U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts

Logo of nihpa

EVOLUTION OF HUMAN EMOTION

Joseph e. ledoux.

New York University, 4 Washington Place, New York, NY 10003, 212-998-3930. Nathan Kline Institute, 140 Old Orangeburg Road, Orangeburg, NY 10962

Basic tendencies to detect and respond to significant events are present in the simplest single cell organisms, and persist throughout all invertebrates and vertebrates. Within vertebrates, the overall brain plan is highly conserved, though differences in size and complexity also exist. The forebrain differs the most between mammals and other vertebrates. The classic notion that the evolution of mammals led to radical changes such that new forebrain structures (limbic system and neocortex) were added has not held up, nor has the idea that so-called limbic areas are primarily involved in emotion. Modern efforts have focused on specific emotion systems, like the fear or defense system, rather than on the search for a general purpose emotion systems. Such studies have found that fear circuits are conserved in mammals, including humans. Animal work has been especially successful in determining how the brain detects and responds to danger. Caution should be exercised when attempting to discuss other aspects of emotion, namely subjective feelings, in animals since there are no scientific ways of verifying and measuring such states except in humans.

Introduction

The topic of emotion and evolution typically brings to mind Darwin’s classic treatise, Emotions in Man and Animals ( Darwin, 1872 ). In this book Darwin sought to extend his theory of natural selection beyond the evolution of physical structures and into the domain of mind and behavior by exploring how emotions too might have evolved. Particularly important to his argument was the fact that certain emotions are expressed similarly in people around the world, including in isolated areas where there had been little contact with the outside world and thus little opportunity for emotional expressions to have been learned and culturally transmitted. This suggested to him that there must be a strong heritable component to emotions in people. Also important was his observation that certain emotions are expressed similarly across species, especially closely related species, further suggesting that these emotions are phylogenetically conserved.

With the rise of experimental brain research in the late 19 th century, emotion was one of the key topics that early neuroscientists sought to relate to the brain (see LeDoux, 1987 ). The assumption was that emotion circuits are conserved across mammalian species, and that it should be possible to understand human emotions by exploring emotional mechanisms in the non-human mammalian brain.

In this chapter, I will first briefly survey the history of ideas about the emotional brain, and especially ideas that have attempted to explain the emotional brain in terms of evolutionary principles. This will lead to a discussion of fear, since this is the emotion that has been studied most thoroughly in terms of brain mechanisms. The chapter will conclude with a reconsideration of what the term emotion refers to, and specifically which aspects of emotion can be studied in animals and which must be studied in humans.

A Brief History of the Emotional Brain: The Rise and Fall of the Limbic System Theory

All organisms, even single cell organisms, must have the capacity to detect and respond to significant stimuli in order to survive. Bacteria, for example, approach nutrients and avoid harmful chemicals ( Macnab and Koshland, 1972 ). With the evolution of multicellular, metazoan organisms with specialized systems, particularly a nervous system, the ability to detect and respond to significant events increases in sophistication ( Shepherd, 1983 ).

Invertebrates, the oldest and largest group of multicellular organisms, exhibit a wide variety of types of nervous systems. However, all vertebrates share a common basic brain plan consisting of three broad zones (hindbrain, midbrain, and forebrain) with conserved basic circuits ( Nauta and Karten, 1970 ; Swanson, 2002 ; Bulter and Hodos, 2005 ; Striedter, 2005). In spite of this overall similarity, differences in size and complexity exist. For example, the forebrain differs the most between mammals and reptiles. On the basis of such differences, the classic view of forebrain evolution emerged in the first half of the 20 th century (e.g. Smith, 1924 ; Herrick, 1933 ; Arien Kappers et al, 1936 ; Papez, 1937 ; MacLean, 1949 , 1952 ). According to this view, with the emergence of mammals, the forebrain plan underwent radical changes in which new structures, especially cortical structures, were added. These were layered over and covered the reptilian forebrain, which mainly consisted of the basal ganglia. First came “primitive” cortical regions in early mammals. In these organisms the basic survival functions related to feeding, defense and procreation were taken care of by fairly undifferentiated (weakly laminated) cortical regions (primitive cortex, including the hippocampus and cingulate cortex) and related subcortical areas (such as the amygdala) that were closely tied to the olfactory system. Later mammals added highly novel, laminated cortical regions (neocortex) that made possible enhanced non-olfactory sensory processing and cognitive functions (including learning and memory, reasoning, and planning capacities, and, in humans, language).

The basic principle that equated cognition with evolutionarily new cortex (neocortex) and emotion with older cortex and related subcortical forebrain regions culminated in Paul MacLean’s limbic system theory of emotion (1949 , 1952 , 1970 ). The term limbic was first used by the French anatomist Paul Broca as a structural designation for a rim of cortex in the medial wall of the hemisphere. Broca called this rim the limbic lobe ( le grande lobe limbique ) (limbic is from the Latin word for rim, limbus ). MacLean built on the classic findings of comparative anatomists such as Herrick and Papez, and experimental findings from Walter Cannon, Phillip Bard and Henrich Kluver and Paul Bucy ( Cannon, 1929 ; Bard, 1928 ; Kluver and Bucy, 1937 ) to transform the limbic lobe into an emotion system, the limbic system. The limbic system was defined anatomically as the primitive medial cortical areas and interconnected subcortical nuclei (including the amygdala and septum).

MacLean called the limbic system the paleomammalian brain (since it was said to have emerged with the evolution of early mammals), and contrasted it with the reptilian brain (basal ganglia and brainstem). In more recent mammals the neocortex, also called the neomammalian brain, was said by MacLean to increases in size and complexity at the expense of the limbic system. The decrease of the limbic system reduced the dependence of humans on base emotions, and the increase in the neocortex allowed humans greater control over remaining emotional circuits as well as greater cognitive capacities.

The limbic system concept stimulated much research in the 1950s, 60s, and 70s. However, it has been criticized on a number of grounds, and has been rejected by many scientists ( Swanson, 1983 ; LeDoux, 1987 , 1991 , 1996 ; Kotter and Meyer, 1992 ; Butler and Hodos, 2005 ). Because the limbic system concept continues to be referred to in some scientific circles (e.g. Panksepp, 1998 , 2005 ) and persists in many lay accounts of the brain, it is worth considering why it is not acceptable.

First, the theory presumes that the neocortex and limbic system are unique mammalian specializations. Neither of these ideas is accepted by contemporary comparative neuroanatomists ( Nauta and Karten, 1970 ; Northcutt and Kaas, 1995 ; Butler and Hodos, 2005 ; Striedter, 2005). Birds and reptiles, for example, have been shown to have structures that correspond with both mammalian neocortex and with MacLean’s cortical and subcortical limbic areas (hippocampus, amygdala). Second, MacLean argued the architecture of limbic areas is ill-suited for cognitive processes. However, the hippocampus, viewed by MacLean as the centerpiece of the limbic system and a central structure for emotional functions, is recognized as one of the key areas related to higher cognitive functions, such as declarative or explicit memory ( Squire, 1987 ; Eichenbaum, 2002 ) and spatial cognition ( O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978 ). Third, efforts to define the system have failed. Connectivity with old cortex is a flawed criterion if old cortex is itself an unjustified notion. Connectivity with the hypothalamus once seemed plausible, since that was a way of distinguishing relevant and irrelevant cortical areas ( Issacson, 1982 ). However, as anatomical techniques improved, areas from the neocortex were also found to be connected with the hypothalamus, as were areas of the spinal cord, potentially extending the limbic system across the entire brain. Finally, and perhaps most important, there is no evidence that the limbic system, however defined, functions as an integrated system in the mediation of emotion. While specific areas of the limbic system contribute to some emotional functions, these areas do not do so because they belong to a limbic system that evolved to perform emotional functions. Indeed, relatively few limbic areas have been shown to contribute to emotional functions. As noted above, the hippocampus, the centerpiece of the limbic system theory of emotion, has been strongly implicated in cognitive functions but the evidence for a role in emotion is far less impressive.

The limbic system theory attempted to explain all emotions within a single anatomical concept. Contemporary researchers are more inclined to focus on tasks designed to study the brain systems of specific emotions. As we will see, this has been a more profitable empirical approach.

Contributions of the Amygdala to Avoidance Conditioning: An Early Approach to Linking Emotional Behavior to the Limbic System

Why, then, has the limbic system concept persisted for so long given that it proved questionable on evolutionary, structural, and functional grounds? The key reason can be summarized in the term “guilt by association.” One limbic area, the amygdala, has consistently been implicated in emotional behavior. Because the amygdala was part of the limbic system concept, its involvement vindicated the whole concept. This does not mean that the amygdala is the only structure involved in emotion, but instead that the amygdala is one area that has been extensively implicated in emotion, in part because of the behavioral tasks that have most often been used to study emotion.

The amygdala was first implicated in emotion through studies of the Kluver-Bucy Syndrome, a set of unusual behaviors observed in monkeys after removal of large areas of the temporal lobe ( Kluver and Bucy, 1937 ). Monkeys with such lesions attempted to eat inappropriate items and copulate with inappropriate partners, and lost their fear of snakes and humans. It was concluded that the animals had psychic blindness, an inability to appreciate the significance or value of visual stimuli. Weiskrantz (1956) attempted to localize the effects within the temporal lobe using a behavioral task where behavior was guided by stimulus value. Specifically, he used an avoidance conditioning paradigm where monkeys learned to use a cue to signal when to perform a behavioral response in order to avoid receiving a painful shock. Such a paradigm was viewed as especially useful in assessing the role of the amygdala in processing threats that lead to fear. Damage targeted to the amygdala disrupted performance, leading Weiskrantz to conclude that the amygdala was responsible for the inability of animals with temporal lobe damage to use stimulus value to guide behavior, and thus that an important function of the amygdala was to ascertain stimulus value. Specifically, Weiskrantz proposed that the amygdala processes the rewarding and punishing consequences of events. However, the data were essentially about aversive or punishing events since avoidance conditioning is a fear-based paradigm.

Subsequently, throughout the 1960s and 1970s, avoidance conditioning paradigms were used to study the contribution of the amygdala to emotion, an especially to fear. The bulk of the evidence was consistent, in general, with the idea that the amygdala is a key structure in avoidance conditioning, and by implication, in processing the value of emotional stimuli. Such findings were treated as evidence in support of the limbic system theory of emotion since the amygdala was part of the limbic system.

By the mid 1980s, thirty years of research on the brain mechanisms of avoidance had been conducted. While it seemed clear that the amygdala was somehow involved, there was considerable confusion as exactly what its role might be ( Sarter and Markowitsch, 1985 ). There are several explanations likely for this unsettled state of affairs. First, there was little appreciation of the anatomical complexity of the amygdala, a brain region with a dozen or so nuclei, each with subnuclei ( Amaral et al, 1992 ; Pitkanen et al, 1997 ; LeDoux, 2007 ). Failure to recognize this anatomical complexity may have led to confusion. Indeed, more recent work has shown that different nuclei and subnuclei have different functions ( Repa et al, 2001 ; LeDoux, 2007 ). Second, the behavioral complexity of avoidance conditioning itself was not fully appreciated. Avoidance tasks can be constructed in various ways (active, passive, signaled, unsignaled), and each involves the learning of a Pavlovian association and an instrumental association ( Cain and LeDoux, 2007 ; Cain et al., 2010 ; LeDoux et al., 2009 ; Choi et al, 2010 ; Amorapanth et al, 2000 ; Lazaro-Munoz et al., 2010 ). In retrospect, failure to separate these components also probably played a role in adding confusion to efforts to understand the brain mechanisms of avoidance.

Contribution of the Amygdala to Fear: Studies of Aversive Pavlovian Conditioning

During the 1960s, researchers began using Pavlovian conditioning to pursue the cellular and molecular mechanisms of learning in invertebrates (e.g. Kandel and Spencer, 1968 ; Kandel et al., 1986 ). The success of this approach, together with the fact that avoidance conditioning was stuck in a rut, led mammalian researchers to turn to Pavlovian conditioning as well ( Thompson, 1986 ; Kapp et al, 1979 ; LeDoux et al, 1984 ; Tischler and Davis, 1983 ).

As mentioned already, Pavlovian conditioning is the initial phase of avoidance conditioning. After the subjects rapidly undergo Pavlovian conditioning, they then slowly learn to perform avoidance responses using the CS as a warning signal. Indeed, the emotional learning that occurs in avoidance conditioning occurs during the Pavlovian phase. Pavlovian conditioning is thus a more direct means of studying emotional processing. Perhaps Pavlovian conditioning would be easier to understand.

In Pavlovian fear conditioning, the subject receives a neutral conditioned stimulus (CS), usually a tone, followed by an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US), typically footshock. After one or at most a few pairings, the CS comes to elicit innate emotional responses that naturally occur in the presence of threatening stimuli, such as predators. For example, after conditioning a CS elicits defensive freezing behavior and associated autonomic and endocrine responses that support the behavior ( Blanchard and Blanchard, 1969 ; Bolles and Fanselow, 1980 ; LeDoux et al, 1984 ). The subject does not have to learn to perform these responses. The responses are innate. What is learned is an association that allows a novel stimulus, a warning of danger, to elicit the defensive responses in anticipation of the actual danger.

With the simpler approach provided by fear conditioning, as opposed to avoidance, much progress was made in mapping the circuitry, including the regions in the brain where the CS and US converge to form the associations and the regions involved in the control of emotional responses by the CS in animals (see LeDoux, 2000 ; Maren, 2001 , 2005 ; Rodrigues et al, 2004 ; Johansen et al, in review ; Davis, 1992 ; Davis et al., 1997 ; Fanselow and Poulos, 2005 ; Pape and Pare, 2010 ) and humans ( Phelps and LeDoux, 2005 ; Phelps, 2006 ; Sehlmeyer et al., 2009 ; Kim et al., 2011 ; Whalen and Phelps, 2009 ). In brief, CS and US convergence occur in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala (LA), and specifically in the dorsal subregion of the LA. This convergence leads to synaptic plasticity and the formation of a CS-US association. Damage to LA, inactivation of LA, or manipulation of a variety of molecular pathways in LA prevents fear conditioning. A second important region is central nucleus of the amygdala (CE). Manipulations of the region also disrupts conditioning. LA and CE are connected directly and by way of various intra-amygdala pathways. Once the CS-US association is formed, later exposure to the CS results in the retrieval of the learned association formed by CS-US convergence during conditioning. Information then flows from LA to CE, which then connects to hypothalamic and brainstem areas that control behavioral, autonomic, and hormonal responses that help the organism cope with the threat. Plasticity also occurs in CE, and in CS processing regions and motor control regions. This simplified description omits many details.

