Cookies on GOV.UK

We use some essential cookies to make this website work.

We’d like to set additional cookies to understand how you use GOV.UK, remember your settings and improve government services.

We also use cookies set by other sites to help us deliver content from their services.

You have accepted additional cookies. You can change your cookie settings at any time.

You have rejected additional cookies. You can change your cookie settings at any time.

a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

  • Education, training and skills
  • Pupil wellbeing, behaviour and attendance

A review of the literature on anxiety for educational assessments

This review pulls together statistics and academic literature relating to the causes, symptoms and effects of assessment-related anxiety.

Applies to England

A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments.

PDF , 770 KB , 63 pages

This file may not be suitable for users of assistive technology.

This review is part of a wider collection of material focused on managing test anxiety. It pulls together statistics and academic literature relating to the causes, symptoms and effects of assessment-related anxiety, known as test anxiety in the literature. It is an academic piece of writing that considers what is currently known about test anxiety, by drawing on existing evidence and research, and may be of particular interest to specialists and practitioners working in the field.

Related content

Is this page useful.

  • Yes this page is useful
  • No this page is not useful

Help us improve GOV.UK

Don’t include personal or financial information like your National Insurance number or credit card details.

To help us improve GOV.UK, we’d like to know more about your visit today. We’ll send you a link to a feedback form. It will take only 2 minutes to fill in. Don’t worry we won’t send you spam or share your email address with anyone.

Supporting Student Success: The Role of Test Anxiety, Emotional Intelligence, and Multifaceted Intervention

  • First Online: 11 November 2022

Cite this chapter

a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

  • Christopher L. Thomas   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-6804-5318 4 ,
  • Kristie Allen   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-0061-6107 5 ,
  • Clara Madison Morales   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-5971-5631 4 &
  • Jaren Mercer   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-5545-3504 4  

1553 Accesses

1 Citations

Test anxiety is a complex cognitive-motivational phenomenon that impairs the academic performance of learners at all educational levels. Effectively supporting test-anxious students requires understanding the environmental and internal factors that elicit and perpetuate test anxiety. As such, one of the aims of the chapter is to provide an overview of the role of situational appraisal, self-knowledge, and controlled processing in maladaptive reactions to evaluative events as outlined in the Self-Regulatory Executive Function Processing Model. Further, we highlight the role of enduring personality dispositions and emotion-focused self-perceptions (i.e., dimensions of trait emotional intelligence) in test anxiety. Finally, we seek to provide educators with practical ideas about how to reduce test anxiety based on decades of theoretical and applied research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Abdollahi, A., & Abu Talib, M. (2015). Emotional intelligence moderates perfectionism and test anxiety among Iranian students. School Psychology International, 36 (5), 498–512. https://doi.org/10.1177/0143034315603445

Article   Google Scholar  

Aldridge, A. A., & Roesch, S. C. (2008). Developing coping typologies of minority adolescents: A latent profile analysis. Journal of Adolescence, 31 (4), 499–517.

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Archambault, I., & Dupéré, V. (2017). Joint trajectories of behavioral, affective, and cognitive engagement in elementary school. The Journal of Educational Research, 110 (2), 188–198. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2015.1060931

Austin, E. J., Saklofske, D., & Mastoras, S. M. (2010). Emotional intelligence, coping, and exam-related stress in Canadian undergraduate students. Australian Journal of Psychology, 52 , 42–50.

Axelson, R. D., & Flick, A. (2010). Defining student engagement. Change: The Magazine of Higher Learning, 43 (1), 38–43.

Bardi, A., & Guerra, V. M. (2011). Cultural values predict coping using culture as an individual difference variable in multicultural samples. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42 (6), 908–927.

Bandura, A. (2005). The evolution of social cognitive theory. In K. G. Smith & M. A. Hitt (Eds.), Great minds in management (pp. 9–35). Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar  

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84 (2), 191–215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191

Bar-On, R. (1997). The emotional intelligence inventory (EQ–i): Technical manual . Multi-Health Systems.

Ben-Eliyahu, A., Moore, D., Dorph, R., & Schunn, C. D. (2018). Investigating the multidimensionality of engagement: Affective, behavioral, and cognitive engagement across science activities and contexts. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 53 , 87–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.01.002

Benjamin, M., McKeachie, W. J., Lin, Y., & Holinger, D. P. (1981). Test anxiety: Deficits in information processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73 , 816–824.

Boyle, C. C., Stanton, A. L., Ganz, P. A., Crespi, C. M., & Bower, J. E. (2017). Improvements in emotion regulation following mindfulness meditation: Effects on depressive symptoms and perceived stress in younger breast cancer survivors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 85 (4), 397–402.

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Blankstein, K. R., Flett, G. L., Boase, P., & Toner, B. B. (1990). Thought listing and endorsement measures of selfreferential thinking in test anxiety. Anxiety Research, 2 (2), 103–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(89)90001-9

Bracken, B. A., & Brown, E. F. (2006). Behavioral identification and assessment of gifted and talented students. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 24 (2), 112–122. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282905285246

Brand, S., Ahmadpanah, M., Keshavarz, M., Haghighi, M., Jahangard, L., Bajoghli, H., Sadeghi Bahmani, D., & Holsboer-Trachsler, E. (2016). Higher emotional intelligence is related to lower test anxiety among students. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 1 , 133–136. https://doi.org/10.2147/ndt.s98259

Cao, T. H., Jung, J. Y., & Lee, J. (2017). Assessment in gifted education: A review of the literature from 2005 to 2016. Journal of Advanced Academics, 28(3) , 163–203. https://doi.org/10.1177/1932202X17714572

Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1988). A control-process perspective on anxiety. Anxiety Research, 1 (1), 17–22.

Cassady, J. C. (2004). The influence of cognitive test anxiety across the learning–testing cycle. Learning and Instruction, 14 (6), 569–592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.09.002

Cassady, J. C., & Johnson, R. E. (2002). Cognitive test anxiety and academic performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27 (2), 270–295. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.2001.1094

Cassady, J. C., & Thomas, C. L. (2020). Identifying and supporting students with affective disorders in schools: Academic anxieties and emotional information processing. In A. J. Martin, R. A. Sperling, & K. J. Newton (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology and students with special needs (pp. 52–74). Routledge.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Cassady, J. C., & Boseck, J. J. (2008). Educational psychology and emotional intelligence: Toward a functional model for emotional information processing in schools. In J. C. Cassady & M. A. Eissa (Eds.), Emotional intelligence: Perspectives of educational and positive psychology (pp. 3–24).

Cizek, G., & Burg, S. (2006). Addressing test anxiety in a high-stakes environment . Corwin Press.

Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social-information processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115 , 74–101.

Daly, A. L., Chamberlain, S., & Spalding, V. (2011). Test anxiety, heart rate and performance in A-level French speaking mock exams: An exploratory study. Educational Research, 53 (3), 321–330. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131881.2011.598660

Deming, D. J., & Figlio, D. (2016). Accountability in US education: Applying lessons from K-12 experience to higher education. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 30 (3), 33–56. https://doi.org/10.1257/jep.30.3.33

Depreeuw, E. A. M. (1984). A profile of the test anxious student. International Review of Applied Psychology, 33 , 221–232.

Dyson, R., & Renk, K. (2006). Freshman adaption to university life: Depressive symptoms, stress, and coping. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62 , 1231–1244.

Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2020). From expectancy-value theory to situated expectancy-value theory: A developmental, social cognitive, and sociocultural perspective on motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61 , 101859. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101859

Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7 (2), 336–353. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336

Eysenck, M. W., & Derakshan, N. (2011). New perspectives in attentional control theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 50 (7), 955–960. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.019

Federici, R. A., Skaalvik, E. M., & Tangen, T. N. (2015). Students’ perceptions of the goal structure in mathematics classrooms: Relations with goal orientations, mathematics anxiety, and help-seeking behavior. International Education Studies, 8 (3), 146–158.

Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1985). If it changes it must be a process: Study of emotion and coping during three stages of a college examination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48 (1), 150–170. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.48.1.150

Frederickson, N., Petrides, K. V., & Simmonds, E. (2012). Trait emotional intelligence as a predictor of socioemotional outcomes in early adolescence. Personality and Individual Differences, 52 , 323–328. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.034

Friedman, I. A., & Bendas-Jacob, O. (1997). Measuring perceived test anxiety in adolescents: A self-report scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 57 (6), 1035–1046. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164497057006012

Galassi, J. P., Frierson, H. T., & Sharer, R. (1981). Behavior of high, moderate, and low test anxious students during an actual test situation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49 (1), 51–62. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.49.1.51

Giacobbi, P. R., Lynn, T. K., Wetherington, J. M., Jenkins, J., Bodendorf, M., & Langley, B. (2004). Stress and coping during the transition to university for first-year female athletes. The Sport Psychologist, 18 (1), 1–20.

Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15 , 1–38.

Hembree, R. (1988). Correlates, causes, effects, and treatment of test anxiety. Review of Educational Research, 58 (1), 47–77. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543058001047

Hultberg, P., Calonge, D. S., & Lee, A. E. S. (2018). Promoting long-lasting learning through instructional design. Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, 18 (3), 26–43. https://doi.org/10.14434/josotl.v18i3.23179

Johns, M., Inzlicht, M., & Schmader, T. (2008). Stereotype threat and executive resource depletion: Examining the influence of emotion regulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137 , 691–705.

Kern, C. L., & Crippen, K. J. (2017). The effect of scaffolding strategies for inscriptions and argumentation in a science cyberlearning environment. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 26 (1), 33–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-016-9649-x

Lane, S., Raymond, M. R., & Haladyna, T. M. (2016). Handbook of test development (2nd ed.). Routledge.

Lang, P., Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey & D. Sluyter (Eds.), E motional development and emotional intelligence: Implications for educators (pp. 3–31). Basic Books.

Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping . Springer.

Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1987). Transactional theory and research on emotions and coping. European Journal of Personality, 1 , 141–169.

Leckie, G., & Goldstein, H. (2019). The importance of adjusting for pupil background in school value‐added models: A study of Progress 8 and school accountability in England. British Educational Research Journal, 45(3) , 518–537. https://doi.org/10.1002/berj.3511

Liebert, R. M., & Morris, L. W. (1967). Cognitive and emotional components of test anxiety: A distinction and some initial data. Psychological Reports, 20 , 975–978. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1967.20.3.975

Lovett, B. J., & Nelson, J. M. (2017). Test anxiety and the Americans with disabilities act. Journal of Disability Policy Studies, 28 (2), 99–108. https://doi.org/10.1177/1044207317710699

Lowe, P. A. (2019). Examination of test anxiety in samples of Australian and US higher education students. Higher Education Studies, 9 (4), 33–43. https://doi.org/10.5539/hes.v9n4p33

Lowe, P. A. (2021). The test anxiety measure for college students-short form: Development and examination of its psychometric properties. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 39 (2), 139–152. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282920962947

Lowe, P. A., Lee, S. W., Witteborg, K. M., Prichard, K. W., Luhr, M. E., Cullinan, C. M., et al. (2008). The test anxiety inventory for children and adolescents (TAICA) examination of the psychometric properties of a new multidimensional measure of test anxiety among elementary and secondary school students. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 26 (3), 215–230.

Maguire, R., Egan, A., Hyland, P., & Maguire, P. (2017). Engaging students emotionally: The role of emotional intelligence in predicting cognitive and affective engagement in higher education. Higher Education Research & Development, 36 (2), 343–357. https://doi.org/10.1080/07294360.2016.1185396

Martins, A., Ramalho, N., & Morin, E. (2010). A comprehensive meta-analysis of the relationship between emotional intelligence and health. Personality and Individual Differences, 49 (6), 554–564. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.029

Matthews, G., Hillyard, E. J., & Campbell, S. E. (1999). Metacognition and maladaptive coping as components of test anxiety. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6 , 111–125. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0879(199905)

Matthews, G., & Wells, A. (2004). Rumination, depression, and metacognition: The S-REF model. In C. Papageorgiou & A. Wells (Eds.), Rumination: Nature, theory, and treatment (pp. 125–151). Wiley.

Mavroveli, S., & Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J. (2011). Trait emotional intelligence influences on academic achievement and school behaviour. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81 , 112–134. https://doi.org/10.1348/2044-8279.002009

Mikolajczak, M., Roy, E., Luminet, O., Fillée, C., & De Timary, P. (2007). The moderating impact of emotional intelligence on free cortisol responses to stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32 (8–10), 1000–1012.

Mikolajczak, M., Bodarwé, K., Laloyaux, O., Hansenne, M., & Nelis, D. (2010). Association between frontal EEG asymmetries and emotional intelligence among adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 48 (2), 177–181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.10.001

Mikolajczak, M., Avalosse, H., Vancorenland, S., Verniest, R., Callens, M., Van Broeck, N., et al. (2015). A nationally representative study of emotional competence and health. Emotion, 15(5), 653. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000034

Morris, L. W., & Liebert, R. M. (1970). Relationship of cognitive and emotional components of test anxiety to physiological arousal and academic performance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 35 (3), 332–337. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030132

Morris, L. W., Davis, M. A., & Hutchings, C. H. (1981). Cognitive and emotional components of anxiety: Literature review and a revised worry–emotionality scale. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73 (4), 541–555. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.73.4.541

Nairn, R. C., & Merluzzi, T. V. (2019). Enhancing coping skills for persons with cancer utilizing mastery enhancement: A pilot randomized clinical trial. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 42 (3), 423–439.

Naveh-Benjamin, M. (1991). A comparison of training programs intended for different types of test-anxious students: Further support for an information-processing model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83 (1), 134.

Naveh-Benjamin, M., McKeachie, W. J., & Lin, Y. (1987). Two types of test-anxious students: Support for an information processing model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79 , 131–136.

Newton, P. E. (2007). Clarifying the purposes of educational assessment. Assessment in Education, 14 (2), 149–170. https://doi.org/10.1080/09695940701478321

Nonis, S. A., & Hudson, G. I. (2010). Performance of college students: Impact of study time and study habits. Journal of Education for Business, 85 (4), 229–238.

O’Carroll, P. J., & Fisher, P. (2013). Metacognitions, worry and attentional control in predicting OSCE performance test anxiety. Medical Education, 47 (6), 562–568. https://doi.org/10.1111/medu.12125

OECD. (2008). Measuring improvements in learning outcomes: Best practices to assess the value-added of schools (Paris, Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development Publishing & Centre for Educational Research and Innovation). OECD.

Book   Google Scholar  

Okpala, A. O., Okpala, C. O., & Ellis, R. (2000). Academic efforts and study habits among students in a principles of macroeconomics course. Journal of Education for Business, 75 (4), 219–224.

Owens, M., Stevenson, J., Norgate, R., & Hadwin, J. A. (2008). Processing efficiency theory in children: Working memory as a mediator between trait anxiety and academic performance. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 21 (4), 417–430. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615800701847823

Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., & Perry, R. P. (2002). Academic emotions in students’ self-regulated learning and achievement: A program of qualitative and quantitative research. Educational Psychologist, 37 (2), 91–105. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326985EP3702_4

Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Perry, R. P., Kramer, K., Hochstadt, M., & Molfenter, S. (2004). Beyond test anxiety: Development and validation of the test emotions questionnaire (TEQ). Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 17 (3), 287–316. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615800412331303847

Peltier, C., & Vannest, K. J. (2017). A meta-analysis of schema instruction on the problem-solving performance of elementary school students. Review of Educational Research, 87 (5), 899–920. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654317720163

Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2001). Trait emotional intelligence: Psychometric investigation with reference to established trait taxonomies. European Journal of Personality, 15 (6), 425–448. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.416

Petrides, K. V., Pérez, J. C., & Furnham, A. (2003, July). The Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue): A measure of emotional self–efficacy . Paper presented at the 11th Biennial Meeting of the International Society for the Study of the Individual Differences (ISSID). Graz, Austria.

Petrides, K. V., Frederickson, N., & Furnham, A. (2004). The role of trait emotional intelligence in academic performance and deviant behavior at school. Personality and Individual Differences, 36 , 277–293. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00084-9

Petrides, K. V., Pérez-González, J. C., & Furnham, A. (2007a). On the criterion and incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence. Cognition and Emotion, 21 (1), 26–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930601038912

Petrides, K. V., Pita, R., & Kokkinaki, F. (2007b). The location of trait emotional intelligence in personality factor space. British Journal of Psychology, 98 , 273–289. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712606X120618

Petrides, K. V., Hudry, K., Michalaria, G., Swami, V., & Sevdalis, N. (2011). A comparison of the trait emotional intelligence profiles of individuals with and without Asperger syndrome. Autism, 15 , 671–682. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361310397217

Petrides, K. V., Mikolajczak, M., Mavroveli, S., Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J., Furnham, A., & Pérez-González, J. C. (2016). Developments in trait emotional intelligence research. Emotion Review, 8 (4), 335–341. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073916650493

Petrides, K. V., Sanchez-Ruiz, M., Siegling, A. B., Saklofske, D. H., & Mavroveli, S. (2018). Emotional intelligence as personality: Measurement and role of trait emotional intelligence in educational contexts. In K. V. Keefer, J. D. A. Parker, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), Emotional intelligence in education: Integrating research with practice (pp. 49–81). Springer Nature.

Phye, G. D. (1990). Inductive problem solving: Schema inducement and memory-based transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82 (4), 826–831. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.82.4.826

Preiss, R. W., Gayle, B. M., & Allen, M. (2006). Test anxiety, academic self-efficacy, and study skills: A meta-analytic review. In Classroom communication and instructional processes: Advances through meta-analysis (pp. 99–111). Routledge.

