safety Writing Border, Objective Of Business Plan Competition, Do My Critical Essay On Founding Fathers 7 Customer reviews ...

  19. Adhd Case Study Quizlet

    Adhd Case Study Quizlet - REVIEWS HIRE. 100% Success rate 4.9/5. REVIEWS HIRE. Earl M. Kinkade #10 in Global Rating $ 10.91. Adhd Case Study Quizlet: 4093 Orders prepared. Order now Login. ID 4746278. Finished paper. 4.8/5. REVIEWS HIRE. Nursing Business and Economics Management Aviation +109. Write an essay from varied domains with us! ...

  20. Adhd Case Study Quizlet

    Adhd Case Study Quizlet - Andre Cardoso #30 in Global Rating Reset password. ID 27260. 4.7/5. Completed orders: 145. Nursing Management Business and Economics Psychology +113. Paper Writing Service Price Estimation. Adhd Case Study Quizlet: LET'S FIND OUT. Eric Bl. Research in general takes time. ...

  21. Adhd Case Study Quizlet

    Adhd Case Study Quizlet | Top Writers. ID 27260. 4.9 (2939 reviews) So caring about what I expect... Earl M. Kinkade. #10 in Global Rating. Adhd Case Study Quizlet. 675. Finished Papers.

  22. Adhd Case Study Quizlet

    Adhd Case Study Quizlet - Give Yourself up to Extra Pleasures. 407 . Customer Reviews. phonelink_ring Toll free: 1(888)499-5521 1(888)814-4206. User ID: 231078 / Mar 3, 2021. User ID: 107841. We suggest our customers use the original top-level work we provide as a study aid and not as final papers to be submitted in class. ...

  23. Adhd Case Study Quizlet

    Professional authors can write an essay in 3 hours, if there is a certain volume, but it must be borne in mind that with such a service the price will be the highest. The cheapest estimate is the work that needs to be done in 14 days. Then 275 words will cost you $ 10, while 3 hours will cost you $ 50.