Much has been learned about the molecular mechanisms in LA that make fear conditioning possible ( Blair et al, 2001 ; Schafe et al, 2001 ; Rodrigues et al, 2004 ; Fanselow and Poulos, 2005 ; Maren, 2001 , 2005 ; Pape and Pare, 2010 ; Sah et al, 2008 ; Johansen et al, in review ). In brief, the CS input synapses undergo plasticity when the LA neurons they connect with are depolarized by the shock US. As a result, the ability of the CS to activate the LA cell is potentiated. Plasticity is triggered when the depolarizing US allows calcium to flow into the cell via NMDA receptors and voltage sensitive calcium channels. The elevated calcium activate a number of protein kinases that ultimately lead to phosphoylation of transcription factors such as CREB that lead to gene expression and protein synthesis. The newly synthesized proteins then stabilize the synaptic potentiation, allowing the CS to strongly activate the LA cell for over long periods of time. It is particularly interesting that many of the molecular changes that underlie fear conditioning in mammals have also been show to be important for Palovian conditioning in invertebrates, showing the conserved nature of the molecular mechanisms of learning and memory.

The advances made in understanding Pavlovian fear conditioning made it possible to revisit the neural system of avoidance and related aversive instrumental behaviors ( Cain and LeDoux, 2007 ; Cain et al., 2010 ; LeDoux et al., 2009 ; Choi et al, 2010 ; Amorapanth et al, 2000 ; Lazaro-Munoz et al., 2010 ). This work showed that as in fear conditioning, the LA is essential for forming the CS-US association. But in contrast to fear conditioning, in avoidance information flows from the LA to the basal amygdala (not to the CE). Extrapolating from appetitive conditioning finding ( Everitt et al, 1989 , 1999 ; Cardinal et al, 2002 ), it has been proposed that connections from the basal amygdala to the ventral striatum allow the CS-US association to control aversively motivated instrumental behavior.

Avoidance responses are not emotional responses per se. They are simply responses. An animal can learn to avoid harm by running, climbing, pressing, swimming, or even remaining stationary. The animal learns to do what it needs to do to attain safety. But the same responses could be used to obtain food if the animal is hungry and those responses are a way to gain access to food. In contrast, in Pavlovian fear conditioning the CS elicits specific emotional responses, fear or defense responses. Researchers were much more inclined to discuss Pavlovian conditioning results specifically in terms of fear/defense circuits.

Comparative Observations

Amygdala areas have been implicated in fear conditioning in a variety of mammals, including rats, mice, rabbits, and monkeys (see LeDoux, 1996 , 2000 ; Johansen et al, in review ; LeDoux, 2000 ; Maren, 2001 , 2005 ; Rodrigues et al, 2004 ; Davis, 1992 ; Davis et al., 1997 ; Fanselow and Poulos, 2005 ; Pape and Pare, 2010 ). This suggests strong conservation of the circuitry within mammals, including humans. Indeed, a large body of work implicates the human amygdala in fear conditioning and in instrumental responses like avoidance ( Phelps and LeDoux, 2005 ; Phelps, 2006 ; Whalen et al., 2004 ; Dolan and Vuilleumier, 2003 ; Whalen and Phelps, 2009 ; Delgado et al., 2009 ; Labar, 2003 ; Ousdal et al., 2008 ; Gianaros et al., 2008 ; Damasio, 1994 ; Bechara et al., 1995 ). Thus, damage to the amygdala in humans prevents fear conditioning from occurring and functional imaging studies show that activity increases in the amygdala during fear conditioning. Additionally, a number of studies have found amygdala activation in response to angry or fearful faces, considered to be unconditioned threat stimuli ( Adolphs, 2008 ). Thus, findings involving both lesion studies and functional imaging suggest strong correspondence with the animal literature, at least at a gross anatomical level. Techniques available for studying the human brain do not allow precise localization of specific nuclei, though some progress is being made in this regard ( Davis et al., 2011 ; Bach et al., 2011 ).

An important question concerns the nature of the amygdala in non-mammals and the role of the homologous structure in fear conditioning. According to classic view, areas such as the amygdala, being paleomammalian structures, should not exist in reptiles. However, in the 1970s, Cohen (1975) claimed to have indentified the amygdala in avian species and found that lesions of this regions disrupted of Pavlovian fear conditioning in pigeons. More recently, there has been much debate about what constitutes the amygdala, and specifically individual amygdala nuclei, in reptiles and birds ( Karten, 1997 ; Martinez-Garcia et al., 2002 ; Lanuza et al., 1998 ; Moreno and Gonzalez, 2007 ; Bruce and Neary, 1995 ). Using connectivity patterns established in mammals, areas believed to be homologous to the lateral and central nucleus have been identified in lizards ( Martinez-Garcia et al., 2002 ; Lanuza et al., 1998 ). When threatened, these animals undergo tonic immobility, and damage to the CE homologue interferes with this defensive response ( Davies et al., 2002 ).

Much more work is needed to resolve what constitutes the amygdala in non-mammalian vertebrates and to determine whether the functions of the amygdala known to exist in mammals have some relation to the function of the homologous regions in the vertebrate ancestors of mammals.

Amygdala Contributions to Other Emotions

While the contribution of the amygdala to fear has been most thoroughly studied, it is clear that the amygdala contributes to other emotional states as well. A relatively large body of research has focused on the role of the amygdala in processing of rewards and the use of rewards to motivate and reinforce behavior ( Cardinal et al., 2002 ; Everitt et al., 1999 ; Holland and Gallagher, 2004 ; Murray, 2007 ; Salzman et al., 2007 ; Nishijo et al., 2008 ). As with aversive conditioning, the lateral, basal, and central amygdala have been implicated in different aspects of reward learning and motivation, as well as drug addiction. The amygdala has also been implicated in emotional states associated with aggressive, maternal, sexual, and ingestive (eating and drinking) behaviors ( Bahar et al., 2003 ; Galaverna et al., 1993 ; Miczek et al., 2007 ; Pfaff, 2005 ; Siegel and Edinger, 1981 ). Less is known about the detailed circuitry involved in these emotional states than is known about fear.

Emotional Evolution in Perspective

There is no shortage of theories that have speculated about the relation of emotion circuits to brain evolution. In the tradition of Darwin, basic emotions theorists have proposed that certain emotions are innate, in part because they are expressed the same in people around the world ( Tomkins, 1962 ; Ekman, 1977 , 1992 ; Izard, 1971 , 1992 ; Plutchik, 1980 ; Buck, 1981 ). These innate emotions are said to be mediated by affect programs in the brain. An affect program, in effect, is psychological description of a dedicated neural circuit. Some neuroscientist have adopted the basic emotions idea, and have proposed specific circuits for different basic emotions ( Panksepp, 1980 , 1998 , 2005 ), though the basic emotions discussed do not completely correspond with those proposed in the psychological theories.

The above discussion of the amygdala and its role in fear and defense might be construed as a mini-version of basic emotions theory, a version focused on one basic emotion. However, there is a fundamental difference between the approach I take and the approach of basic emotions theorists.

The goal of basic emotions theories is to understand subjective states of conscious experience that humans label with emotion words (fear, love, sadness, joy, etc). Their goal is to understand “feelings.” This is also true of brain science theories of emotion focused on basic emotions. Panksepp (1980 , 1998 , 2005 ), for example, searches for brain systems in animals that underlie feelings in the animals as a way of understanding the brain systems that underlie human feelings. Vocalizations that result from tickling a rat are ways of indexing joyful or pleasurable feelings in the rat brain, and freezing, flight and fight behaviors are markers of fearful feelings.

The approach I take is quite different ( LeDoux, 1984 , 1996 , 2002 , 2008 ). I use emotional behavior as a means of indexing circuits that have evolved to allow organisms to deal with challenges and opportunities in their environments. I make no assumption about what an animal is feeling, since I believe it is not possible to scientifically measure, and thus not possible to research, feelings in animals other than humans. I do not deny that other animals may have feelings. I simply question whether these can be studied using scientific methods. Beyond this methodological barrier, I am also critical of attempts to equate feelings in humans and other animals for other reasons. First, most studies that have explored conscious experience in humans have found that when information (including emotional information) reaches awareness the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is active, and if information is experimentally prevented from reaching awareness this area is not active (for summary see LeDoux, 2008 ). The dorsolateral granular prefrontal cortex is a unique primate specialization ( Preuss, 1995 ; Wise, 2008 ) and has features in the human brain that are lacking in other primates (Semendeferi et al., 2011). If human conscious experience depends on these unique features of brain organization, we should be cautious about attributing the kinds of mental states made possible by these features to animals that lack the feature or the brain region. Second, language is a unique human capacity, and conscious experience, including emotional experience, is influenced by language. The once disputed idea that language, and the cognitive processes required to support language functions, add complexity to human experience, has regained respect ( Lakoff, 1987 ; Lucy, 1997 ). In the absence of language experience cannot be partitioned in the same way-- English speakers can partition fear and anxiety into more than 30 categories ( Marks, 1987 ). The diversity with which non-verbal organisms can conceptualize the world and their experiences in it is thus likely constricted by the absence of language.

In sum, basic tendencies to detect and respond to significant events are present in the simplest single cell organisms, and persist throughout all invertebrates and vertebrates. Within vertebrates, the overall brain plan is highly conserved, though differences in size and complexity also exist. The forebrain differs the most between mammals and other vertebrates, though the old notion that the evolution of mammals led to radical changes such that new forebrain structures were added has not held up. Thus, the idea that mammalian evolution is characterized by the addition of a limbic system (devoted to emotion) and a neocortex (devoted to cognition) is flawed. Modern efforts to understand the brain mechanisms of emotion have made more progress by focusing on specific emotion systems, like the fear or defense system, rather than on efforts to find a single brain system devoted to emotion. Also, progress has been made in animal studies by focusing on emotion in terms of brain circuits that contribute to behaviors related to survival functions. Efforts to use scientific methods identify circuits in animals that might correspond to circuits in the human brain that are responsible for conscious feelings are not likely to succeed since we have no way of scientifically measuring feelings in animals. Conscious feelings are an important topic, but one that is best pursued through studies of humans.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms446011f1.jpg

Fear is the emotion most thoroughly understood in the brain. Much of the progress made has involved studies of Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. During conditioning the conditioned stimulus (CS), usually a tone, and the unconditioned stimulus (US), usually a footschock, converge in the lateral nuclecus of the amygdala (LA) to induce synaptic plasticity of the CS inputs (CS–US convergence not shown). The CS is then able to flow through amygdala circuits to the central nucleus (CE) to control the expression of hard-wired, automatic, defensive reactions (freezing behavior, autonomin nervous system, ANS, activity, and hormonal release). CE outputs also activate networks in that control the release of neuromodulators, such as norepinephrine (NE), dopamine (DA), acetylcholine (ACh), and serotonin (5HT) throughtout the brain. These, like hormonal feedback, help add intensity to and prolong the duration of the aroused state. In addition to these various automatic responses controlled by CE, the LA also sends information, via the basal nucleus (B) to the ventral striatum, especially the nucleus accumbens. The latter connections are likely to be involved in the invigoration of goal-directed behaviors that allow the organism to act in certain ways on the basis of past instrumental learning or on-the-spot decisions about how to cope with the threat. Other abbreviations: ITC, intercalated nuclei of the amydala; CG, central gray; LH, lateral hypothalamus; PVN, paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus; VP, ventral pallidum.