Putwain, D. W. (2008). Deconstructing test anxiety. Educational and Behavioral Difficulties, 13 , 141–155. https://doi.org/10.1080/13632750802027713

Putwain, D. W., & Daly, A. L. (2014). Test anxiety prevalence and gender differences in a sample of English secondary school students. Educational Studies, 40 , 554–570. https://doi.org/10.1080/03055698.2014.953914

Putwain, D. W., & Symes, W. (2014). The perceived value of maths and academic self-efficacy in the appraisal of fear appeals used prior to a high-stakes test as threatening or challenging. Social Psychology of Education, 17 (2), 229–248. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-014-9249-7

Putwain, D. W., & Symes, W. (2016). The appraisal of value-promoting messages made prior to a high-stakes mathematics examination: The interaction of message-focus and student characteristics. Social Psychology of Education, 19 (2), 325–343. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-016-9337-y

Putwain, D. W., & von der Embse, N. P. (2018). Teachers use of fear appeals and timing reminders prior to high-stakes examinations: Pressure from above, below, and within. Social Psychology of Education, 21 , 1001–1019.

Putwain, D. W., Woods, K. A., & Symes, W. (2010). Personal and situational predictors of test anxiety of students in post-compulsory education. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80 (1), 137–160.

Putwain, D. W., Shah, J., & Lewis, R. (2014). Performance-evaluation threat does not always adversely affect verbal working memory in high test anxious persons. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 13 , 120–136. https://doi.org/10.1891/1945-8959.13.1.120

Putwain, D. W., Symes, W., & Wilkinson, H. M. (2017). Fear appeals, engagement, and examination performance: The role of challenge and threat appraisals. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 87 , 16–31.

Putwain, D. W., & Symes, W. (2020). The four Ws of test anxiety: What is it, why is it important, where does it come from, and what can be done about it? Psychologica, 63 , 31–52. https://doi.org/10.14195/1647-8606_63-2_2

Riley, H., & Schutte, N. S. (2003). Low emotional intelligence as a predictor of substance-use problems. Journal of Drug Education, 33 , 391–398. https://doi.org/10.2190/6DH9-YT0M-FT99-2X05

Romano, L., Tang, X., Hietajärvi, L., Salmela-Aro, K., & Fiorilli, C. (2020). Students’ trait emotional intelligence and perceived teacher emotional support in preventing burnout: The moderating role of academic anxiety. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17 (13), 4771. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17134771

Article   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Roos, A. L., Goetz, T., Voracek, M., Krannich, M., Bieg, M., Jarrell, A., & Pekrun, R. (2021). Test anxiety and physiological arousal: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 33 (2), 579–618. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-020-09543-z

Saklofske, D. H., Austin, E. J., Galloway, J., & Davidson, K. (2007). Individual difference correlates of health-related behaviors: Preliminary evidence for links between emotional intelligence and coping. Personality and Individual Differences, 42 , 491–502. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.006

Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D., & Yoo, S. H. (2009). The positive psychology of emotional intelligence. In S. J. Lopez & C. R. Snyder (Eds.), Oxford handbook of positive psychology (pp. 237–248). Oxford University Press.

Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J., Mavroveli, S., & Poullis, J. (2013). Trait emotional intelligence and its links to university performance: An examination. Personality and Individual Differences, 54 , 658–662. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.013

Sarason, I. G. (1960). Empirical findings and theoretical problems in the use of anxiety scales. Psychological Bulletin, 57 (5), 403–415.

Sarason, I. G. (1984). Stress, anxiety, and cognitive interference: Reactions to tests. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46 , 929–938.

Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115 , 336–356.

Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J., et al. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 25 , 167–177.

Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., & Hine, D. W. (2011). The association of ability and trait emotional intelligence with alcohol problems. Addiction Research and Theory, 19 , 265–265. https://doi.org/10.3109/16066359.2010.512108

Schwarzer, R. (1996). Thought control of action: Interfering self-doubts. In I. G. Sarason, G. R. Pierce, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Cognitive interference: Theories, methods, and findings (pp. 99–115). Guilford Publications.

Segool, N. K., Carlson, J. S., Goforth, A. N., Von Der Embse, N., & Barterian, J. A. (2013). Heightened test anxiety among young children: Elementary school students’ anxious responses to high-stakes testing. Psychology in the Schools, 50 (5), 489–499. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.21689

Segool, N. K., von der Embse, N., Mata, A. D., & Gallant, J. (2014). Cognitive behavioral model of test anxiety in a high-stakes context: An exploratory study. School Mental Health, 6 (1), 50–61. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12310-013-9111-7

Seipp, B. (1991). Anxiety and academic performance: A meta-analysis of findings. Anxiety Research, 4 (1), 27–41. https://doi.org/10.1080/08917779108248762

Sirois, F. M., & Hirsch, J. K. (2013). Associations of psychological thriving with coping efficacy, expectations for future growth, and depressive symptoms over time in people with arthritis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 75 (3), 279–286. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.06.004

Siu, A. F. (2009). Trait emotional intelligence and its relationships with problem behavior in Hong Kong adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 47 (6), 553–557.

Spielberger, C. D., Gonzalez, H. P., Taylor, D. J., Algaze, B., & Anton, W. D. (1978). Examination stress and test anxiety. In C. D. Spielberger & I. G. Sarason (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 5). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.12.022

Suinn, R. M. (1984). Generalized anxiety disorder. In S. M. Turner (Ed.), Behavioral theories and treatment of anxiety (pp. 279–320). Plenum Press.

Symes, W., & Putwain, D. W. (2020). The four Ws of test anxiety. Psychologica, 63 (2), 31–52.

Sweller, J. (2011). Cognitive load theory. In J. P. Mestre & B. H. Ross (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Cognition in education (pp. 37–76). Elsevier Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12387691-1.00002-8

Thomas, C. L., & Allen, K. (2021). Driving engagement: Investigating the influence of emotional intelligence and academic buoyancy on student engagement. Journal of Further and Higher Education, 45 , 107–119.

Thomas, C. L., & Cassady, J. C. (2019). The influence of personality factors, value appraisals, and control appraisals on cognitive test anxiety. Psychology in the Schools, 56 , 1568–1582.

Thomas, C. L., Cassady, J. C., & Heller, M. (2017). The influence of emotional intelligence, cognitive test anxiety, and coping strategies on undergraduate performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 55 , 40–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.001

Thomas, C. L., Cassady, J. C., & Heath, J. (2020). Examining the psychometric properties of the FRIEDBEN test anxiety scale using exploratory structural equation modeling. International Journal of School and Educational Psychology, 8 , 213–226. https://doi.org/10.1080/21683603.2018.1522281

Uphill, M. A., Rossato, C. J., Swain, J., & O’Driscoll, J. (2019). Challenge and threat: A critical review of the literature and an alternative conceptualization. Frontiers in Psychology, 10 , 1255. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01255

Veenman, M. V. J., Kerseboom, L., & Imthorn, C. (2000). Test anxiety and metacognitive skillfulness: Availability versus production deficiencies. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 13 , 391–412.

von der Embse, N. P., & Putwain, D. W. (2015). Examining the context of instruction to facilitate student success. School Psychology International, 36 (6), 552–558. https://doi.org/10.1177/0143034315612144

von der Embse, N. P., Kilgus, S. P., Segool, N., & Putwain, D. (2013). Identification and validation of a brief test anxiety screening tool. International Journal of School and Educational Psychology, 1 , 246–258.

von der Embse, N. P., Mata, A. D., Segool, N., & Scott, E. C. (2014). Latent profile analyses of test anxiety: A pilot study. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 32 (2), 165–172. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282913504541

von der Embse, N., Jester, D., Roy, D., & Post, J. (2018). Test anxiety effects, predictors, and correlates: A 30-year meta-analytic review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 227 , 483–493. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.048

von der Embse, N. P., Putwain, D. W., & Francis, G. (2021). Interpretation and use of the multidimensional test anxiety scale (MTAS). School Psychology, 36 (2), 86–96. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000427

Wells, A., & Matthews, G. (1996). Modelling cognition in emotional disorder: The S-REF model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34 (11–12), 881–888.

Wells, A. (2000). Emotional disorders and metacognition: Innovative cognitive therapy . John Wiley & Sons Ltd..

Wells, A. (2019). Breaking the cybernetic code: Understanding and treating the human metacognitive control system to enhance mental health. Frontiers in Psychology, 10 , 2621.

West, A., Mattei, P., & Roberts, J. (2011). Accountability and sanctions in English schools. British Journal of Educational Studies, 59 (1), 41–62. https://doi.org/10.1080/00071005.2010.529416

Wiggins, G. P. (1993). The Jossey-Bass education series. Assessing student performance: Exploring the purpose and limits of testing . Jossey-Bass.

Wimmer, L., von Stockhausen, L., & Bellingrath, S. (2019). Improving emotion regulation and mood in teacher trainees: Effectiveness of two mindfulness trainings. Brain and behavior, e01390 . https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.1390

Zeidner, M. (1998). Test anxiety: The state of the art . Plenum Press.

Zeidner, M., & Matthews, G. (2005). Evaluation anxiety: Current theory and research. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 141–163). Guilford Publications.

Download references

Conflicts of Interest

We have no known conflicts of interest.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

School of Education, University of Texas at Tyler, Tyler, TX, USA

Christopher L. Thomas, Clara Madison Morales & Jaren Mercer

Department of Psychology and Counseling, University of Texas at Tyler, Tyler, TX, USA

Kristie Allen

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christopher L. Thomas .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

Center for Integrated Mental Health Care (CIMHC) Federal University of Sao Paulo, São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil

Luiz Ricardo Vieira Gonzaga

Pontifical Catholic University of Campinas, Campinas, São Paulo, Brazil

Letícia Lovato Dellazzana-Zanon

University of Sorocaba – UNISO, São Paulo, Brazil

Andressa Melina Becker da Silva

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2022 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Thomas, C.L., Allen, K., Morales, C.M., Mercer, J. (2022). Supporting Student Success: The Role of Test Anxiety, Emotional Intelligence, and Multifaceted Intervention. In: Gonzaga, L.R.V., Dellazzana-Zanon, L.L., Becker da Silva, A.M. (eds) Handbook of Stress and Academic Anxiety. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-12737-3_3

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-12737-3_3

Published : 11 November 2022

Publisher Name : Springer, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-031-12736-6

Online ISBN : 978-3-031-12737-3

eBook Packages : Behavioral Science and Psychology Behavioral Science and Psychology (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

  • Seeing double at Eurovision
  • Access Offers Change of Direction for Dawn
  • Calls for better partnerships between youth and education sectors to enrich pupils’ lives
  • NWSLC College Apprentice Shortlisted for East Midlands Business Awards

Shopping Cart

No products in the basket.

a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

A review of the literature on anxiety for educational assessments

a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

This review pulls together statistics and academic literature relating to the causes, symptoms and effects of assessment-related anxiety.

A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

PDF , 770KB , 63 pages

This file may not be suitable for users of assistive technology. Request an accessible format.

If you use assistive technology (such as a screen reader) and need aversion of this document in a more accessible format, please email [email protected] .Please tell us what format you need. It will help us if you say what assistive technology you use.

This review is part of a wider collection of material focused on managing test anxiety. It pulls together statistics and academic literature relating to the causes, symptoms and effects of assessment-related anxiety, known as test anxiety in the literature. It is an academic piece of writing that considers what is currently known about test anxiety, by drawing on existing evidence and research, and may be of particular interest to specialists and practitioners working in the field.

Published 14 February 2020

Related Articles

a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

SPEED NETWORKING SHOWCASES STEM CAREERS AT THE MTI

Seventy school pupils from across the region have interviewed more than 30 ambassadors from automotive and engineering businesses at a speed networking event at the…

a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

Bath Spa University awards Honorary Degrees to seven outstanding individuals

The current Turner Prize winner and esteemed Sculptor Veronica Ryan OBE was among seven Honorary Doctorates awarded by Bath Spa University at Summer Graduation ceremonies…

a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

GOODWOOD CEO VISITS HSDC HAVANT STUDENTS

Goodwood Chief Executive Officer Chris Woodgate recently visited our Havant Campus to discuss his career journey to date with a large number of enthusiastic students.…

a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

Oxford Business College’s Platinum Support for SMEs

Oxford Business College has been praised after becoming a Platinum sponsor, one of the first educational organisations in the UK to step forward and support…

a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

Study Towards a Degree for Less than the Price of a Daily Meal Deal – learndirect Launches Affordable University Pathway 

The UK’s leading online learning provider has launched a range of degree pathway courses for students looking for an alternative to traditional university amid the…

a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

Students awarded prizes for achievements and making a difference

From setting up their own businesses and volunteering in the community, to excelling in their studies, supporting fellow students and making an impact with new…

a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

Leicester College Trainer Assessor wins ‘Advanced Apprentice of the Year’ award

Katy Herrod, Trainer Assessor in Leicester College’s Taste Restaurant has won the ‘Advanced Apprentice of the Year’ award at the Leicester Apprenticeship Hub 2023 awards. …

Albert Education partnership offers green boost to film students

Albert Education partnership offers green boost to film students

The University of Winchester has signed a new partnership which will boost its film students’ job prospects and make their courses more environmentally sustainable. From…

students studying in library

UA92 and The Moulding Foundation agree £250,000 partnership

The Moulding Foundation has agreed to support a new bursary scheme launched by Manchester education establishment, UA92 (University Academy 92), to support students from disadvantaged…

a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

Apprentice electricians reflect on a successful first year while others look to the future in latest industry podcast from SECTT and SELECT

Apprentices from both ends of the electrical training journey have once again revealed their behind-the-scenes secrets in the second episode of the new industry podcast…

You must be logged in to post a comment.

There was a problem reporting this post.

Block Member?

Please confirm you want to block this member.

You will no longer be able to:

  • See blocked member's posts
  • Mention this member in posts

Please allow a few minutes for this process to complete.

Privacy Overview

Digital Education Resource Archive (DERA)

  • Browse by Year
  • Browse by Organisations

A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

Howard, Emma , Office of Qualifications and Examinations Regulation (OFQUAL), corp creator. (2020) A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments. [ Research and Analysis ]

EPrints Logo

A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments - RESEARCH AND ANALYSIS - Gov.uk

  • Download HTML
  • Download PDF

A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments - RESEARCH AND ANALYSIS - Gov.uk

  • Uncategorized

MULTI-SECTORAL NUTRITION STRATEGY 2014-2025

  • Health & Fitness

Needs Assessment of the Korean Community in Kingston - Principle Researcher: Dr Heidi Seetzen, CResCID, Kingston University Researcher: Dr Carlie ...

  • Hobbies & Interests

New Dimensions School of Hair Design - 621 Kentucky Avenue Joplin, MO 64801 Phone: (417)782-2875 Website: newdimensionsschoolofhairdesign.com ...

  • Style & Fashion

2021 2025 Planning Report - Institutional Academic, Research, and Student Service Plans - Nevada System of Higher ...

  • Arts & Entertainment

Student Info - Narrabundah College

  • Food & Drink

Starting secondary school in Lambeth 2020/21

  • Cars & Machinery

ACADEMIC CATALOG 2020-2021

  • Home & Garden

HOLY CROSS STUDENT 2020 - S - Holy Cross Catholic ...

  • Government & Politics

Exeter Education Incubator Café Online 2021 - Project Catalogue Monday 5th July - Friday 16th July 2021

  • World Around

The Aquila Digital Community - The University of Southern Mississippi - The Aquila ...

Mental Wellbeing

Education for Mental Health Toolkit - Anxiety and Learning

Anxiety and learning.

The qualitative literature (1) highlights concern among some academics about what constitutes anxiety, what is a tolerable amount of anxiety or stress, what students should be able to ‘push through’ and what should be treated with concern.

Much of this discussion reflects the general difficulty in understanding mental and emotional states and confusion in language. The word ‘stress,’ for example, can be used in a variety of ways, to mean anything from mild nerves or anticipatory excitement through to high levels of anxiety and fear. This spectrum can potentially be broken up into discrete experiences in theory but, in real life situations, it is difficult to identify absolute dividing lines between what may be helpful, tolerable, intolerable and harmful.

Education for Mental Health

Download a digital copy of the full toolkit, the staff development toolkit and case studies.

In this resource we use the word ‘stretch’ for the experience of being challenged in ways that can be positive for learning, wellbeing and achievement. This experience is sometimes referred to as Eustress in the literature and has been shown to be helpful in motivating someone to engage in helpful behaviours (such as studying and academic performance) (2-4). As the students in our co-creation project highlighted, challenge can be good for wellbeing. Being challenged pushes us to grow and develop. Meeting and overcoming challenges by mastering new skills and knowledge has powerful, positive payoffs for wellbeing.

It should also be borne in mind that boredom can have a negative effect on wellbeing (5-6). A lack of challenge in our lives can lead to low motivation and a lack of meaning and purpose. The students in our co-creation group reported that when they found modules boring, they became disengaged, lost motivation and began to doubt their future, which in turn reduced their mood.

On the other side of this, high levels of stress and anxiety can reduce cognitive functioning at a neurological level. (7) (Our use of the words ‘stress’ and ‘anxiety’ in this resource reflects this experience). This reduces students’ ability to engage in complex thinking, to access old memories or make new, complex memories, to problem solve and to maintain concentration. In other words, anxiety reduces the capacity for learning and academic performance at a biological level.