  • Adolphs R. Fear, faces, and the human amygdala. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2008; 18 :166–172. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Amaral DG, Price JL, Pitkänen A, Carmichael ST. Anatomical organization of the primate amygdaloid complex. In: Aggleton JP, editor. The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction. New York: Wiley-Liss, Inc; 1992. pp. 1–66. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Amorapanth P, LeDoux JE, Nader K. Different lateral amygdala outputs mediate reactions and actions elicited by a fear-arousing stimulus. Nature Neuroscience. 2000; 3 :74–79. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ariëns Kappers CU, Huber CG, Crosby EC. The Comparative Anatomy of the Nervous System of Vertebrates, Including Man. New York: Macmillan Company; 1936. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bach DR, Talmi D, Hurlemann R, Patin A, Dolan RJ. Automatic relevance detection in the absence of a functional amygdala. Neuropsychologia. 2011; 49 :1302–1305. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bahar A, Samuel A, Hazvi S, Dudai Y. The amygdalar circuit that acquires taste aversion memory differs from the circuit that extinguishes it. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2003; 17 :1527–1530. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bard P. A diencephalic mechanism for the expression of rage with special reference to the sympathetic nervous system. American Journal of Physiology. 1928; 84 :490–515. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Adolphs R, Rockland C, Damasio AR. Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science. 1995; 269 :1115–1118. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blair HT, Schafe GE, Bauer EP, Rodrigues SM, LeDoux JE. Synaptic plasticity in the lateral amygdala: a cellular hypothesis of fear conditioning. Learn & Memory. 2001; 8 :229–242. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC. Crouching as an index of fear. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1969; 67 :370–375. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bolles RC, Fanselow MS. A perceptual-defensive-recuperative model of fear and pain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1980; 3 :291–323. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bruce LL, Neary TJ. The limbic system of tetrapods: a comparative analysis of cortical and amygdalar populations. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 1995; 46 :224–234. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Buck R. The evolution and development of emotion expression and communication. In: Brehm SS, Kassin SM, Gibbons FX, editors. Developmental Social Psychology. New York: Oxford Univ. Press; 1981. pp. 127–151. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Butler AB, Hodos W. Comparative Vertebrate Neuroanatomy: Evolution and Adaptation. 2. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, Inc; 2005. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cain CK, Choi JS, LeDoux JE. Active Avoidance and Escape Learning. In: Koob G, Thompson R, Le Moal M, editors. Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience. New York: Elsevier; 2010. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cain CK, LeDoux JE. Escape from fear: a detailed behavioral analysis of two atypical responses reinforced by CS termination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 2007; 33 :451–463. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cannon WB. Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear, and rage. Vol. 2. New York: Appleton; 1929. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cardinal RN, Parkinson JA, Hall J, Everitt BJ. Emotion and motivation: the role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2002; 26 :321–352. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carter CS, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD. The contribution of the anterior cingulate cortex to executive processes in cognition. Reviews in the Neurosciences. 1999; 10 :49–57. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch DM, Botvinick MM, Noll D, Cohen JD. Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science. 1998; 280 :747–749. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Choi JS, Cain CK, LeDoux JE. The role of amygdala nuclei in the expression of auditory signaled two-way active avoidance in rats. Learn & Memory. 2010; 17 :139–147. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cohen DH. Involvement of the avian amygdalar homologue (archistriatum posterior and mediale) in defensively conditioned heart rate change. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1975; 160 :13–35. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Damasio A. Descarte’s error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Gosset/Putnam; 1994. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Darwin C. The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: Fontana Press; 1872. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Davies DC, Martinez-Garcia F, Lanuza E, Novejarque A. Striato-amygdaloid transition area lesions reduce the duration of tonic immobility in the lizard Podarcis hispanica. Brain Research Bulletin. 2002; 57 :537–541. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Davis FC, Johnstone T, Mazzulla EC, Oler JA, Whalen PJ. Regional response differences across the human amygdaloid complex during social conditioning. Cerebral Cortex. 2011; 20 :612–621. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Davis M. The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1992; 15 :353–375. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Davis M, Walker DL, Lee Y. Amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis: differential roles in fear and anxiety measured with the acoustic startle reflex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences. 1997; 352 :1675–1687. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Delgado MR, Jou RL, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA. Avoiding negative outcomes: tracking the mechanisms of avoidance learning in humans during fear conditioning. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2009; 3 :33. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dolan RJ, Vuilleumier P. Amygdala automaticity in emotional processing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2003; 985 :348–355. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Eichenbaum H. The cognitive neuroscience of memory. New York: Oxford University Press; 2002. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ekman P. Biological and cultural contributions to body and facial movement. In: Blacking J, editor. The Anthropology of the Body. London: Academic Press; 1977. pp. 39–84. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ekman P. An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion. 1992; 6 :169–200. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Everitt BJ, Cador M, Robbins TW. Interactions between the amygdala and ventral striatum in stimulus- reward associations: studies using a second-order schedule of sexual reinforcement. Neuroscience. 1989; 30 :63–75. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Everitt BJ, Parkinson JA, Olmstead MC, Arroyo M, Robledo P, Robbins TW. Associative processes in addiction and reward. The role of amygdala-ventral striatal subsystems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1999; 877 :412–438. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fanselow MS, Poulos AM. The neuroscience of mammalian associative learning. Annual Review of Psychology. 2005; 56 :207–234. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Galaverna OG, Seeley RJ, Berridge KC, Grill HJ, Epstein AN, Schulkin J. Lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala. I: Effects on taste reactivity, taste aversion learning and sodium appetite. Behavioural Brain Research. 1993; 59 :11–17. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gianaros PJ, Sheu LK, Matthews KA, Jennings JR, Manuck SB, Hariri AR. Individual differences in stressor-evoked blood pressure reactivity vary with activation, volume, and functional connectivity of the amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008; 28 :990–999. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Herrick CJ. The functions of the olfactory parts of the cerebral cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U S A. 1933; 19 :7–14. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Holland PC, Gallagher M. Amygdala-frontal interactions and reward expectancy. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2004; 14 :148–155. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Isaacson RL. The Limbic System. New York: Plenum Press; 1982. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Izard CE. The Face of Emotion. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1971. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Izard CE. Basic emotions, relations among emotions, and emotion-cognition relations. Psychological Review. 1992; 99 :561–565. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Johansen J, Cain C, Ostroff L, LeDoux JE. Neural Mechanisms of Fear Learning and Memory. Cell in review. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kandel ER, Klein M, Castellucci VF, Schacher S, Goelet P. Some principles emerging from the study of short- and long-term memory. Neuroscience Research. 1986; 3 :498–520. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kandel ER, Spencer WA. Cellular neurophysiological approaches to the study of learning. Physiological Reviews. 1968; 48 :65–134. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kapp BS, Frysinger RC, Gallagher M, Haselton JR. Amygdala central nucleus lesions: effect on heart rate conditioning in the rabbit. Physiology & Behavior. 1979; 23 :1109–1117. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Karten HJ. Evolutionary developmental biology meets the brain: the origins of mammalian cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U S A. 1997; 94 :2800–2804. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kim MJ, Loucks RA, Palmer AL, Brown AC, Solomon KM, Marchante AN, Whalen PJ. Behavioural Brain Research. 2011. The structural and functional connectivity of the amygdala: From normal emotion to pathological anxiety. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kluver H, Bucy PC. “Psychic blindness” and other symptoms following bilateral temporal lobectomy in rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Physiology. 1937; 119 :352–353. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kotter R, Meyer N. The limbic system: a review of its empirical foundation. Behavioural Brain Research. 1992; 52 :105–127. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • LaBar KS. Emotional memory functions of the human amygdala. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports. 2003; 3 :363–364. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lakoff G. Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the mind. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press; 1987. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lanuza E, Belekhova M, Martinez-Marcos A, Font C, Martinez-Garcia F. Identification of the reptilian basolateral amygdala: an anatomical investigation of the afferents to the posterior dorsal ventricular ridge of the lizard Podarcis hispanica. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1998; 10 :3517–3534. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lazaro-Munoz G, LeDoux JE, Cain CK. Sidman instrumental avoidance initially depends on lateral and Basal amygdala and is constrained by central amygdala-mediated Pavlovian processes. Biological Psychiatry. 2010; 67 :1120–1127. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • LeDoux JE. Cognition and emotion: processing functions and brain systems. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience. New York: Plenum Publishing Corp; 1984. pp. 357–368. [ Google Scholar ]
  • LeDoux JE. Emotion. In: Plum F, editor. Handbook of Physiology. 1: The Nervous System. Vol V, Higher Functions of the Brain. Bethesda: American Physiological Society; 1987. pp. 419–460. [ Google Scholar ]
  • LeDoux JE. Emotion and the limbic system concept. Concepts in Neuroscience. 1991; 2 :169–199. [ Google Scholar ]
  • LeDoux JE. The Emotional Brain. New York: Simon and Schuster; 1996. [ Google Scholar ]
  • LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2000; 23 :155–184. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • LeDoux JE. Synaptic Self: How our brains become who we are. New York: Viking; 2002. [ Google Scholar ]
  • LeDoux J. The amygdala. Current Biology. 2007; 17 :R868–874. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • LeDoux JE. Emotional colouration of consciousness: how feelings come about. In: Weiskrantz L, Davies M, editors. Frontiers of Consciousness: Chichele Lectures. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2008. pp. 69–130. [ Google Scholar ]
  • LeDoux JE, Sakaguchi A, Reis DJ. Subcortical efferent projections of the medial geniculate nucleus mediate emotional responses conditioned to acoustic stimuli. Journal Neuroscience. 1984; 4 :683–698. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • LeDoux JE, Schiller D, Cain C. Emotional Reaction and Action: From Threat Processing to Goal-Directed Behavior. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The Cognitive Neurosciences. 4. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2009. pp. 905–924. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lucy JA. Linguistic Relativity. Annual Review of Anthropology. 1997; 26 :291–312. [ Google Scholar ]
  • MacLean PD. Psychosomatic disease and the “visceral brain”: recent developments bearing on the Papez theory of emotion. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1949; 11 :338–353. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • MacLean PD. Some psychiatric implications of physiological studies on frontotemporal portion of limbic system (visceral brain) Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1952; 4 :407–418. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • MacLean PD. The triune brain, emotion and scientific bias. In: Schmitt FO, editor. The Neurosciences: Second Study Program. New York: Rockefeller University Press; 1970. pp. 336–349. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Macnab RM, Koshland DE., Jr The gradient-sensing mechanism in bacterial chemotaxis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U S A. 1972; 69 :2509–2512. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maren S. Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2001; 24 :897–931. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maren S. Synaptic mechanisms of associative memory in the amygdala. Neuron. 2005; 47 :783–786. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Marks I. Fears, Phobias, and Rituals: Panic, Anxiety and Their Disorders. New York: Oxford University Press; 1987. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Martinez-Garcia F, Martinez-Marcos A, Lanuza E. The pallial amygdala of amniote vertebrates: evolution of the concept, evolution of the structure. Brain Research Bulletin. 2002; 57 :463–469. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Miczek KA, de Almeida RM, Kravitz EA, Rissman EF, de Boer SF, Raine A. Neurobiology of escalated aggression and violence. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007; 27 :11803–11806. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moreno N, Gonzalez A. Evolution of the amygdaloid complex in vertebrates, with special reference to the anamnio-amniotic transition. Journal of Anatomy. 2007; 211 :151–163. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Murray EA. The amygdala, reward and emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007; 11 :489–497. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nauta WJH, Karten HJ. A general profile of the vertebrate brain, with sidelights on the ancestry of cerebral cortex. In: Schmitt FO, editor. The Neurosciences: Second Study Program. New York: The Rockefeller University Press; 1970. pp. 7–26. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nishijo H, Hori E, Tazumi T, Ono T. Neural correlates to both emotion and cognitive functions in the monkey amygdala. Behavioural Brain Research. 2008; 188 :14–23. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Northcutt RG, Kaas JH. The emergence and evolution of mammalian neocortex. Trends in Neurosciences. 1995; 18 :373–379. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • O’Keefe J, Nadel L. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Clarendon Press; 1978. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ousdal OT, Jensen J, Server A, Hariri AR, Nakstad PH, Andreassen OA. The human amygdala is involved in general behavioral relevance detection: evidence from an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging Go-NoGo task. Neuroscience. 2008; 156 :450–455. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Panksepp J. Hypothalamic integration of behavior: rewards, punishments, and related psychological processes. In: Morgane PJ, Panksepp J, editors. Handbook of the Hypothalamus. Vol. 3, Behavioral Studies of the Hypothalamus. New York: Marcel Dekker; 1980. pp. 289–431. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Panksepp J. Affective Neuroscience. New York: Oxford U. Press; 1998. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Panksepp J. Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in animals and humans. Consciousness and Cognition. 2005; 14 :30–80. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pape HC, Pare D. Plastic synaptic networks of the amygdala for the acquisition, expression, and extinction of conditioned fear. Physiological Reviews. 2010; 90 :419–463. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Papez JW. A proposed mechanism of emotion. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry. 1937; 79 :217–224. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pfaff D. Hormone-driven mechanisms in the central nervous system facilitate the analysis of mammalian behaviours. Journal of Endocrinology. 2005; 184 :447–453. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Phelps EA. Emotion and cognition: insights from studies of the human amygdala. Annual Review of Psychology. 2006; 57 :27–53. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Phelps EA, LeDoux JE. Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron. 2005; 48 :175–187. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pitkänen A, Savander V, LeDoux JE. Organization of intra-amygdaloid circuitries in the rat: an emerging framework for understanding functions of the amygdala. Trends in Neuroscience. 1997; 20 :517–523. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Plutchik R. Emotion: A Psychoevolutionary Synthesis. New York: Harper & Row; 1980. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Preuss TM. Do rats have a prefrontal cortex? The Rose-Woolsey-Akerty Program Reconsidered. J Cog Neurosci. 1995; 7 :1–24. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Repa JC, Muller J, Apergis J, Desrochers TM, Zhou Y, LeDoux JE. Two different lateral amygdala cell populations contribute to the initiation and storage of memory. Nature Neuroscience. 2001; 4 :724–731. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rodrigues SM, Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. Molecular mechanisms underlying emotional learning and memory in the lateral amygdala. Neuron. 2004; 44 :75–91. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sah P, Westbrook RF, Luthi A. Fear conditioning and long-term potentiation in the amygdala: what really is the connection? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2008; 1129 :88–95. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Salzman CD, Paton JJ, Belova MA, Morrison SE. Flexible neural representations of value in the primate brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2007; 1121 :336–354. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sarter MF, Markowitsch HJ. Involvement of the amygdala in learning and memory: a critical review, with emphasis on anatomical relations. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1985; 99 :342–380. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schafe GE, Nader K, Blair HT, LeDoux JE. Memory consolidation of Pavlovian fear conditioning: a cellular and molecular perspective. Trends in Neuroscience. 2001; 24 :540–546. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sehlmeyer C, Schoning S, Zwitserlood P, Pfleiderer B, Kircher T, Arolt V, Konrad C. Human fear conditioning and extinction in neuroimaging: a systematic review. PLoS One. 2009; 4 :e5865. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Semendeferi K, Teffer K, Buxhoeveden DP, Park MS, Bludau S, Amunts K, Travis K, Buckwalter J. Spatial Organization of neurons in the frontal pole sets humans apart from great apes. Cerebral Cortex. 2010; 21 :1485–97. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shepherd G. Neurobiology. New York: Oxford; 1983. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Siegel A, Edinger H. Neural control of aggression and rage behavior. In: Morgane PJ, Panksepp J, editors. Handbook of the Hypothalamus, Vol. 3, Behavioral Studies of the Hypothalamus. New York: Marcel Dekker; 1981. pp. 203–240. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smith GE. The Evolution of Man. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 1924. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Squire L. Memory and Brain. New York: Oxford; 1987. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Swanson LW. The hippocampus and the concept of the limbic system. In: Seifert W, editor. Neurobiology of the Hippocampus. London: Academic Press; 1983. pp. 3–19. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Swanson LW. Brain Architecture: Understanding the Basic Plan. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thompson RF. The neurobiology of learning and memory. Science. 1986; 233 :941–947. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tischler MD, Davis M. A visual pathway that mediates fear-conditioned enhancement of acoustic startle. Brain Research. 1983; 276 :55–71. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tomkins SS. Affect, Imagery, Consciousness. New York: Springer; 1962. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weiskrantz L. Behavioral changes associated with ablation of the amygdaloid complex in monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1956; 49 :381–391. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weiss S. Forward frontal fields: phylogeny and fundamental function. Trends in Neuroscience. 2008; 31 :599–608. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Whalen PJ, Kagan J, Cook RG, Davis FC, Kim H, Polis S, McLaren DG, Somerville LH, McLean AA, Maxwell JS, Johnstone T. Human amygdala responsivity to masked fearful eye whites. Science. 2004; 306 :2061. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Whalen PJ, Phelps EA. The human amgydala. New York: Guilford Press; 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
  • DOI: 10.5860/choice.192178
  • Corpus ID: 143300546

The History of Emotions: An Introduction

  • Jan Plamper
  • Published 22 March 2015
  • Psychology, History, Political Science, Philosophy, Art

169 Citations

The history of emotions: past, present, future, histories of emotion in communist and post-communist europe after 1945, labels, rationality, and the chemistry of the mind: moors in historical context, introduction to the edward elgar research handbook on law and emotion, introductory essay: emotion, affect, and the eighteenth century, an interview with jan plamper: on the history of emotions, history looks forward: interdisciplinarity and critical emotion research, emotions and the global politics of childhood, enacting musical emotions. sense-making, dynamic systems, and the embodied mind, 3 references, are emotions a kind of practice (and is that what makes them have a history) a bourdieuian approach to understanding emotion, against constructionism: the historical ethnography of emotions, the affective turn: historicising the emotions, related papers.