Anxiety is a fear response to a perceived threat. Students become anxious about education when they view it or the environment as a threat to them (8). This fear may be stimulated by social and cultural experiences, which, for instance, leave students feeling marginalised, ostracised or humiliated. Alternatively, it may arise from a fear of failure or the perceived consequences of failure. These consequences can be emotional and practical. Students can be scared of the emotional pain they will feel if they fail. They can also fear the practical outcomes of failure, which may not be realistic e.g., they may fear that failing an assignment might lead to them having to drop out of university.

The failures some students fear can be more than the technical failure of an assignment. Some students may view a mark of 75% as a failure to achieve against their pre-conceived expectations, as articulated by our co-creation project group. Some may feel that getting an answer wrong in class is an example of failure. All of these types of failure will present risks to them – humiliation, ostracism from their peer group, etc.

The key to understanding the relationship between boredom, anxiety, stretch and learning is finding productive balance. Vygotsky’s (9) concept of scaffolded learning provides a pedagogic framework in which the aim of the curriculum should be to stretch students to their zone of proximal development – just outside their current comfort zone. This is likely to engage students in stretch, avoiding both boredom and anxiety. It may help to explain this explicitly to students and to help them recognise their strengths and successes within this framework.

When students are appropriately stretched, risk is contained and feels within the student’s control. To achieve this, when faced with a learning or assessment task, students will:

  • be appropriately prepared and will understand what they have to do and how to do it.
  • recognise their own skills and resources.
  • have the necessary skills to undertake the task or will be able to develop them as a result of completing the task.
  • have the necessary and appropriate support.
  • have the resources they need – including time. 
  • be intrinsically motivated and focussed on the aspects of the task that are meaningful to them.
  • be in an environment that feels psychologically safe.

Key lessons

  • Being stretched can be good for wellbeing and learning. 
  • Being pushed into anxiety can reduce cognitive functioning, impacting on students’ ability to learn and perform.
  • Balance is key – keep students in their proximal zone of development.
  • To do this, students need to be prepared for tasks, have the necessary resources and skills, know and understand what they need to do and have the appropriate support and environment in which to learn.
  • A useful guiding maxim is that if students need to know, understand or be able to do something, it must be taught to them first. If students have previous experience of a task and know how to tackle it, they will be less anxious.
  • Normalising mistakes in the classroom (online or face to face) can create a learning environment that lowers anxiety and increases learning.
  • Use classroom activities to identify students’ current level of knowledge and understanding, so teaching and learning activities can be calibrated to the group’s zone of proximal development.

Buff line

References 

Jones e, priestley m, brewster l, wilbraham sj, hughes g, spanner l. student wellbeing and assessment in higher education: the balancing act. assessment & evaluation in higher education. 2021 apr 3;46(3):438-50. available from: doi:  10.1080/02602938.2020.1782344  , gibbons c. stress, eustress and the national student survey. psychology teaching review. 2015;21(2):86-92., o’sullivan g. the relationship between hope, eustress, self-efficacy, and life satisfaction among undergraduates. social indicators research. 2011 mar 1;101(1):155-72., bourgeois, t. j. (2018). effect of eustress, flow, and test anxiety on physical therapy psychomotor practical examinations (doctoral dissertation, walden university). 2018., pekrun r, goetz t, daniels lm, stupnisky rh, perry rp. boredom in achievement settings: exploring control–value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. journal of educational psychology. 2010 aug;102(3):531., pekrun r, goetz t, titz w, perry rp. academic emotions in students’ self-regulated learning and achievement: a program of qualitative and quantitative research. educational psychologist. 2002;37:91–105, marin mf, lord c, andrews j, juster rp, sindi s, arsenault-lapierre g, fiocco aj, lupien sj. chronic stress, cognitive functioning and mental health. neurobiology of learning and memory. 2011 nov 1;96(4):583-95., howard e, a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments. online: ofqual 2020. available from:  https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/865832/a_review_of_the_literature_concerning_anxiety_for_educational_assessment.pdf  , vygotsky ls, cole m. mind in society: development of higher psychological processes. harvard university press; 1978..

©Advance HE 2020. Company limited by guarantee registered in England and Wales no. 04931031 | Company limited by guarantee registered in Ireland no. 703150 | Registered charity, England and Wales 1101607 | Registered charity, Scotland SC043946 | VAT Registered number GB 152 1219 50. Registered UK Address: Advance HE, Innovation Way, York Science Park, Heslington, York, YO10 5BR, United Kingdom | Registered Ireland Address: Advance HE, First Floor, Penrose 1, Penrose Dock, Cork, T23 Kw81, Ireland.

Systematic review of student anxiety and performance during objective structured clinical examinations

Affiliations.

  • 1 Texas Health Harris Methodist Fort Worth Hospital, Fort Worth, Texas, United States; Department of Medical Education, Texas Christian University, Fort Worth, Texas, United States. Electronic address: [email protected].
  • 2 University of North Texas System College of Pharmacy, Fort Worth, Texas, United States.
  • PMID: 33092780
  • DOI: 10.1016/j.cptl.2020.07.007

Introduction: Test anxiety is well studied in higher education, but studies primarily concern traditional assessments, such as written examinations. As use of objective structured clinical examination (OSCE) in pharmacy education increases, a closer examination of non-cognitive factors such as test anxiety is warranted. The purpose of this review was to determine the association between OSCE-associated test anxiety with OSCE performance in health professional students.

Methods: A literature search was conducted to identify peer-reviewed literature concerning test anxiety in health professional students associated with OSCE. Investigators searched for a combination of OSCE-related terms with anxiety-related terms using PubMed. Articles were included if they assessed OSCE-related anxiety by quantitative or qualitative methods. Data extracted from eligible articles included demographic data, type of the anxiety survey, associations between OSCE-related anxiety and performance, and other student-factors associated with OSCE-related anxiety.

Results: The literature search yielded 339 articles. Nine articles met eligibility criteria and were included in the review. Results included students from medical, pharmacy, dental, and nursing professional programs. Anxiety was assessed via multiple scales. Six out of the eight studies assessing the relationship OSCE-related anxiety and OSCE performance found no association between the two measures. Contrary to literature concerning test anxiety in higher education, female gender was not associated with OSCE-related anxiety.

Conclusion: OSCE-related anxiety appears to have minimal to no influence on student performance. Future studies should utilize standardized anxiety assessments and should seek to understand anxiety's effects on student wellbeing and burnout.

Keywords: Anxiety; Assessment; Clinical competence; OSCE; Objective structured clinical examination; Performance anxiety; Test anxiety.

Copyright © 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Publication types

  • Systematic Review
  • Clinical Competence*
  • Educational Measurement*
  • Physical Examination

🇺🇦    make metadata, not war

A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

  • Emma Howard
  • Document from Web
  • NonPeerReviewed

Similar works

thumbnail-image

Digital Education Resource Archive

This paper was published in Digital Education Resource Archive .

Having an issue?

Is data on this page outdated, violates copyrights or anything else? Report the problem now and we will take corresponding actions after reviewing your request.

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

The effects of work on cognitive functions: a systematic review.

Pasquale Bufano

  • 1 Institute of Clinical Physiology, National Research Council, Pisa, Italy
  • 2 Department of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, Epidemiology and Hygiene, Italian Workers' Compensation Authority (INAIL), Rome, Italy
  • 3 Occupational Medicine Unit, Foundation IRCCS Ca' Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
  • 4 Department of Clinical Science and Community Health, University of Milan, Milan, Italy
  • 5 Department of Public Health and Pediatrics, University of Turin, Turin, Italy

Introduction: Cognitive functions play a crucial role in individual’s life since they represent the mental abilities necessary to perform any activity. During working life, having healthy cognitive functioning is essential for the proper performance of work, but it is especially crucial for preserving cognitive abilities and thus ensuring healthy cognitive aging after retirement. The aim of this paper was to systematically review the scientific literature related to the effects of work on cognitive functions to assess which work-related factors most adversely affect them.

Method: We queried the PubMed and Scopus electronic databases, in February 2023, according to the PRISMA guidelines (PROSPERO ID number = CRD42023439172), and articles were included if they met all the inclusion criteria and survived a quality assessment. From an initial pool of 61,781 papers, we retained a final sample of 64 articles, which were divided into 5 categories based on work-related factors: shift work ( n  = 39), sedentary work ( n  = 7), occupational stress ( n  = 12), prolonged working hours ( n  = 3), and expertise ( n  = 3).

Results: The results showed that shift work, occupational stress, and, probably, prolonged working hours have detrimental effects on cognitive functioning; instead, results related to sedentary work and expertise on cognitive functions are inconclusive and extremely miscellaneous.