Showing 1 through 3 of 0 Related Papers

  • Shopping Cart

Advanced Search

  • Browse Our Shelves
  • Best Sellers
  • Digital Audiobooks
  • Featured Titles
  • New This Week
  • Staff Recommended
  • Reading Lists
  • Upcoming Events
  • Ticketed Events
  • Science Book Talks
  • Past Events
  • Video Archive
  • Online Gift Codes
  • University Clothing
  • Goods & Gifts from Harvard Book Store
  • Hours & Directions
  • Newsletter Archive
  • Frequent Buyer Program
  • Signed First Edition Club
  • Signed New Voices in Fiction Club
  • Off-Site Book Sales
  • Corporate & Special Sales
  • Print on Demand

Harvard Book Store

Our Shelves
  • All Our Shelves
  • Academic New Arrivals
  • New Hardcover - Biography
  • New Hardcover - Fiction
  • New Hardcover - Nonfiction
  • New Titles - Paperback
  • African American Studies
  • Anthologies
  • Anthropology / Archaeology
  • Architecture
  • Asia & The Pacific
  • Astronomy / Geology
  • Boston / Cambridge / New England
  • Business & Management
  • Career Guides
  • Child Care / Childbirth / Adoption
  • Children's Board Books
  • Children's Picture Books
  • Children's Activity Books
  • Children's Beginning Readers
  • Children's Middle Grade
  • Children's Gift Books
  • Children's Nonfiction
  • Children's/Teen Graphic Novels
  • Teen Nonfiction
  • Young Adult
  • Classical Studies
  • Cognitive Science / Linguistics
  • College Guides
  • Cultural & Critical Theory
  • Education - Higher Ed
  • Environment / Sustainablity
  • European History
  • Exam Preps / Outlines
  • Games & Hobbies
  • Gender Studies / Gay & Lesbian
  • Gift / Seasonal Books
  • Globalization
  • Graphic Novels
  • Hardcover Classics
  • Health / Fitness / Med Ref
  • Islamic Studies
  • Large Print
  • Latin America / Caribbean
  • Law & Legal Issues
  • Literary Crit & Biography
  • Local Economy
  • Mathematics
  • Media Studies
  • Middle East
  • Myths / Tales / Legends
  • Native American
  • Paperback Favorites
  • Performing Arts / Acting
  • Personal Finance
  • Personal Growth
  • Photography
  • Physics / Chemistry
  • Poetry Criticism
  • Ref / English Lang Dict & Thes
  • Ref / Foreign Lang Dict / Phrase
  • Reference - General
  • Religion - Christianity
  • Religion - Comparative
  • Religion - Eastern
  • Romance & Erotica
  • Science Fiction
  • Short Introductions
  • Technology, Culture & Media
  • Theology / Religious Studies
  • Travel Atlases & Maps
  • Travel Lit / Adventure
  • Urban Studies
  • Wines And Spirits
  • Women's Studies
  • World History
  • Writing Style And Publishing
Gift Cards

Add to Cart

A Human History of Emotion: How the Way We Feel Built the World We Know

A sweeping exploration of the ways in which emotions shaped the course of human history, and how our experience and understanding of emotions have evolved along with us. "Eye-opening and thought-provoking!” (Gina Rippon, author of  The Gendered Brain )

We humans like to think of ourselves as rational creatures, who, as a species, have relied on calculation and intellect to survive. But many of the most important moments in our history had little to do with cold, hard facts and a lot to do with feelings. Events ranging from the origins of philosophy to the birth of the world’s major religions, the fall of Rome, the Scientific Revolution, and some of the bloodiest wars that humanity has ever experienced can’t be properly understood without understanding emotions. Drawing on psychology, neuroscience, philosophy, art, and religious history, Richard Firth-Godbehere takes readers on a fascinating and wide ranging tour of the central and often under-appreciated role emotions have played in human societies around the world and throughout history—from Ancient Greece to Gambia, Japan, the Ottoman Empire, the United States, and beyond.   A Human History of Emotion  vividly illustrates how our understanding and experience of emotions has changed over time, and how our beliefs about feelings—and our feelings themselves—profoundly shaped us and the world we inhabit.

There are no customer reviews for this item yet.

Classic Totes

the history of human emotions essay

Tote bags and pouches in a variety of styles, sizes, and designs , plus mugs, bookmarks, and more!

Shipping & Pickup

the history of human emotions essay

We ship anywhere in the U.S. and orders of $75+ ship free via media mail!

Noteworthy Signed Books: Join the Club!

the history of human emotions essay

Join our Signed First Edition Club (or give a gift subscription) for a signed book of great literary merit, delivered to you monthly.

Harvard Book Store

Harvard Square's Independent Bookstore

© 2024 Harvard Book Store All rights reserved

Contact Harvard Book Store 1256 Massachusetts Avenue Cambridge, MA 02138

Tel (617) 661-1515 Toll Free (800) 542-READ Email [email protected]

View our current hours »

Join our bookselling team »

We plan to remain closed to the public for two weeks, through Saturday, March 28 While our doors are closed, we plan to staff our phones, email, and harvard.com web order services from 10am to 6pm daily.

Store Hours Monday - Saturday: 9am - 11pm Sunday: 10am - 10pm

Holiday Hours 12/24: 9am - 7pm 12/25: closed 12/31: 9am - 9pm 1/1: 12pm - 11pm All other hours as usual.

Map Find Harvard Book Store »

Online Customer Service Shipping » Online Returns » Privacy Policy »

Harvard University harvard.edu »

Facebook

  • Clubs & Services

the history of human emotions essay

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

What is the History of Emotions?

Profile image of Barbara H Rosenwein

2019, Social History

Related Papers

Sharon Crozier-De Rosa

The Introduction to this volume introduces students to the broad field of the history of emotion, touching on its development over the last several decades, and its key dimensions in current scholarship. It then explains the structure and logic of the volume, providing hints about how it can be used by students and scholars new to the field. A final section reflects on the limitations and gaps in the volume and field more widely, and the ways that they may be filled by future research.

the history of human emotions essay

Erin Sullivan

Penelope Gouk , Helen Hills

Emotions in Social Life: Social Theories and …

Alexander Dean

In 1941 historian Lucien Febvre challenged historians to reflect upon the emotions, stating that no historian concerned with the social life of individuals can any longer disregard their importance . Historians were quick to see, as Joanna Bourke states, that examination of the “transformations undergone by emotions within societies could provide a unique insight into everyday life .” However, the Primary problem facing historians has been how to define emotions to enable a rigorous academic study . If emotions are to have such a thing, then the historian is behoved to seek out and propose suitable methods to achieve such ends. This essay will seek to review the number of different methodological approaches that have been developed to fulfil this requirement and how such work has gleamed new historical insight. First by looking at how it has been suggested we may correctly interpret the cultural meaning of emotion from the past, from our modern vantage point. This in turn requires a review of the debates surrounding how it is proposed historical analysis of emotions be carried out, or if it can at all. Finally, exploring two fundamental methodological concepts central to the history of the emotions, by using as means of analysis the history such methods have produced, the essay will demonstrate how such studies of emotion can contribute to the wider academic practice.

Andrew Kettler

What Is the History of Emotions? provides historiographical narratives of the roots, current theories, and future goals for this well-established field of historical study. As part of the What Is History? series from Polity Press, which offers introductions to specific historical subfields, this edition provides lessons that can easily be used by graduate students and early career scholars who desire quick references to both the sturdy and wobbly columns that uphold the architecture of the history of emotions.

Passions in Context, No. 1 © Stiftung Einstein Forum https://www.passionsincontext.de/

Barbara H Rosenwein

What are some of the general methodological issues involved in writing a history of the emotions? Before answering this question, we need to address a major problem. If emotions are, as many scientists think, biological entities, universal within all human populations, do they-indeed can they-have much of a history at all? Once it is determined that they are less universal than claimed (without denying their somatic substratum), a host of problems and opportunities for the history of emotions emerge. In this paper, I propose that we study the emotions of the past by considering "emotional communi-ties" (briefly: social groups whose members adhere to the same valuations of emotions and their expression). I argue that we should take into consideration the full panoply of sources that these groups produced, and I suggest how we might most effectively interpret those sources. Finally, I consider how and why emotional change takes place, urging that the history of emotions be integrated into other sorts of histories-social, political, and intellectual .

Emotion Researcher

Riccardo Cristiani , Barbara H Rosenwein

A conversation about the history of emotions with Nicole Eustace, Eugenia Lean, Julie Livingston, Jan Plamper, William Reddy, Barbara H. Rosenwein

Margaret Mullett & Susan Ashbrook Harvey (eds) Managing Emotion: Passions, Affects and Imaginings in Byzantium

Maria G. Xanthou FHEA

In 1941 Lucien Febvre officially launched ‘history of emotions’ as a discipline on its own right. Ever since 1941, it has evolved into an increasingly productive and stimulating area of historical research. In the ’80s the ‘emotional’ or ‘affective’ turn was in full swing: historians interested in the history of society and mentalities in Medieval Europe and in the western world focusing on emotions. The resurgence of interest and its subsequent twenty year study of the interaction between historical conditions and the manifestations of emotions in Greek, Roman and Byzantine texts, images and material culture have already yielded its ἀπαρχαί as regards its set goal. In this thriving discipline, the historian’s task has been defined as follows: to examine the very diverse significance of emotions in society and culture in their broadest definitions (including religion, law, politics, etc.) [Chaniotis 2012, 15]. The discipline is based on the assumption that not only the way the emotions are perceived, namely feelings, but also the feelings themselves are learned. Culture and history are changing and so are feelings as well as their expression. The social relevance and potency of emotions is historically and culturally variable. In the view of many historians, emotion is, therefore, just as fundamental a category of history, as class, race or gender. Over the past years a wide array of methodological approaches have been discussed. A particular school of thought focuses on the historical analysis of emotional norms and rules under the heading of emotionology. In recent years, the history of emotions intersected with modern historiographical approaches such as conceptual history, historical constructivism and the history of the body. Thus, the methodological spectrum of the history of emotions has been enriched by performative, constructivist and practice theory approaches. Currently fundamental methodological concepts include emotive, emotional habitus and emotional practice. Additionally, there are several terms that describe the different scope and binding effect of feeling cultures such as emotional community, emotional regime and emotional style. My aim is to provide a succinct survey of the most important methodological approaches, as they have been already applied in the study of emotions in Greek, Roman and Byzantine texts, images and material culture.

Loading Preview

Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.

RELATED PAPERS

Russell Foster

Phil Hutchinson

Douglas Cairns

Clara Molina

Richard de Koster

"Introduction: Emotional Histories — Beyond the Personalization of the Past and the Abstraction of Affect Theory," Exemplaria 26.1 (2014): 3015"

Stephanie J Trigg

Sociological Forum

Rebecca A Allahyari

Patrick S. O'Donnell

Canadian Bulletin of Medical History Bulletin Canadien D Histoire De La Medecine

Faith Wallis

Cognition & Emotion

Gerrod Parrott

Fayah Haussker

South African Journal of Psychology

Johann Louw

Sherman Tan

Human Affairs: Postdisciplinary Humanities & Social Sciences Quarterly

Eduard Moreno Gabriel

Ramazan Aras

Social Psychology Quarterly

Piotr Winkielman

The Oxford Handbook of Affective Computing

Arvid Kappas

Revista de Estudios Sociales

Rob Boddice , Revista de Estudios Sociales

Aaron Ben-Ze'ev

Psychological Concepts

Louise K.W. Sundararajan

A Companion to Psychological Anthropology: Modernity and Psychocultural Change. Eds. C. Casey and R. Edgerton. Oxford. Basil Blackwell.

Charles Lindholm

Ida Caiazza

Joerg Wettlaufer

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

History of Emotions

After sixteen successful years of research, the Center for the History of Emotions will conclude its work on 30 June 2024 . Our thanks go to the many scholars who shared their ideas and knowledge , were guest researchers, gave lectures or explored new avenues of the history of emotions with us as collaboration partners.

Do emotions have a history? And do they make history? These are the questions that the Research Center History of Emotions seeks to answer. To explore the emotional orders of the past, historians work closely with psychologists and education specialists. In addition, they draw on the expertise of anthropologists, sociologists, musicologists and scholars working on literature and art. Our research rests on the assumption that emotions – feelings and their expressions – are shaped by culture and learnt/acquired in social contexts. What somebody can and may feel (and show) in a given situation, towards certain people or things, depends on social norms and rules. It is thus historically variable and open to change.

A central objective of the Research Center is to trace and analyse the changing norms and rules of feeling. We therefore look at different societies and see how they develop and organise their emotional regimes, codes, and lexicons. Research concentrates on the modern period (18th to 20th centuries). Geographically, it includes both western and eastern societies (Europe, North America and South Asia). Special attention is paid to institutions that have a strong impact on human behaviour and its emotional underpinnings, such as the family, law, religion, the military, the state. Equally important to the Center's research programme is the historical significance of emotions. Emotions are said to motivate human action and thus influence social, political, and economic developments. In this capacity, they are and have been a privileged object of manipulation and instrumentalisation. Who appealed to what kind of emotions for what reasons? To what degree did emotions play a part in/contribute to the formation and dissolution of social groups, communities and movements? These and other questions open doors to a new field of research, one which aims to thoroughly historicise a crucial element of human development.

Conferences & Calls for Papers

Conferences & Calls for Papers

Colloquium

Monographs and Editions

In the Media

In the Media

Webportal

Last updated 27/06/24: Online ordering is currently unavailable due to technical issues. We apologise for any delays responding to customers while we resolve this. For further updates please visit our website: https://www.cambridge.org/news-and-insights/technical-incident

We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings .

Login Alert

the history of human emotions essay

  • < Back to search results
  • A History of Emotions, 1200–1800

A History of Emotions, 1200–1800

the history of human emotions essay

  • This book is no longer available to purchase from Cambridge Core
  • Edited by Jonas Liliequist
  • Export citation

Book description

The history of emotions is an expanding field of research. The essays in this collection examine emotional responses to art and music, the role of emotions in contemporary notions of gender and sexuality and theoretical questions as to their use. Bringing together a series of case studies from points across the medieval and early modern periods, the authors in this volume provide fascinating glimpses into human emotional experience across a variety of cultures.

"‘From this collection we discover that emotions were the objects of constant vigilance and effort, shaped by ever changing ideas and norms concerning the self, the holy, the mind, the arts, friendship, sickness and health. Overall, a fascinating sampling of the best recent work in the history of emotions, framed by a strong introduction and a provocative methodological essay by Barbara H Rosenwein.’"

  • Aa Reduce text
  • Aa Enlarge text

Refine List

Actions for selected content:.

  • View selected items
  • Save to my bookmarks
  • Export citations
  • Download PDF (zip)
  • Save to Kindle
  • Save to Dropbox
  • Save to Google Drive

Save content to

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to .

To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle .

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service .

Save Search

You can save your searches here and later view and run them again in "My saved searches".

Frontmatter pp i-iv

Contents pp v-vi, list of contributors pp vii-x, list of figures pp xi-xii, introduction pp 1-6.

  • By Jonas Liliequist , Umeå University

I - Theoretical Issues

1 - theories of change in the history of emotions pp 7-20.

  • By Barbara H. Rosenwein , Loyola University Chicago

2 - Ottoman Love: Preface to a Theory of Emotional Ecology pp 21-48

  • By Walter Andrews , University of Michigan

II - Emotional Repertoires

3 - preachers, saints, and sinners: emotional repertoires in high medieval religious role models pp 49-64.

  • By Christina Lutter , University of Vienna

4 - Theology and Interiority: Emotions as Evidence of the Working of Grace in Elizabethan and Stuart Conversion Narratives pp 65-78

  • By Paola Baseotto , Insubria University

5 - ‘Finer’ Feelings: Sociability, Sensibility and the Emotions of Gens de Lettres in Eighteenth-Century France pp 79-94

  • By Anne C. Vila , University of Wisconsin-Madison

III - Music and Art

6 - music as wonder and delight: construction of gender in early modern opera through musical representation and arousal of emotions pp 95-104.

  • By Johanna Ethnersson Pontara , Stockholm University

7 - Politesse and Sprezzatura : The History of Emotions in the Art of Antoine Watteau pp 105-118

  • By Pamela W. Whedon , University of North Carolina

IV - Gender, Sexuality and the Body

8 - emotions and gender: the case of anger in early modern english revenge tragedies pp 119-134.

  • By Kristine Steenbergh , Utrecht University

9 - Beauty, Masculinity and Love Between Men: Configuring Emotions with Michael Drayton's Peirs Gaveston pp 135-152

  • By Anu Korhonen , University of Helsinki

10 - ‘Pray, Dr, is there Reason to Fear a Cancer?’ Fear of Breast Cancer in Early Modern Britain pp 153-166

  • By Marjo Kaartinen , University of Turku

V - Uses of Emotions

11 - the little girl who could not stop crying: the use of emotions as signifiers of true conversion in eighteenth-century greenland pp 167-180.

  • By Allan Sortkær , Aarhus University

12 - The Political Rhetoric of Tears in Early Modern Sweden pp 181-206

Notes pp 207-250, index pp 251-259, full text views.

Full text views reflects the number of PDF downloads, PDFs sent to Google Drive, Dropbox and Kindle and HTML full text views for chapters in this book.

Book summary page views

Book summary views reflect the number of visits to the book and chapter landing pages.

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between #date#. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed.

the history of human emotions essay

TED is supported by ads and partners 00:00

The history of human emotions

  • communication
  • personal growth
  • Search Menu

Sign in through your institution

  • Advance articles
  • AHR Interview
  • History Unclassified
  • Submission Guidelines
  • Submission Site
  • Join the AHR Community
  • About The American Historical Review
  • About the American Historical Association
  • AHR Staff & Editors
  • Advertising and Corporate Services
  • Self-Archiving Policy
  • Dispatch Dates
  • Journals on Oxford Academic
  • Books on Oxford Academic

American Historical Association

  • < Previous

AHR Conversation: The Historical Study of Emotions

Nicole Eustace is Associate Professor of History and Co-Director of the Atlantic History Program at New York University. She received her B.A. from Yale University and her Ph.D. from the University of Pennsylvania, where she received her training as a historian of eighteenth-century British America and the early United States. Her works include 1812: War and the Passions of Patriotism (University of Pennsylvania Press, 2012) and Passion Is the Gale: Emotion, Power, and the Coming of the American Revolution (University of North Carolina Press, 2008), as well as articles and essays in the William & Mary Quarterly , the Journal of Social History , the Journal of American History , and numerous encyclopedias and anthologies.

  • Article contents
  • Figures & tables
  • Supplementary Data

Nicole Eustace, Eugenia Lean, Julie Livingston, Jan Plamper, William M. Reddy, Barbara H. Rosenwein, AHR Conversation: The Historical Study of Emotions, The American Historical Review , Volume 117, Issue 5, December 2012, Pages 1487–1531, https://doi.org/10.1093/ahr/117.5.1487

  • Permissions Icon Permissions

In the past few years, the AHR has published five “Conversations,” each on a subject of interest to a wide range of historians: “On Transnational History” (2006), “Religious Identities and Violence” (2007), “Environmental Historians and Environmental Crisis” (2008), “Historians and the Study of Material Culture” (2009), and “Historical Perspectives on the Circulation of Information” (2011). For each the process has been the same: the Editor convenes a group of scholars with an interest in the topic who, via e-mail over the course of several months, conduct a conversation, which is then lightly edited and footnoted, finally appearing in the December issue. The goal has been to provide readers with a wide-ranging consideration of a topic at a high level of expertise, in which the participants are recruited across several fields and periods. It is the sort of publishing project that this journal is uniquely positioned to undertake.

This year's topic is “The Historical Study of Emotions.” Given the expanding range of new methodological and disciplinary forays among historians in recent years, it may be too much to proclaim that we are witnessing an “Emotional Turn.” (Indeed, one conclusion that might be drawn from the June 2012 AHR Forum on “Historiographic ‘Turns’ in Critical Perspective” is that the very concept of “turns” may have outlived its intellectual usefulness.) But it cannot be denied that the study of emotions has become a thriving pursuit among historians from many different areas and periods. Not only does its emergence as a legitimate subfield foster the interrogation of descriptors—love, hate, resentment, passions, pity, happiness, and the like—that historians are more often likely to use uncritically if at all; it also allows for the crossing of the boundaries between private and public, the personal and collective, that often constrain our work. To be sure, a major problem with the study of emotions is how we can access the emotional lives of people in past times: is it possible to go beyond emotional expressions —usually conveyed in language—and attain some assurance that these are indicative of actual emotional states ? This is only one of the questions confronted in the course of this wide-ranging conversation.

Joining the Editor in this conversation are Nicole Eustace, a historian of early America; Eugenia Lean, who studies modern Chinese history, literature, and culture; Julie Livingston, a historian who works in Africa; Jan Plamper, a specialist in Soviet and Russian history; William M. Reddy, who has written widely on modern European as well as comparative history; and Barbara H. Rosenwein, whose research has ranged across the religious, social, and intellectual history of the Middle Ages.

American Historical Association

American Historical Association members

Personal account.

  • Sign in with email/username & password
  • Get email alerts
  • Save searches
  • Purchase content
  • Activate your purchase/trial code
  • Add your ORCID iD

Institutional access

Sign in with a library card.

  • Sign in with username/password
  • Recommend to your librarian
  • Institutional account management
  • Get help with access

Access to content on Oxford Academic is often provided through institutional subscriptions and purchases. If you are a member of an institution with an active account, you may be able to access content in one of the following ways:

IP based access

Typically, access is provided across an institutional network to a range of IP addresses. This authentication occurs automatically, and it is not possible to sign out of an IP authenticated account.

Choose this option to get remote access when outside your institution. Shibboleth/Open Athens technology is used to provide single sign-on between your institution’s website and Oxford Academic.

  • Click Sign in through your institution.
  • Select your institution from the list provided, which will take you to your institution's website to sign in.
  • When on the institution site, please use the credentials provided by your institution. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.
  • Following successful sign in, you will be returned to Oxford Academic.

If your institution is not listed or you cannot sign in to your institution’s website, please contact your librarian or administrator.

Enter your library card number to sign in. If you cannot sign in, please contact your librarian.

Society Members

Society member access to a journal is achieved in one of the following ways:

Sign in through society site

Many societies offer single sign-on between the society website and Oxford Academic. If you see ‘Sign in through society site’ in the sign in pane within a journal:

  • Click Sign in through society site.
  • When on the society site, please use the credentials provided by that society. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.

If you do not have a society account or have forgotten your username or password, please contact your society.

Sign in using a personal account

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members. See below.

A personal account can be used to get email alerts, save searches, purchase content, and activate subscriptions.

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members.

Viewing your signed in accounts

Click the account icon in the top right to:

  • View your signed in personal account and access account management features.
  • View the institutional accounts that are providing access.

Signed in but can't access content

Oxford Academic is home to a wide variety of products. The institutional subscription may not cover the content that you are trying to access. If you believe you should have access to that content, please contact your librarian.

For librarians and administrators, your personal account also provides access to institutional account management. Here you will find options to view and activate subscriptions, manage institutional settings and access options, access usage statistics, and more.

Short-term Access

To purchase short-term access, please sign in to your personal account above.

Don't already have a personal account? Register

Month: Total Views:
November 2016 1
December 2016 8
January 2017 43
February 2017 100
March 2017 141
April 2017 71
May 2017 215
June 2017 117
July 2017 102
August 2017 96
September 2017 107
October 2017 112
November 2017 251
December 2017 662
January 2018 441
February 2018 423
March 2018 356
April 2018 130
May 2018 174
June 2018 90
July 2018 86
August 2018 114
September 2018 331
October 2018 304
November 2018 353
December 2018 135
January 2019 72
February 2019 81
March 2019 104
April 2019 61
May 2019 85
June 2019 45
July 2019 40
August 2019 56
September 2019 74
October 2019 74
November 2019 81
December 2019 61
January 2020 93
February 2020 54
March 2020 57
April 2020 67
May 2020 36
June 2020 15
July 2020 95
August 2020 175
September 2020 206
October 2020 186
November 2020 255
December 2020 219
January 2021 201
February 2021 162
March 2021 216
April 2021 194
May 2021 187
June 2021 146
July 2021 142
August 2021 161
September 2021 168
October 2021 245
November 2021 209
December 2021 188
January 2022 251
February 2022 176
March 2022 178
April 2022 184
May 2022 201
June 2022 138
July 2022 99
August 2022 121
September 2022 194
October 2022 213
November 2022 164
December 2022 150
January 2023 183
February 2023 161
March 2023 189
April 2023 172
May 2023 135
June 2023 110
July 2023 130
August 2023 154
September 2023 180
October 2023 204
November 2023 270
December 2023 153
January 2024 230
February 2024 171
March 2024 360
April 2024 302
May 2024 188
June 2024 140

Email alerts

Citing articles via.

  • Editorial Board
  • Author Guidelines
  • Recommend to your Library

Affiliations

  • Online ISSN 1937-5239
  • Print ISSN 0002-8762
  • Copyright © 2024 The American Historical Association
  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Rights and permissions
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

Change Password

Your password must have 6 characters or more:.

  • a lower case character, 
  • an upper case character, 
  • a special character 

Password Changed Successfully

Your password has been changed

Create your account

Forget yout password.

Enter your email address below and we will send you the reset instructions

If the address matches an existing account you will receive an email with instructions to reset your password

Forgot your Username?

Enter your email address below and we will send you your username

If the address matches an existing account you will receive an email with instructions to retrieve your username

Psychiatry Online

  • June 01, 2024 | VOL. 181, NO. 6 CURRENT ISSUE pp.461-564
  • May 01, 2024 | VOL. 181, NO. 5 pp.347-460
  • April 01, 2024 | VOL. 181, NO. 4 pp.255-346
  • March 01, 2024 | VOL. 181, NO. 3 pp.171-254
  • February 01, 2024 | VOL. 181, NO. 2 pp.83-170
  • January 01, 2024 | VOL. 181, NO. 1 pp.1-82

The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use , including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

Why We Feel: The Science of Human Emotions

  • PROF. DR. CHISTOPH MUNDT ,

Search for more papers by this author

Nothing in our subjective self-experience gives us so much the impression of individuality and uniqueness as our feelings. Not only poetry but art in general has elevated the realm of emotions to a world of its own, which seems to exist of itself without purpose.

Victor Johnston makes us abandon this view with his biopsychological theory of emotions. He points out that the fact that we have feelings and the way we feel are relentlessly determined by the evolutionary selection process. Evolution has refined our system of appraisal of our perceptions, ultimately according to reproduction advantages. Johnston’s rigorous theorizing by means of Darwinian learning principles dismantles human narcissistic illusions of the uniqueness and grandeur of our moods, emotions, affects, and their reflection. Feelings appear as mere by-products of an ever self-refining system for maximum gene survival. This painfully plausible book makes it difficult to hold anything against it; therefore, those who by all means want to stick to man’s uniqueness in creation due to our emotional life should be warned against reading it.

The informed reader may consider the entry to the book somewhat tedious up to the chapter about the basics of learning mechanisms in adaptive biological systems that have survived the evolutionary selection process. Johnston starts off with Socrates’ cave metaphor: what we see as “green” has no such quality equivalent in nature but is entirely made up of perceptual appraisals generated in our brain. The seemingly arbitrary selection, for example, of a tiny band of electromagnetic wave frequencies for our perception of green has been determined by the survival benefit of those biological systems which could sense the green plant for food. Very small differences between frequency bands have to be exaggerated by the “proximal valuing system,” whereas a vast amount of information about other wave frequencies remains unnoticed because it is irrelevant for the reproduction advantage. This means that there is greenness neither in nature nor in our brain. Greenness exists only as “emergent property,” which is a tool for the evolutionary selection process. It just happens to emerge from the development of the organism according to its survival benefit. This emergent property can be submitted to further evolutionary molding.

The basic learning principles extracted by the author, mainly from well-known working mechanisms of the immune system and from other paradigms of evolutionary learning, include the following:

1. The system has to offer to the outside world a random variation of selectable or moldable tools—“emergent properties” like antibody proteins, organ sensations, or feelings—for selection by the evolution process. Randomness of the variations is the most essential principle. Adaptiveness and creativity, the ability to transcend the learning system itself, depend on it.

2. The repetition of this process by “entangled learning processes” of different degrees of freedom entails phylogenetic learning for long-term stability (adaptation to the day-night cycle, for example), ontogenetic learning (as in the morphological and neural network organization of the brain) to adapt to medium-range stable environmental cues like food, and functional adaptability to very volatile short-living cues for adaptation (like mimic changes in the face of a mate).