Discussion: Therefore, workplace health and well-being promotion should consider reducing or rescheduling night shift, the creation of less demanding and more resourceful work environments and the use of micro-breaks to preserve workers’ cognitive functioning both before and after retirement.

Systematic review registration: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42023439172 , identifier CRD42023439172.

1 Introduction

Cognitive functions play a crucial role in the daily life of every human being because they represent the basic mental abilities for performing any activity. They include attention, memory and learning, perceptual motor function, executive functions, and language; each of these allows human beings to perceive the world around them and to make decisions, but also to perform from the simplest task, such as remembering a telephone number or brushing teeth, to more complex ones such as reading a book or using the internet. Among the various activities that we are able to perform with the support of cognitive functions is clearly included the execution of any kind of work. During working life, having healthy cognitive functioning is essential for the proper performance of work, but it is especially crucial for preserving cognitive abilities and thus ensuring healthy cognitive aging after retirement ( Cohen et al., 2019 ). In everyday life, there are a lot of different factors that can contribute to the preservation of adequate cognitive abilities, such as physical activity and social engagement ( Ihle et al., 2015 ), mental activities (e.g., taking courses, learning new languages) or cognitively stimulating leisure activities (e.g., go to the theatre) ( Hultsch et al., 1993 ; Schooler and Mulatu, 2001 ), and it is well known that even a high cognitive level of one’s job leads to cognitive enrichment ( Stern et al., 1995 ; Schooler et al., 1999 ), in the same way as all other variables. On the contrary, however, there are also many factors that negatively impact cognitive functions such as, for example, prolonged exposure to stress leading to the loss of neurons, particularly in the hippocampus, which is altered by the increase in glucocorticoids and results in cognitive impairment ( McEwen and Sapolsky, 1995 ), sleep problems such as insomnia can induce cognitive impairment ( Brownlow et al., 2020 ) but even acute states of sleep deprivation can trigger deterioration in one or more cognitive functions ( Wang et al., 2021 ; Bufano et al., 2023 ), and sedentary behaviour is associated with lower cognitive performance ( Falck et al., 2017 ). All these factors, both positive and negative, may be intrinsic in different types of work and may contribute to both adequate preservation of cognitive functions and cognitive impairment. Therefore, the study of how work can have an impact on cognitive function, both in a positive way and, above all, in a preventive health perspective, in a negative way, has become a priority in the field of prevention and healthy aging, as shown by the growing interest of the European community ( Salazar-Xirinachs, 2012 ; Vendramin et al., 2012 ; Belin et al., 2016 ) and latest research on the topic ( Vives et al., 2018 ; Rinsky-Halivni et al., 2022 ; Bonzini et al., 2023 ).

If we look at cognitive aging from the perspective of normative aging, there are three main theories explaining the effects of work on cognitive functions and take into account both the level of mental activity experienced during working life and the type of working environment encountered by the worker. The primary theory, which currently guides the line of research, is the “use-it-or-lose-it” hypothesis ( Hultsch et al., 1999 ; Salthouse, 2006 , 2016 ), according to which the individual level of cognitive functioning depends on two mechanisms: differential preservation and preserved differentiation. For the first one, individuals who are mentally active tends to preserve their cognitive functions more than those who are not mentally active; the second one postulates that individuals with higher levels of cognitive functioning during life retain these beneficial levels as they age. There are two other prominent theories: “the Brain or Cognitive reserve hypothesis” ( Mortimer and Graves, 1993 ; Stern, 2002 ; Fratiglioni and Wang, 2007 ) and “Schooler’s theory of environmental influences on cognitive functioning” ( Schooler, 1984 , 1990 ). The cognitive reserve hypothesis states that people who attend mentally stimulating environments will have improved neuronal development, resulting in increased cognitive reserve. This cognitive reserve could be critical in facing neuronal insults and cognitive decline, increasing individual resilience, and thus leading to healthier aging ( Mortimer and Graves, 1993 ; Stern, 2002 ; Fratiglioni and Wang, 2007 ). The Schooler’s theory holds that complex environments that promotes cognitive effort lead people to develop their intellect and apply new cognitive processes, thereby improving cognitive functioning. Conversely, if the environment do not stimulate high levels of cognitive functioning, intellectual abilities will not be maintained and there will be a decrease in cognitive functioning ( Schooler, 1984 , 1990 ). All these theoretical frameworks follow the same line: by stimulating cognitive functions in a positive way, either through mentally active jobs or enriched working environments, cognitive functions are preserved and protected, and people move toward healthier cognitive aging.

On the other hand, however, these theories remain valid and applicable as long as we talk about normative aging, since there are several factors intrinsic to various types of work that can have a negative impact on cognitive functions, causing short-term cognitive impairment and early cognitive decline after retirement. Referring to the negative factors mentioned above (i.e., sleep disturbances, stress, and sedentary behaviour), there are several studies in the literature that relate them to different types of work and show the negative effects they could have on cognitive functioning. Shift work (i.e., an employment practice designed to keep a service or production line operational at all times, even during the night) is certainly linked to sleep disturbances that can induce cognitive impairment, but also to cardiovascular diseases ( Kantermann et al., 2012 ) which can lead to the same outcome ( Picano et al., 2014 ). Highly demanding jobs that induce work-related stress (i.e., a response that people may have when exposed to job demands and pressures that do not match their knowledge and skills) may increase the risk of dementia ( Andel et al., 2012 ), and sedentary behaviour [i.e., any waking behaviour characterized by an energy expenditure ≤1.5 metabolic equivalents (METs), while in a sitting, reclining, or lying posture ( Marconcin et al., 2021 )], caused by office work has as a potential outcome cognitive impairment ( Park et al., 2020 ), although its effects are often mixed ( Magnon et al., 2018 ).

We identified some work-related factors that can adversely affect cognitive functions and lead to cognitive impairment and probable pathological aging after retirement, but they are probably not the only ones; a comprehensive understanding of what all these factors are would be very important both from a prevention perspective and from a care perspective, with the aim of protecting their cognitive health and trying to ensure healthy cognitive aging.

Therefore, to achieve this goal and obtain a comprehensive overview of work-related factors influencing cognitive functions, our idea is to gather all the scientific literature on the topic through a systematic review using both, specific keywords related to the above-mentioned factors that negatively adverse cognitive functions, and generic keywords to try to broaden the search and discover other factors that may also exist.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 study design and search strategy.

We conducted this systematic review following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines ( Moher et al., 2009 ; Page et al., 2021 ). PRISMA comprises a 27-item checklist to ensure and promote the quality of systematic reviews; this checklist is reported in Supplementary Tables S1 , S3 (for abstract checklist). The protocol employed in the current systematic review has been submitted for registration (ID number = CRD42023439172) to the international prospective register for systematic reviews database (PROSPERO. Available online https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42023439172 ; accessed on July 06, 2023). The retrieval process consisted of three phases.

2.2 Systematic search phases

2.2.1 preliminary research and definition of keywords.

During the first phase, started in February 2023, we carried out preliminary research in absence of specific keywords based on the review of Fisher et al. (2017) , to define search criteria and identify the specific keywords to be used. Before defining the final list of keywords, we started from a very large list that, when entered into search engines, produced a massive number of results. We therefore removed nonspecific keywords and, on the fourth attempt, based on our research question, defined specific keywords (see Table 1 ) related to two domains: “Work” and “Cognitive Functions,” for inclusion in search engines to conduct the systematic research.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 . List of selected keywords.

2.2.2 Systematic search and definition of PICOS

The second phase, conducted in February 2023, consisted of a systematic search among titles, abstracts, and keywords of scientific papers, using the electronic databases PubMed and Scopus, based on the selected keywords, properly combined through the Boolean operators “AND” and “OR”; no temporal restriction was set, so all articles published up to February 2023 in Italian and English were included in the systematic search. The search strategy, including all keywords used and the number of studies found from each database, was analytically reported in Table 2 .

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 . Search strategy.

The final selection of papers for inclusion was carried out according to the Population, Intervention, Comparison, Outcomes, and Study Design (PICOS) worksheet ( Moher et al., 2009 ; Page et al., 2021 ), summarised in Table 3 . We defined the following PICOS criteria: Population, we chose “Healthy Workers (or not diagnosed)” as the inclusion criteria and “Clinical based population” as exclusion criteria because we aimed to avoid including populations in which, an organic disease or mental disorder could affect cognitive functions by generating a bias in the assessment; Interventions, we chose as inclusion criteria “Assessment of cognitive function without chemical/ or neurotoxic, or physical hazard exposure” to avoid including jobs where the worker’s exposure to various substances could create a bias in the assessment of cognitive function; Comparisons, we chose as inclusion criteria “Control Group (Between or Within design)” in order to have a comparison between two populations working different jobs (e.g., shift work vs. non-shift work), or between the same population during different shifts (night shift work vs. day shift work), in order to see if there was a variation in cognitive functioning due to the factor the study was investigating; Outcomes, we chose as inclusion criteria “Change or non-change of cognitive function according to different conditions,” to have a real evaluation of cognitive functioning through the use of cognitive test, thus studies with a quantitative measure of cognitive functioning; Study Design, we chose “Review, Scoping Review, Narrative Review, Systematic Review, Meta-Analysis, Editorial, Book, Case Report, Conference Review, and Conference Paper” as exclusion criteria following the guidelines to carry out a Systematic Review.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 . Population, intervention, comparison, outcomes and study design (PICOS) worksheet.