3. Steering the degree of variation at its optimum between preservation of ascertained adaptive functions and instigation of the evolutionary process by increasing the number of variations presented to the outside world for selection is another important principle, recently exemplified with the heat proteins (see, for example, the disputation between Gould and Dawkins provided by Groß [1] ).

Johnston applies his generalized Darwinian learning theory to the development of a novel method to gain the most adequate facial composite to track a criminal. Instead of asking a witness to describe the face of the criminal, Johnston offered a random sample of faces of which the witness had to select the one that best resembles the suspect. Of this, another sample of random variations was generated, and so forth. The result turned out to be far more accurate than those faces which emerged from the usual description procedure.

Transferring the principles of the evolutionary learning theory to the world of feelings means, first, that feelings—i.e., appraisals—are considered nothing else than “emergent properties” that the evolutionary selection acts upon. Johnston considers feelings to be “proximate values” whose task has become to steer the “two-loop” (phylogenetic and ontogenetic) learning model by selecting the best-fitting hypothesis from several hypotheses generated by the sensory input. From the establishment of lasting behavior patterns up to more volatile memory storage, feelings decide what is kept and what is let go according to the reproduction advantage. The tuning spectrum of hedonic versus unhedonic does it. Johnston exemplifies this with many amusing observations and, in the end, with an artificial intelligence creature, a dog-like sniffer whose sensory and feeling systems decide whether it can sniff successfully to get hold of its prey or runs into starvation.

One most disenchanting for human narcissism but most plausible example of Darwinian learning theory applied to emotions is Johnston’s work on beauty and erotic partner selection, for which he won a scientific award. To prove that our feelings for beauty and sex appeal do not follow arbitrary individual patterns but the stern rules of the “net reproduction gain,” Johnston invited the world to choose the most attractive female and male faces of those he posted on the World Wide Web. Statistical evaluation demonstrated that from full lips (due to little fat cushions after estrogen rise in puberty) to the hip-waist ratio for the pelvis most likely to deliver without complications, every sign of sexual attraction is in fact related to the “net reproduction gain,” i.e., the likelihood to win the most fertile and successfully delivering woman. The attractiveness of male faces seemed to rest mainly with signs of health and physical strength. Furthermore, according to the selection experiment, our feelings are tuned to the age of highest fertility. Again, these cues cause exaggerated feelings, in the sense of a “discriminant amplifier,” to waste not even a minimal reproduction advantage.

The same learning principles underlie memory. Johnston claims that memory stores only experience related to events that matter for biological survival. Memories work by indexing episodes according to their hedonic tone—the basis for man’s reasoning on future events. The ability of “mind reading” is based on the comparison of currently perceived and formerly experienced social situations and their past and presumptive future social sequelae. It is obvious that this ability has an enormous impact on the competence to adjust to and influence other individuals’ feelings and hence to improve the reproduction rate.

Social feelings have played an increasing role in evolution for man’s reproduction advantage. Experimental games like “the prisoner’s dilemma” suggest that modesty and cooperation may do better than directly acting on the individual’s instant advantage. Johnston describes a set of feelings particularly adjusted to this somewhat paradoxical condition: an elaborate emotional monitoring system for the mutuality of affection, protection, and concern. If the reciprocity is broken by recklessness, a “pop out feeling” indicates the loss of the “investment”—hopefully not too late. In addition to anger, “pop out emotions” are fear and love—Johnston comments at length on their serendipity and their potential reproduction advantages. Emotional framing for decisions by a hedonic tone is also seen in the context of gene survival. The group size, for example, plays a crucial role in the amount of reciprocal interactions of social altruism offered by the group members to each other. Johnston quotes studies that relate the maximum group size allowing for altruism to the size of hunting bands in prehistoric times—the root of an adjustment that may no longer fit, like many others: the “naturalist’s fallacy.” The nepotistic, oligarchic regimes in some countries may resemble these mechanisms, however.

Johnston’s book is both amusing and plausible, but its basic theories are not novel. For the clinician, the harvest of the great factual knowledge in this book is not as rich as it is for the theoretician and biologist. The mechanisms of learning, the language of emotions in primary relationships, and the consequences of its failure or distortion (2) do not play major roles in the book, although it starts with a clinical case vignette of schizophrenia. The neurophysiology of brain systems that generate and process emotions is touched upon only very briefly. There is a vast body of research that strives to disentangle the universally stable basic patterns of emotions from their individually changeable parts (3 – 5) . The microsocial and individual developments of distorted patterns of emotional language dealt with in trauma research could also build a bridge to clinical aspects, which obviously was not an intention of the author.

What lasts as a stimulating and original message of the book for the clinician is the generalization of principles of the Darwinian transgenerational “learning theory” concerning DNA under the selection process to other biological systems in general. Although DNA does not “learn” but just happens to change under the pressure of selection, the task of balancing the preservation of functions and their modification is the same in truly learning biological systems. Johnston’s application of the “Russian doll” learning processes to neuropsychology seems promising.

The strength of this book, namely its rigorous Darwinism and the relentless uncovering of what is seen as the ultimate purpose of feeling, nevertheless causes disillusionment and even disenchantment. Historical comparisons come to mind. After the overstretched adoration of rationality in the late enlightenment toward the end of the eighteenth century, when reason was put on the altar, the Sturm and Drang period swept away logic and returned to the realm of feelings as a colorful but less calculable world in its own right. Johnston has not indulged in those worlds which are best represented by art and religion. Certainly he would find “net-reproductive gains” in societies with superior ethical and religious systems; and fine arts may be considered tentative appraisals that at some time will lead to “emergent properties,” but a little bit of illusion may also be necessary to survive.

By Victor S. Johnston. Cambridge, Mass., Perseus Books, 1999, 224 pp., $26.00.

1. Groß M: Hitzeschockprotein hilft der Evolution auf die Sprünge: Spektrum der Wissenschaft. Scientific American German Edition 1999, pp 12–16 Google Scholar

2. Harris PL: The child’s understanding of emotion: developmental change and the family environment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1994 ; 35:3–28 Crossref , Medline ,  Google Scholar

3. Russell JA: Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expression? a review of the cross-cultural studies. Psychol Bull 1994 ; 115:102–141 Crossref , Medline ,  Google Scholar

4. Ekman P: Strong evidence for universals in facial expressions: a reply to Russell’s mistaken critique. Psychol Bull 1994 ; 115:268–287 Crossref , Medline ,  Google Scholar

5. Izard CE: Innate and universal facial expressions: evidence from developmental and cross-cultural research. Psychol Bull 1994 ; 115:288–391 Crossref , Medline ,  Google Scholar

  • Data Analysis Tools for Neural Data Cognitive Computing
  • Introduction to Cognitive Computing 3 May 2021

the history of human emotions essay

The History of Emotions Blog

Conversations about the history of feeling from www.qmul.ac.uk/emotions.

The History of Emotions Blog

What is the history of emotions? Part II

What is the history of emotions? And why does it matter?

As part of our first anniversary at the History of Emotions blog, I asked members and friends of the Centre for the History of the Emotions to write a paragraph or two on what they thought about the history of emotions, how they got involved in it, why it mattered, and what were their favourite publications in the field.

The dozen answers below range widely. Several are by PhD students here at Queen Mary, others are by established academics. Recurring themes include the relationship between scientific and humanistic attempts to get to grips with human mental life. The recommended reading (and viewing) includes some classics in the history of emotions and some more surprising items too from the fields of philosophy and anthropology.

This series of mini-posts starts with some warm words of support from the incoming Head of the School of History at Queen Mary, Professor Miri Rubin.  

Miri Rubin Professor of Medieval and Early Modern History, QMUL

Author of: Emotion and Devotion: The Meanings of Mary in Medieval Culture

Having the Centre for the History of Emotions, and its lively blog, among us in the School of History has been a real inspiration. We have become better historians, for being reminded of the importance – in all that we study – of tears and fears, hopes and mopes. Emotions-awareness does not redefine what we do, but it enhances our understanding and refines our sensibilities. It also makes us feel part of a vast network of lively and interesting scholars.

Rhodri Hayward QMUL Centre for the History of the Emotions

The history of emotions is a radical discipline.  It takes as its subject matter what appear at first instance, to be the most authentic and intractable aspects of our identities and reminds us that these things are transitory and contingent.  It teaches us that our feelings are not determined by deep psychology or biology but are instead historical constructions borne out of an accident of our language, relationships and material circumstances.  In doing this, the discipline opens up the possibility of new forms of politics and new ways of working on the self.  If our inner life is determined by the world around us then the first step to radical change or personal transformation is engagement with that wider environment.  And as recent work in the discipline makes clear, this wider environment that sustains emotions includes a vast range of elements from friendships and family structures to popular novels, pension schemes or animal experiments.

Although our emotional categories might be transient and rooted in particular cultures they are none the less powerful.  Anyone who has lived with small children or awkward flatmates will know that statements about inner feelings (‘I feel cross’, ‘I’m disappointed’) serve as bargaining chips as we negotiate competing agendas.  They also work, as the anthropologist, Vincent Crapanzano argued in his Hermes’ Dilemma and Hamlet’s Desire (1992)   ‘to call the context’: announcing one’s feelings changes our understanding of a situation.  Over recent years I’ve become interested in this traffic between the political shaping of the emotions and the emotional shaping of politics.  Alongside the writings of Roger Smith, Thomas Dixon and Vincent Crapanzano, I have found Kurt Danziger’s work on the history of psychological objects enormously useful here (e.g. Naming the Mind ).  Danziger’s account of the how psychological categories are first constructed and then serve to coordinate political arguments or economic decisions is a good tool for interrogating the enforced fun of the current government’s happiness agenda.

Jen Wallis PhD Student, QMUL Centre for the History of the Emotions

When I first mention to people that I work within the ‘Centre for the History of the Emotions’, I often get an amused smirk in response – as in ‘Gosh, they really will teach anything at universities these days’. Once you talk it over, though, they generally realise it’s not all intangible trendy theory. In the history of psychiatry, for example, emotion is at the heart (and head) of the matter: the nineteenth-century asylum (my own area of study) was a site where emotions were to be calmed – or sometimes awakened, depending on the nature of the illness – and where ideas were formulated about ‘appropriate’ emotions. Investigating the point at which an emotion crossed over from the normal to the pathological is crucial, then, in understanding nineteenth-century responses to a whole range of mental illnesses.

Tiffany Watt-Smith British Academy Post-Doctoral Fellow, Department of Drama and Centre for the History of the Emotions, QMUL

In 2009, Judge Daniel Rozak of Illinois was handing down a sentence on a felony drug charge when the defendant’s cousin, watching from the gallery, let out an enormous yawn. Thirty-three-year old Clifton Williams was promptly served with a 6-month jail sentence for contempt of court. ‘It was not a simple yawn’, the attorney’s office later explained to the incredulous Williams family. ‘It was an attempt to undermine the court’s authority’.

'Self-portrait, yawning' by Joseph Ducreux

Yawns, flinches, shudders, sighs, smiles, tears, frowns and pouts are never ‘simple’. Like language, these fleeting emotional expressions are slippery, their meanings shifting across different contexts, times and cultures. Of course we’ll never know what Williams really felt in the moment of his yawn. Perhaps tiredness brought on the offending gesture. Perhaps his yawn spewed forth from a maelstrom of anger and frustration, manifesting as boredom, scorn and derision. But from a historian’s point of view, the feelings behind Williams’s yawn matter less than the cultural assumptions its performance highlights. Read by the Judge as an attempt to ridicule and challenge the operation of his power, Williams yawn is very revealing indeed.

Tracing the role of emotional expressions – even ones so marginal and inconsequential-seeming as yawns – in social interaction can help us understand a great deal about cultures of the past. These minor emotional skirmishes show a great deal about how power operates in any given society, how gender, race and class roles are performed, how alliances are formed, hierarchies challenged, manners regulated, expectations swerved and confidences betrayed. Emotions bind us to our communities, our relationships forged and broken on the strength of our feelings, but in reality these negotiations may be subtle ones, taking place over a quizzical look, an almost imperceptible shrug or a giggle stifled. The fact that emotional expressions are governed by rules of display determining what is appropriate or inappropriate reminds us that emotions exist in more than one dimension. We habitually think of our feelings as private, subjective experiences, yet as the history of emotions affirms, emotions are also part of a matrix of cultural identities and social interactions too.

Historians of emotion do their work in this social dimension, shining a light on what for previous generations may have been considered too trivial and too fleeting for study. One of the things historians of emotion do well is to make the familiar strange again, reminding us of the contingency of our own assumptions about feelings through comparison with the past. Over eight-hundred years ago in twelfth-century France, yawning was not an affectation of the angry and disenfranchised, but rather the mark of an ardent young lover. While in English romances unrequited lovers sighed, in Occitan they yawned for the object of their affections, as a verse written by the troubadour Arnaut Daniel in 1170 testifies:

Day-long I stretch, all times, like a bird preening, And yawn for her

Liz Gray PhD Student, QMUL Centre for the History of the Emotions

Emotions are personal ‘things’. They are pervasive and they are how we all connect – with each other, and with our past. They are also one of the ways, often the first way we connect with history in general. But my emotions are mine, and your emotions are yours. And the emotions I felt five years ago are related to, but not the same as, the emotions I feel today. The emotions you felt five years ago are related to, but not the same as, the emotions you feel today. So what is there to gain from studying the history of emotions?

By understanding the subjectivity of our emotions, we are able to approach history more objectively. Through studying the history of emotions we can pick apart the basis for the emotions in each historical setting. Instead of empathising with the emotional feelings we read in our sources, we can take a step back and get closer to understanding what those emotions meant to the person, people, communities we are studying. And at the same time, it allows us to take a step back and assess our own feelings and reactions towards the history we are studying.

Jane Mackelworth PhD Student, Centre for the History of the Emotions and Centre for Studies of Home, QMUL

My interest in the history of the emotions stems from my training in both psychology and history. I graduated in psychology with the intention of studying for a doctorate in clinical psychology. However, after working in both clinical and health psychology departments I realized that this was not the right course of action for me. There was (at that time) a heavy focus on cognitive behavioural therapy which was based upon a very particular model of human behaviour. The emphasis was on a ‘scientist practitioner’ approach which stressed the ability to stand back from your clients, observe an objective detachment and apply a scientific framework to access the mind (and the problems) of the person sitting opposite you. Values such as empathy and others which derive from a more humanistic tradition were unpopular and seen as unhelpful. It felt to me as though there was little interest in attempting to uncover a person’s emotional experience. Rather the attention was on changing their thoughts, within a limited time frame, no matter how traumatic their previous experiences may have been. Emotions had become, it felt to me, rather a dirty residue.

In such interactions, objects which might seem trifling were invested with intense emotional significance. They acted as the litmus test of relations between people. This was why so much of the substance of witchcraft testimony apparently concerns the giving of objects of minute value. With the witch giving went wrong.

I think that history is at its most powerful in its study of things that we now take for granted, as though they are real, or constant. Where the emotions are taught elsewhere it is almost always from a realist point of view, as though this is fact. And so today, for example, our emotions, particularly those thought of as ‘disordered’ are seen as stemming from our biological make up, our genes. Yet even a brief historical glimpse at the experience of emotions challenges this. Emotions are always given meaning in a particular time and context. So, for example, we learn a lot about a society by seeking to understand why the emotions accompanying hysteria were generally ascribed to women.

Another piece of work I read recently and enjoyed was an article in Social and Cultural History by Claire Langhamer entitled ‘Love, Selfhood and Authenticity in Post-War Britain’ .  In this article, Langhamer explores the ways in which our understanding of love changed over the twentieth century. She argues that it has become seen as a route to self-actualization, thus it has become more about a route to personal fulfillment or liberation. She suggests that Love came to be seen increasingly as being about ‘chemistry’ and ‘emotional intimacy’ rather than ‘respect’ and ‘affection’ (p. 280). This also ties in with a greater focus in the twentieth century on the right to be happy and fulfilled.

Realising that emotions are culturally, and historically experienced is a curious realization. It is immensely liberating, because we realize that the meaning we give to emotions (and therefore the way that we experience them) is so intrinsically socially constructed. However, this moment of liberation is so fleeting because it is followed so quickly by the overwhelming sense that we can only ever be products of our age, so powerful is cultural and social conditioning. Thus, the emotions that we feel now are as real (and as different) as the emotions that were felt in the past.

Carolyn Burdett Department of English and Humanities, Birkbeck, University of London

For me, thinking about emotions and/in history began with empathy. In November 2004, a US literary scholar, Suzanne Keen, sent a request to the VICTORIA listserv explaining that she was writing a book about empathy and the novel, and asking for respondents’ experiences of empathising while reading Victorian fiction. Her request produced a long debate, full of thoughtful and intelligent insight about reading novels. As I followed it, though, I began to feel distracted by the way in which the terms sympathy and empathy were being used. Sometimes they seemed to be interchangeable, while sometimes respondents stressed differences between them; at times, too, they were applied to either Victorian authors or to Victorian readers or to both. What struck me most, was that I couldn’t recall ever coming across the term empathy in the literature of the period. Did the Victorians empathise, I wondered; and the OED confirmed that they indeed did not. Empathy was coined in 1909 by the British-born, US-based experimental psychologist, Edward Bradford Titchener, to translate a German word, Einfühlung , which was in turn first used as a noun in an 1873 work, On the Optical Sense of Form , by the German aesthetician Robert Vischer.

This was the starting point for my research into what happens to a distinctively Victorian notion of sympathy in the final decades of the nineteenth century, and the coining of empathy in the first decade of the twentieth. In the meantime, interest in, and research about, empathy has boomed. In 2005, a quick search of the British Library’s Integrated Catalogue for ‘empathy’ produced 179 hits. Today, the BL’s new (and admittedly more inclusive) ‘Main Catalogue’ shows over 5,000 results. The tab showing ‘Creation date’ is interesting:

  • Before 1972  (17)
  • 1972 To 1985  (20)
  • 1985 To 1993  (128)
  • 1993 To 2001  (888)
  • After 2001  (4,216)

In its first uses, though, empathy was primarily associated with aesthetic experience. In the hands of its initial exponent in Britain, the writer Vernon Lee, empathy hovered uncertainly between being a distinctively corporeal reaction to looking at beautiful forms which affected the viscera and thus feelings of health and well-being, and a more opaque mental experience which might ultimately explain the human impulse to metaphor and symbolisation. Lee, like many late Victorians, moved between enthusiastic hope that human experience might, eventually, be explicable in terms of verifiable, testable and indisputable physiological facts and a dogged conviction that the diversity, inventiveness and strangeness of life, human feelings and histories called for other ways of seeing things. When we contemplate empathy today, this Victorian debate remains a salutary case, reminding us that how our brains fire up is likely just one small part of the story.

Chris Millard PhD Student, QMUL Centre for the History of the Emotions

As human beings inhabiting the twenty-first century, emotions are part of the very fabric of our existence. While contemporary experts seek to root our emotional responses in some deep evolutionary past or specific neurochemical reaction (or both), the history of emotions can show how these claims have a history of their own. Through the history of emotions, the varied ways in which human beings have understood themselves across time and space be analysed. We can seek out different understandings of emotional states that have occurred in the past without attempting to collapse them into more or less approximate versions of today’s emotions du jour . Our understanding of emotional states constitutes a substantial proportion of how we understand ourselves, and unthinkingly to hand over interpretive authority in this area to neurochemists or evolutionary psychologists seems misguided at best. An appropriately historical understanding of emotions leads to a stress on the contingency and malleability of the intellectual systems and shorthand we use in order to make sense of internal mental, psychological and emotional states.

Mark Honigsbaum Former PhD Student of QMUL Centre for the History of the Emotions; University of Zurich

Long before I became a journalist and even longer before I became an historian, my first love was moral and political philosophy and, though I didn’t realise at the time, it was via Hume, Rousseau, Marx, and Mill that I first became interested in the place of emotions in history. The emotions, or the ‘passions’ as Hume called them, were central to these thinkers’ visions of a more just and moral society, hence Marx’s conviction that capitalism was founded on the ‘alienation’ or misery of it’s workers and Mill’s concern to maximise the happiness of the greatest number.

Today, such preoccupations are more present in our culture than ever. From David Cameron’s ‘well-being’ index to the ‘fury’ at bankers’ bonuses and the ‘anger’ felt in Greece and elsewhere at neo-liberal austerity programmes, we are used to the idea that emotions are important objects of epidemiological regulation. But it wasn’t until I read William Reddy’s  The Navigation of Feeling that I realised that this had not always been the case.

Put that way, Reddy’s thesis sounds simplistic. Didn’t we always knows that the French Revolution fed off the passions of the mob? Yes, but until Reddy we didn’t  realise how far these passions were products of particular historical regimes and political and cultural practices. Nor did we really appreciate how the mere act of emoting could change what was actually felt. As Reddy puts it, ‘emotives are influenced directly by and alter what they “refer” to… emotives do things to the world.’

There has been much debate since about how far historians should be prepared to run with the emotives. Reddy himself presented his theory as an argument ‘against constructionism’ and at times seems to come perilously close to endorsing an essentialist view of the emotions. However, I would argue that Reddy’s real aim is to outline a third way between essentialist and Foucauldian constructivist positions by showing how power is always located in emotional control – in other words, in who gets to express and who must repress certain feelings.

At a  time of great financial and political uncertainty, when we are daily being urged by Cameron and others to ‘calm down’ lest we become infected by Greek emotionalism, these insights are more pertinent than ever. I also like to think that Mill would have had sympathy with Reddy’s approach. ‘Ask yourself whether you are happy,’ he once famously asked himself, ‘and you cease to be so.’ Now that’s what I call an emotive.

Rebecca Kingston Political Science, University of Toronto

Author of Public Passion: Rethinking the Grounds for Political Justice

In his recent inaugural lecture as Chichele Chair of Social and Political Theory at Oxford University, Jeremy Waldron called on political theorists to abandon almost exclusive focus on questions of virtue (or character) and ideas of justice and liberty, issues which dominated the field throughout most of the twentieth century, and instead refocus study on questions related to the practice of political institutions (separation of powers, judicial review, etc). The underlying justification for advocating this turn rested on the practical issue that we are living in societies where people disagree and the most important political questions facing us today involve how we come to reasonable accommodation through our shared authoritative institutions (rather some ideal form of dispute resolution).  While laudable in its objective to move the field away from excessively abstract theorising, this position also demonstrates some blindness to what is central to the practice of politics and the working of institutions, and that is emotions that shape and are shaped by the political process.

In this context, the history of emotions matter because in a general way these studies help to demonstrate that ultimately individuals and groups give meaning to the institutions and practices that shape their lives. More specifically, though, in terms of my field of political theory, these studies can demonstrate that institutions are also limited in what they can do and that there is paradoxically both a certain hubris and excessive humility in thinking that political challenges can be resolved by institutional arrangements.

There is hubris because in the face of varied emotional responses and cultural contexts similar institutions function in vastly different ways; and there is excessive humility because greater focus on human emotion in history provides greater insight into ways in which we can move beyond traditional limits of political goods in security and liberty, and seek new goals of emotional wellbeing that can be effected by a much broader array of political and social strategies.

If there is any writer who has most influenced me in my reflections and for whom I have the greatest awe and respect it must be Jonathan Lear. My reading of his book Aristotle: The Desire to Understand marks a turning point in my intellectual development as it brought to me an understanding of the importance of emotion even at the core of intellectual endeavour. His more recent work Radical Hope I consider to be a masterpiece as it weaves together a deep understanding of emotional life as irreducibly tied to questions of moral philosophy and the good life and driven by a deep sense of humanism.

Jules Evans Policy Director, QMUL Centre for the History of the Emotions

Author of Philosophy for Life, and Other Dangerous Situations

I’m probably the least qualified in the Centre to answer the question, so let me instead say what interests me about the history of the emotions. I am particularly interested in cognitive psychology, and the intersection between our biology and our beliefs and values. Our minds, bodies and emotions are deeply affected by the meaning we ascribe to events – and that meaning is constantly changing through culture and history. We can uncover or excavate the cultural constructions that shape our emotions. The point of doing that, for me, is to help liberate us from those constructions that serve us least well and cause us most suffering.

In terms of my favourite work of the history of the emotions, I think it would be Adam Curtis’ BBC documentary, The Century of the Self – the first bit of history of emotions I ever encountered, which left a lasting impression on me and many other people who saw it.

Thomas Dixon Director, QMUL Centre for the History of the Emotions

For me, the point of studying the history of emotions is to liberate ourselves from contemporary psychology. By anatomising the feelings of the past, pulling apart the beliefs, bodily states, physical places and material cultures of which they were composed, we can undermine the idea that there are universal or ‘basic’ hardwired emotions that are felt by all people in all places. To use history imaginatively, to inhabit the worldviews and mental pictures of the people whose lives and ideas we study, is simultaneously to understand the past and to destabilise the present. We learn that what we feel now, how we feel it, and what we think our feelings reveal and express, is historically contingent and far from universal. Because our beliefs about our own mental states are themselves inescapable constituents of our mental lives – including, for instance, the belief in the power of involuntary invisible entities called ‘emotions’ – to study the history of ideas about emotions is itself to study the history of emotional experiences.

Febvre’s 1941 essay was an invitation to his fellow historians to get to work on this fascinating new field of history; to examine representations of emotions and sensibility in conduct books, court records, paintings, sculpture, music, and novels. ‘I am asking for a vast collective investigation to be opened,’ he wrote, ‘on the fundamental sentiments of man and the forms they take. What surprises we may look forward to!’

5 thoughts on “ What is the history of emotions? Part II ”

Pingback: What is the History of Emotions? » Pouvoir des arts

Pingback: 10 links for May — News from Somewhere

Pingback: What is the history of emotions? Part III | The History of Emotions Blog

Pingback: What is the history of emotions? | ACCESS

Pingback: The uses of a history of emotions « Serendipities

Comments are closed.

The Daily Show Fan Page

the history of human emotions essay

Explore the latest interviews, correspondent coverage, best-of moments and more from The Daily Show.

Extended Interviews

the history of human emotions essay

The Daily Show Tickets

Attend a Live Taping

Find out how you can see The Daily Show live and in-person as a member of the studio audience.

Best of Jon Stewart

the history of human emotions essay

The Weekly Show with Jon Stewart

New Episodes Thursdays

Jon Stewart and special guests tackle complex issues.

Powerful Politicos

the history of human emotions essay

The Daily Show Shop

Great Things Are in Store

Become the proud owner of exclusive gear, including clothing, drinkware and must-have accessories.

About The Daily Show

A Historical and Theological Description of Jesus Christ

This essay is about the historical and theological description of Jesus Christ. It discusses his life as a Jewish preacher in first-century Judea, his teachings on love and forgiveness, and his role in founding Christianity. The essay highlights the absence of a physical description in biblical texts, the belief in Jesus as both divine and human, and the significance of his crucifixion and resurrection. It also touches on his influence on ethical principles and his broader spiritual significance across different religions. The essay underscores Jesus’ lasting impact on history, culture, and religion.

How it works

Jesus Christ is truly a big deal in human history, respected by billions worldwide. His life and teachings are the bedrock of Christianity, shaping everything from culture to philosophy and religion. To talk about Jesus means digging into both the history of his life and the beliefs about him held by believers.

Historically, Jesus was a Jewish preacher from Nazareth, living in Judea around 2,000 years ago. The stories we have about him mostly come from the New Testament in the Bible, which tells us he was born in Bethlehem and grew up in Nazareth.

His parents, Mary and Joseph, were devout Jews. Jesus started preaching about the Kingdom of God when he was about 30, doing miracles and gathering a big group of followers.

We don’t have a clear picture of what Jesus looked like from the Bible, so artists and early Christian writings imagined him with a beard and long hair, fitting in with how people looked back then. They often showed him as caring and serious, matching his teachings and personality.

In terms of belief, Christians think Jesus is God’s Son and the promised Messiah (Christ) from the Old Testament. The big idea in Christianity is the Incarnation, which says Jesus is fully God and fully human. This mix is a big mystery that’s at the heart of what Christians believe, showing how Jesus connects God and people in a special way.

One of the most important times in Jesus’ life was his crucifixion, a harsh Roman punishment. Christians believe he died to make up for everyone’s sins. Then, three days later, he came back to life—this is a huge deal in Christianity, showing that sin and death don’t win, and people who believe get to live forever.

Jesus’ teachings, written down in the gospels, are all about love, forgiveness, and being humble. The Sermon on the Mount, found in Matthew’s gospel, is super famous for saying things like, “Blessed are the poor in spirit,” and talking about how to live in a good way. He also told stories called parables, like the Good Samaritan and the Prodigal Son, which teach deep lessons about life and how to treat others.

Jesus didn’t just teach—he did amazing things, too. According to the gospels, he healed sick people, made blind folks see again, and even brought dead people back to life. These miracles showed his power and his love for people, making folks pay attention to what he had to say.