2.2.3 Application of PICOS study design exclusion criteria

The final phase consisted of a first step in which, following PICOS criteria related to the Study Design section, we excluded all reviews, both narrative and systematic ones, meta-analyses, and conference papers, to substantially reduce the number of included studies.

2.2.4 Title and abstract selection

By reading the titles and abstract we excluded all studies that did not match with the research question.

2.2.5 Full-text selection according to PICOS criteria

Finally, we included in the systematic review only clinical or epidemiological studies that investigated the effects of work on cognitive functions in groups of workers. The included papers were read thoroughly to obtain the data of our interest.

2.2.6 Synthesis method

The included papers were clustered according to the work-related factors that may affect cognitive functions and then, for each study, we extracted all the information shown in Tables 4 – 8 as described next:

www.frontiersin.org

Table 4 . Shift work and cognitive function: retrieved studies and their main outcomes.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 5 . Sedentary work and cognitive function: retrieved studies and their main outcomes.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 6 . Occupational stress and cognitive function: retrieved studies and their main outcomes.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 7 . Prolonged working hours and cognitive function: retrieved studies and their main outcomes.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 8 . Expertise and cognitive function: retrieved studies and their main outcomes.

Study = name of authors, year; Country (Region) = where the study took place; Goal of study = Objective of the study; Sample = sample size; Cognitive domains investigated = which cognitive domain were investigated; Cognitive functions assessed for each domain = which cognitive functions were investigated for each domain; Cognitive tests used for each function = which cognitive test were used to evaluate each cognitive function; Synthesis of main results = narrative summary of results.

2.2.7 Study risk of bias assessment

We assessed the risk of bias for each study in Supplementary Table S2 by compiling the AXIS tool ( Downes et al., 2016 ).

3.1 Flow diagram

Figure 1 shows all the phases in the systematic review process. The research carried out on PubMed and Scopus yielded 13,455 and 48,066 studies, respectively. These 61,781 papers were merged in a non-redundant database and 49,255 papers remained. Then, by eliminating all studies not related to our research question and all studies that did not match PICOS criteria related to Study Design (exclusion for all types of reviews, meta-analysis, and conference papers), the number of studies was reduced to 703, of which 2 were not retrieved. Finally, by applying all the PICOS criteria, we obtained 64 studies to be included in the systematic review.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 . Flow diagram.

3.2 Study selection and characteristics

Two independent reviewers (P.B. and M.L.) checked the pool of 49,255 abstracts collected from PubMed and Scopus search engine outputs; any disagreement was discussed with M.B. as the arbiter. Titles and abstracts were screened, and 48,550 were removed through a semi-automatic script created via MATLAB (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, United States), according to PICOS criteria related to Study Design (14,590 papers) and irrelevance to the research question (34,258 papers). The remaining 703 papers were checked for eligibility according to the remaining PICOS criteria, all clinical based population studies (58), all studies regarding chemical/physical risk (272), all studies with no control group (123) and lastly, all studies without a quantitative evaluation of cognitive functions (184) were excluded, 2 papers were not retrieved even after explicit request to the corresponding authors. Finally, 64 articles were included, summarized in Tables 4 – 8 , which shows the main information of each, divided according to work-related factors that may affect cognitive functions.

3.3 Synthesized findings

In this subsection, for each study, we reported the main findings of pertinence for this systematic review grouped according to work-related factors that may affect cognitive functions.

3.3.1 Shift work and cognitive functions

Among 64 studies included in the systematic review, 39 were related to the effect of shift work on cognitive functions ( Table 4 ) ( Deary and Tait, 1987 ; Orton and Gruzelier, 1989 ; Lingenfelser et al., 1994 ; Smith et al., 1995 ; Petru et al., 2005 ; Rouch et al., 2005 ; Saricaoğlu et al., 2005 ; Griffiths et al., 2006 ; Chang et al., 2011 , 2013a , b ; Anderson et al., 2012 ; Shwetha and Sudhakar, 2012 , 2014 ; Niu et al., 2013 ; Özdemir et al., 2013 ; Veddeng et al., 2014 ; Kazemi et al., 2016 , 2018 ; Maltese et al., 2016 ; Titova et al., 2016 ; Vajravelu et al., 2016 ; Haidarimoghadam et al., 2017 ; Soares and de Almondes, 2017 ; Williams et al., 2017 ; Persico et al., 2018 ; Taylor et al., 2019 ; Abdelhamid et al., 2020 ; Adams and Venter, 2020 ; Athar et al., 2020 ; James et al., 2021 ; Prasad et al., 2021 ; Stout et al., 2021 ; Sun et al., 2021 ; Zhao et al., 2021 ; An et al., 2022 ; Benítez-Provedo et al., 2022 ; Esmaily et al., 2022 ; Peng et al., 2022 ).

Abdelhamid et al. (2020) evaluated the differences in cognitive functions among 50 anaesthesia residents before and after a 24-h shift and found that after the shift their cognitive processes were impaired. Adams and Venter (2020) wanted to quantify the effects of shift work on cognitive functions in anaesthesiology trainees and observed that after 14-h night shift they had a decline in reaction time, psychomotor functions and attention. An et al. (2022) explored the effects of different work shifts on cognitive and executive performance in nurses, showing that reported fatigue after the evening shift can negatively affect reaction time. Anderson et al. (2012) sought to quantify the cognitive deterioration due to repeated exposure to 24- to 30-h shift and found progressive impairment due to chronic sleep deficiency. Athar et al. (2020) investigated the influence of shift work on executive functions, finding impairment of areas deputed to executive functions, such as prefrontal area. Benítez-Provedo et al. (2022) evaluated the change in several cognitive functions after a guard shift in physicians, noting lower performance in executive functions and attention. Chang and colleagues carried out three studies ( Chang et al., 2011 , 2013a , b ) on nurses: in the first one (2011) they compared cognitive performance at the time of maximum fatigue (3–4 a.m. on the last night shift of the rotation) among nurses who had been working for two, three, or four consecutive night shifts; the results showed greater impairment of perceptual and motor ability after two consecutive night shifts compared with four consecutive night shifts; in the second ( Chang et al., 2013a ), they explored changes in cognitive functions in the daytime of nurses working on fast rotating shifts and found a poorer performance on visual attentive task; in the third ( Chang et al., 2013b ), they explored the same changes as the previous study but after one block of fast forward rotating shift (2 days, 2 evenings, and 2 nights), finding no changes in cognitive performance. Deary and Tait (1987) evaluated the effect of sleep loss due to night shift on cognitive performance of medical house officers and found a negative effect on memory. Esmaily et al. (2022) evaluated the relationship between shift work and cognitive performance in nurses and the study showed that shift work can impair working memory and attention especially after a night shift. Griffiths et al. (2006) characterized the cognitive performance of anaesthetic registrars before and after a night shift noting a deterioration in performance speed at the end of night shift and a decline in cognitive performance after a series of night shifts. Haidarimoghadam et al. (2017) estimated the effects of consecutive night shifts and shift length on cognitive performance in a sample of room operators, finding a significant negative impact on reaction time and a reduction in correct responses and response time to the working memory task. James et al. (2021) determined the extent of changes in cognition in nurses after three consecutive 12-h shifts, detecting a significant negative effect on reaction time and number of psychomotor lapses. Kazemi and colleagues carried out two studies ( Kazemi et al., 2016 , 2018 ) on control room operators: in the first ( Kazemi et al., 2016 ), they found a decline in cognitive performance after day and night shifts; in the second ( Kazemi et al., 2018 ), they measured cognitive performance after seven and four consecutive night shifts and detected that those who worked seven consecutive night shifts had significantly better cognitive performance than those who worked four consecutive night shifts. Lingenfelser et al. (1994) evaluated cognitive functions in hospital doctors after a night shift, noting a considerable decrease. Maltese et al. (2016) assessed cognitive performance in a group of intensivist doctors after a night shift and found that it was significantly impaired. Niu et al. (2013) investigated selective attention in nurses with different shifts, noting that after the night shift both attention and operation speed of work were impaired. Orton and Gruzelier (1989) studied the effects of night shifts on young doctors and concluded that working long hours may adversely affect the cognitive functions. Özdemir et al. (2013) analysed the effects of shift work on health care workers’ memory, attention, and learning, finding a significantly poorer cognitive performance after night shift, particularly for working memory. Peng et al. (2022) noted a decrease in decision making competence due to night shift among nurses. Persico et al. (2018) evaluated cognitive performance of emergency physician after a night shift of 14 h or 24 h compared with a rest night at home and found a significant alteration of cognitive abilities after a 24-h shift but not after a 14-h shift. Petru et al. (2005) compared cognitive and psychomotor performance between night shift and early-late shift in a sample of automobile workers; the results indicated that, if chosen voluntarily, night shift had no immediate effects on performance compared with early-late shift. Prasad et al. (2021) evaluated changes in cognitive functions after 6 or 12 h shift compared with a day off in a sample of anaesthesia residents; they found significant differences in sustained attention performance between the 6 and 12 shift and the day off but not for memory and executive functions. Rouch et al. (2005) studied the long-term influence of shift work on verbal memory and speed performances, demonstrating a trend toward impairment over the long term. Saricaoğlu et al. (2005) investigated memory and learning between 12-h day shift and night shift in a sample of anaesthesia residents, noting that cognitive functions may be impaired after the night shift. Shwetha and Sudhakar (2012 , 2014) carried out two studies: in both studies they compared cognitive functions in business process outsourcing (BPO) shift workers versus non-BPO employees not working in shifts; it was found that BPO employees performed poorly in mental speed, learning, memory, and response inhibition but not in working memory. Smith et al. (1995) studied the effect of shift work on cognitive performance in nuclear power workers, finding significant variations in alertness and performance during 12-h night shift. Soares and de Almondes (2017) explored the connection between sleep quality (night shift versus day shift) and visuospatial perception in petrochemical workers; the results suggested that good sleep quality (day shift) could be associated with better visuospatial performance. Stout et al. (2021) sought to determine the impact of shift work on cognitive functions in firefighters and reported that even minimal sleep disruption, caused by night shift, affected cognitive functioning, increasing risk of poor work performance or injury. Sun et al. (2021) compared cognitive functions among different work shifts in an emergency department and found night shift work to be associated with a decrease in cognitive performance. Taylor et al. (2019) compared cognitive performance and vigilance at the start and end of each work shift within a three-shift schedule in police forces; the results showed a significant main effect of shift type in visuo-spatial task. Titova et al. (2016) investigated whether self-reported shift work history had an association with cognitive performance and results indicated that shift work history was related to poorer performance. Vajravelu et al. (2016) compared cognitive functions in night shift workers and day workers finding an alteration of attention in night shift workers. Veddeng et al. (2014) assessed whether gynaecologists have impaired laparoscopic skills and/or reduced cognitive functions after long on-call hours; results showed a small increase in reaction time but no other signs of reduced cognitive functions. Williams et al. (2017) investigated whether measurable executive function differences existed following a single overnight call versus routine shift, demonstrating that overnight call residents had both sensorimotor and cognitive slowing compared to routine daytime shift residents. Zhao et al. (2021) explored the effects of shift work on sleep and cognitive functions in middle-aged male miners, noting that night shift workers in adulthood would tend to have impaired working memory ( Chang et al., 2013b ).