Beyond the miracles and teaching, Jesus cared a lot about everyone, especially those who were left out by society, like tax collectors, prostitutes, and sick people. His message was pretty radical for his time, shaking up how people thought about rules and religion. It’s why he got in trouble with the authorities and ended up arrested and killed on a cross.

Jesus’ impact goes way past his own time. Christianity, based on his life and teachings, is the world’s biggest religion today. His ideas about what’s right and wrong have influenced things like laws, human rights, and how we fight for fairness. He’s also a big figure in Islam, where he’s seen as a prophet, and in other religions, showing just how important he is to many people.

In the end, describing Jesus Christ means looking at both the history and what people believe about him. He’s a figure who’s made a huge impact, inspiring and challenging folks all over the world. Whether you see him as a historical figure or a religious leader, Jesus keeps showing us what love, sacrifice, and making things right are all about.

Remember, this is just a start for your own thinking and learning. If you need more help, think about talking to experts at EduBirdie—they’re great at making sure your work meets all the rules for school and helping you understand things better.

owl

Cite this page

A Historical and Theological Description of Jesus Christ. (2024, Jun 28). Retrieved from https://papersowl.com/examples/a-historical-and-theological-description-of-jesus-christ/

"A Historical and Theological Description of Jesus Christ." PapersOwl.com , 28 Jun 2024, https://papersowl.com/examples/a-historical-and-theological-description-of-jesus-christ/

PapersOwl.com. (2024). A Historical and Theological Description of Jesus Christ . [Online]. Available at: https://papersowl.com/examples/a-historical-and-theological-description-of-jesus-christ/ [Accessed: 30 Jun. 2024]

"A Historical and Theological Description of Jesus Christ." PapersOwl.com, Jun 28, 2024. Accessed June 30, 2024. https://papersowl.com/examples/a-historical-and-theological-description-of-jesus-christ/

"A Historical and Theological Description of Jesus Christ," PapersOwl.com , 28-Jun-2024. [Online]. Available: https://papersowl.com/examples/a-historical-and-theological-description-of-jesus-christ/. [Accessed: 30-Jun-2024]

PapersOwl.com. (2024). A Historical and Theological Description of Jesus Christ . [Online]. Available at: https://papersowl.com/examples/a-historical-and-theological-description-of-jesus-christ/ [Accessed: 30-Jun-2024]

Don't let plagiarism ruin your grade

Hire a writer to get a unique paper crafted to your needs.

owl

Our writers will help you fix any mistakes and get an A+!

Please check your inbox.

You can order an original essay written according to your instructions.

Trusted by over 1 million students worldwide

1. Tell Us Your Requirements

2. Pick your perfect writer

3. Get Your Paper and Pay

Hi! I'm Amy, your personal assistant!

Don't know where to start? Give me your paper requirements and I connect you to an academic expert.

short deadlines

100% Plagiarism-Free

Certified writers

Help | Advanced Search

Computer Science > Computation and Language

Title: esc-eval: evaluating emotion support conversations in large language models.

Abstract: Emotion Support Conversation (ESC) is a crucial application, which aims to reduce human stress, offer emotional guidance, and ultimately enhance human mental and physical well-being. With the advancement of Large Language Models (LLMs), many researchers have employed LLMs as the ESC models. However, the evaluation of these LLM-based ESCs remains uncertain. Inspired by the awesome development of role-playing agents, we propose an ESC Evaluation framework (ESC-Eval), which uses a role-playing agent to interact with ESC models, followed by a manual evaluation of the interactive dialogues. In detail, we first re-organize 2,801 role-playing cards from seven existing datasets to define the roles of the role-playing agent. Second, we train a specific role-playing model called ESC-Role which behaves more like a confused person than GPT-4. Third, through ESC-Role and organized role cards, we systematically conduct experiments using 14 LLMs as the ESC models, including general AI-assistant LLMs (ChatGPT) and ESC-oriented LLMs (ExTES-Llama). We conduct comprehensive human annotations on interactive multi-turn dialogues of different ESC models. The results show that ESC-oriented LLMs exhibit superior ESC abilities compared to general AI-assistant LLMs, but there is still a gap behind human performance. Moreover, to automate the scoring process for future ESC models, we developed ESC-RANK, which trained on the annotated data, achieving a scoring performance surpassing 35 points of GPT-4. Our data and code are available at this https URL .
Comments: Pre-print
Subjects: Computation and Language (cs.CL)
Cite as: [cs.CL]
  (or [cs.CL] for this version)
  Focus to learn more arXiv-issued DOI via DataCite

Submission history

Access paper:.

  • HTML (experimental)
  • Other Formats

license icon

References & Citations

  • Google Scholar
  • Semantic Scholar

BibTeX formatted citation

BibSonomy logo

Bibliographic and Citation Tools

Code, data and media associated with this article, recommenders and search tools.

  • Institution

arXivLabs: experimental projects with community collaborators

arXivLabs is a framework that allows collaborators to develop and share new arXiv features directly on our website.

Both individuals and organizations that work with arXivLabs have embraced and accepted our values of openness, community, excellence, and user data privacy. arXiv is committed to these values and only works with partners that adhere to them.

Have an idea for a project that will add value for arXiv's community? Learn more about arXivLabs .

COMMENTS

  1. Introduction to the Special Section on the History of Emotions

    Just as George Eliot's red cloth and epaulets intimate rank and embody reverence and romantic interest, historian Katie Barclay suggests in her essay that emotion should be viewed not only as an inner, psychological phenomenon, but also as inhering in, or sticking to, objects and places in our communities. Barclay (2021) challenges us to imagine a history of emotion in line with posthumanist ...

  2. An Introduction to History of Science and the Emotions

    Historians of emotions have thus far provided meager tools for incorporating animal emotions into history: as part of our human histories and as part of their histories. ... The essays study developments in the history of technology and the human sciences (medicine, physiology, and psychiatry) on two different continents (North America and ...

  3. History of emotions

    The history of emotions is a field of historical research concerned with human emotion, especially variations among cultures and historical periods in the experience and expression of emotions. Beginning in the 20th century with writers such as Lucien Febvre and Peter Gay, an expanding range of methodological approaches is being applied.

  4. State of the Field: The History of Emotions

    Emotions have appeared in histories, and associated writings, for centuries. History writing has sometimes valued the emotional; in eighteenth-century Europe, the centrality of the idea of sympathetic exchange to communication ensured that many historians of the period sought to produce feelings in their readers.1 The emotions of historical subjects too have long been of interest.

  5. EVOLUTION OF HUMAN EMOTION

    The decrease of the limbic system reduced the dependence of humans on base emotions, and the increase in the neocortex allowed humans greater control over remaining emotional circuits as well as greater cognitive capacities. The limbic system concept stimulated much research in the 1950s, 60s, and 70s.

  6. The History of Emotions: A Very Short Introduction

    Emotions are visceral reactions and beliefs regarding the world that could be intimate and private. The history of emotions is plural rather than singular since emotions are complex mental states that resist reduction. Emotions can be felt as spontaneous outbursts and are also culturally learned performances. This VSI explores how emotions gain ...

  7. NEW HISTORIES OF EMOTION

    REVIEW ESSAYS NEW HISTORIES OF EMOTION The History of Emotions: An Introduction. By Jan Plamper. Translated by Keith Tribe. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press, 2015. ... transform even the most basic features of human experience. This review essay engages with the central arguments of these books, and offers a somewhat different ...

  8. The History of Emotions: An Introduction

    The History of Emotions: An Introduction. Jan Plamper. Published 22 March 2015. Psychology, History, Political Science, Philosophy, Art. - The first book-length introduction to one of the fastest-growing fields of historical discipline - Offers a synthesis of the work already carried out in the field and an agenda for the direction of future ...

  9. From passions to emojis

    This history of the term 'emotion' is one reason why many historians would agree with the sentiment expressed by Penelope Gouk and Helen Hills in their introduction to an important 2005 collection of essays when they state that, 'Our starting-point is that "the emotions", unchanging within human nature, transcending historical ...

  10. Introduction to the special section on the history of emotions

    In this series of stimulating reflective essays, prominent scholars of emotion and its history address the challenges and rewards of interdisciplinarity, recent work in the field, and the many conceptions of "emotion"—a polyvocality that presents limitations as well as opportunities. ... 2140 History & Systems. Population Group. Human ...

  11. Introductory Essay: Emotion, Affect, and the Eighteenth Century

    involving high-stakes human emotions," 17 implying that literature is a labora-tory of feeling. According to scholars, depictions of emotion in the literature of ... umvirate of emotions, history, and the eighteenth century articulates "effortful . 276 THE EIGHTEENTH CENTURY ... These essays best demonstrate the dynamic emotional life flourish-

  12. A Human History of Emotion: How the Way We Feel Built the World We Know

    Author Richard Firth-Godbehere. Publisher Little, Brown Spark. Publication Date 2021-11-02. Section New Hardcover - Nonfiction / Psychology. Type New. Format Hardcover. ISBN 9780316461313. A sweeping exploration of the ways in which emotions shaped the course of human history, and how our experience and understanding of emotions have evolved ...

  13. (PDF) The History of Emotions

    2018. "The history of emotions" is the first accessible book on the theories, methods, achievements, and problems in this burgeoning field of historical inquiry. Historians of emotion borrow heavily from the disciplines of anthropology, psychology, philosophy and neuroscience, and stake out a claim that emotions have a past and change over time.

  14. (PDF) What is the History of Emotions?

    The discipline is based on the assumption that not only the way the emotions are perceived, namely feelings, but also the feelings themselves are learned. Culture and history are changing and so are feelings as well as their expression. The social relevance and potency of emotions is historically and culturally variable.

  15. History of Emotions

    History of Emotions. After sixteen successful years of research, the Center for the History of Emotions will conclude its work on 30 June 2024. Our thanks go to the many scholars who shared their ideas and knowledge, were guest researchers, gave lectures or explored new avenues of the history of emotions with us as collaboration partners.

  16. PDF Defining Emotion: A Brief History

    The effort to define the term "emotion" has a long history in the discipline of psychology. Izard's survey (2010) canvassed prominent emotion theo-rists and researchers on their working definitions of emotion. The particu-lar assumptions about emotion reported, as well as the conclusion that the term "emotion" lacks a consensus ...

  17. PDF University of Birmingham The History of the Emotions

    one that takes the 'conditions of [human] existence' as a crucial ... books under review in this essay talk about methodology very little, if at all. Ute Frevert's Emotions in History - Lost and Found offers a concise and very readable introduction to the history of modern European emotions, touching on questions of method in the ...

  18. A History of Emotions, 1200-1800

    The history of emotions is an expanding field of research. The essays in this collection examine emotional responses to art and music, the role of emotions in contemporary notions of gender and sexuality and theoretical questions as to their use. ... the authors in this volume provide fascinating glimpses into human emotional experience across ...

  19. Tiffany Watt Smith: The history of human emotions

    The words we use to describe our emotions affect how we feel, says historian Tiffany Watt Smith, and they've often changed (sometimes very dramatically) in response to new cultural expectations and ideas. Take nostalgia, for instance: first defined in 1688 as an illness and considered deadly, today it's seen as a much less serious affliction. In this fascinating talk about the history of ...

  20. The puzzle of human emotions: some historical considerations from the

    The review first considers the etymology of the term 'emotion' and relates it to the 17th century understanding of the passions. It further looks at the secu-larization of the passions in the 18th century theory of the nerves, and 19th century perspectives on the theory of emotions as described by authors such as Charles Bell, Alexander ...

  21. AHR Conversation: The Historical Study of Emotions

    Abstract. In the past few years, the AHR has published five "Conversations," each on a subject of interest to a wide range of historians: "On Transnational History" (2006), "Religious Identities and Violence" (2007), "Environmental Historians and Environmental Crisis" (2008), "Historians and the Study of Material Culture" (2009), and "Historical Perspectives on the ...

  22. Why We Feel: The Science of Human Emotions

    Victor Johnston makes us abandon this view with his biopsychological theory of emotions. He points out that the fact that we have feelings and the way we feel are relentlessly determined by the evolutionary selection process. Evolution has refined our system of appraisal of our perceptions, ultimately according to reproduction advantages.

  23. What is the history of emotions? Part II

    The emotions, or the 'passions' as Hume called them, were central to these thinkers' visions of a more just and moral society, hence Marx's conviction that capitalism was founded on the 'alienation' or misery of it's workers and Mill's concern to maximise the happiness of the greatest number.

  24. Wonder: a Deep Dive into its Essence

    Essay Example: Wonder is a profound and multifaceted emotion that transcends cultural boundaries and resonates deeply within human experience. It is more than mere curiosity or amazement; rather, wonder encapsulates the awe-inspiring sensation of encountering something extraordinary, beyond

  25. The Tragedy of the Holocaust: a Dark Chapter in Human History

    The essay covers the origins, implementation, and aftermath of the Holocaust, highlighting the extreme cruelty of concentration camps and the enduring impact on global consciousness and human rights. Memorials and survivor testimonies emphasize the importance of education and remembrance to prevent future atrocities.

  26. The Impact and Origins of World War i

    Essay Example: World war I, often called large war, stands so as critical connection in human history, deeply mimiced landscape 20 - ?? beginning geopolitical century with patient consequence/pls. His origins sediment in co-operation authorities ambitions, nationalism heat, tangled wedding rings

  27. The Multifaceted Collapse of the Holocaust

    Essay Example: The Holocaust stands as one of the most harrowing episodes in human history, characterized by the systematic annihilation of six million Jews, along with millions of others, under Nazi rule from 1941 to 1945. The end of this atrocity was not precipitated by a singular event but.

  28. The Daily Show Fan Page

    The source for The Daily Show fans, with episodes hosted by Jon Stewart, Ronny Chieng, Jordan Klepper, Dulcé Sloan and more, plus interviews, highlights and The Weekly Show podcast.

  29. A Historical and Theological Description of Jesus Christ

    Essay Example: Jesus Christ is truly a big deal in human history, respected by billions worldwide. His life and teachings are the bedrock of Christianity, shaping everything from culture to philosophy and religion. To talk about Jesus means digging into both the history of his life and the beliefs.

  30. [2406.14952] ESC-Eval: Evaluating Emotion Support Conversations in

    Emotion Support Conversation (ESC) is a crucial application, which aims to reduce human stress, offer emotional guidance, and ultimately enhance human mental and physical well-being. With the advancement of Large Language Models (LLMs), many researchers have employed LLMs as the ESC models. However, the evaluation of these LLM-based ESCs remains uncertain. Inspired by the awesome development ...