3.3.2 Sedentary work and cognitive functions

Among 64 studies included in the systematic review, 7 were related to the effect of sedentary work on cognitive functions ( Table 5 ) ( John et al., 2009 ; Ohlinger et al., 2011 ; Russell et al., 2016 ; Baker et al., 2018a , b , c ; Bojsen-Møller et al., 2019 ).

Baker and colleagues carried out three studies ( Baker et al., 2018a , b , c ): in the first ( Baker et al., 2018a ), they examined the impact of prolonged standings on cognitive functions, finding that sustained attention deteriorated, while creative problem solving improved; in the second ( Baker et al., 2018b ), they investigated whether the use of a movement intervention when undertaking prolonged standing affected discomfort and cognitive functions and found no advantage in discomfort or cognitive functions; in the third ( Baker et al., 2018c ), they assessed cognitive functioning over 2 h of prolonged sitting and observed that increased discomfort in all areas of the body that can affect cognitive functioning. Bojsen-Møller et al. (2019) explored the associations between physical activity, sitting behaviour (SB) and cognitive functions: they found that duration of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA) was associated with better selective attention performance but results did not support the hypothesis that more time spent in MVPA or less time in SB was associated with better cognitive functions. John et al. (2009) assessed the differences between treadmill walking and sitting conditions on motor skills and cognitive functioning during work; results showed that treadmill walking caused a decrease in fine motor skills and math problem solving compared with the sitting condition however no differences were found concerning selective attention, processing speed or reading comprehension. Ohlinger et al. (2011) estimated the ability of performing cognitive tasks in workers while sitting, standing or walking at an active workstation; results indicated that the addition of the walking task did not alter performance in immediate recall and selective attention, but caused a decline in motor speed performance. Russell et al. (2016) examined the effect of sitting versus standing for 1 h per day for five consecutive days on cognitive functions and found no differences between the two conditions.

3.3.3 Occupational stress and cognitive functions

Among 64 studies included in the systematic review, 12 were related to the effect of occupational stress on cognitive functions ( Table 6 ) ( Van Der Linden et al., 2005 ; Elovainio et al., 2009 ; Nguyen et al., 2012 ; Vuori et al., 2014 ; Eskildsen et al., 2015 ; Gutshall et al., 2017 ; Landolt et al., 2017 ; de Souza-Talarico et al., 2020 ; Gafarov et al., 2021 ; Zhuo et al., 2021 ; Farahat et al., 2022 ; Cano-López et al., 2023 ).

Cano-López et al. (2023) characterized the level of burnout among primary healthcare professionals and its relationship with cognitive functions; results suggested that burnout was related to poor cognitive functioning. de Souza-Talarico et al. (2020) found that work-related stress was associated with lower performance in delayed recall, verbal fluency, and executive functions in middle aged workers. Eskildsen et al. (2015) examined whether patients presenting work-related stress complaints displayed cognitive impairment. When compared to stress-free, patients showed slightly reduced performance across all measured domains of the neuropsychological test battery. Elovainio et al. (2009) tested the association between high strain, active jobs and cognitive function in middle aged men and women; results showed that longer exposure to high strain job was associated with lower scores in most of the cognitive performance test, but these associations disappeared on adjustment for employment grade. Farahat et al. (2022) assessed work-related stress in healthcare workers and its impact on cognitive functions, noting an impairment due to stress exposure. Gafarov et al. (2021) investigated the effect of workplace stress on cognitive functions of young workers, and those who experienced stress displayed a decrease in cognitive functions. Gutshall et al. (2017) examined the effect of occupational stress on working memory in police officers, finding a deficit after a stressful event. Landolt et al. (2017) assessed the relationship between work stress, decision-making and reaction times in jockeys; higher occupational stress was significantly correlated with decrements in decision-making. Nguyen et al. (2012) examined the relationship between stress and change in cognitive functioning in farmworkers ascertaining that stress was an important risk factor for poor cognitive functioning. Van Der Linden et al. (2005) evaluated the association between burnout levels and attentional deficits, finding that burnout was associated with difficulties in voluntary control over attention and that the level of such difficulties varied along with the severity of burnout symptoms. Vuori et al. (2014) investigated the relationship between work stress and cognitive performance, noting a negative association between work stress and speed of memory retrieval. Zhuo et al. (2021) evaluated the association of job strain with cognitive ability and finding that high strain jobs were associated with lower levels of cognitive ability.

3.3.4 Prolonged working hours and cognitive functions

Among 64 studies included in the systematic review, 3 were related to the effect of prolonged working hours on cognitive functions ( Table 7 ) ( Proctor et al., 1996 ; Ansiau et al., 2008 ; Virtanen et al., 2009 ).

Ansiau et al. (2008) examined the consequences of working condition on the previous day on cognitive performance the following day, finding that both physical activity and working before 6 a.m. or after 10 p.m. on the previous day were significantly associated with poorer cognitive performance. Proctor et al. (1996) evaluated whether increased overtime work predicted impairment in cognitive performance, finding that increased overtime was significantly associated with impaired performance on several tests of attention and executive function. Virtanen et al. (2009) examined the association between long working hours and cognitive function in middle age and results showed that long working hours may have a negative effect on cognitive performance in middle age.

3.3.5 Expertise and cognitive functions

Among 64 studies included in the systematic review, 3 were related to the effect of expertise on cognitive functions ( Table 8 ) ( Colonia-Willner, 1998 ; Ovaskainen and Heikkilä, 2007 ; Wagner-Hartl et al., 2018 ).

Colonia-Willner (1998) sought to determine differences in cognitive performance in the domain of intelligence between expert and non-expert bank managers, and the results suggested that stabilization of some aspects of intelligence may occur in older age when workers are more experienced. Ovaskainen and Heikkilä (2007) determined the differences in cognitive abilities and productivity between timber harvester operators and students operators; results showed no differences in cognitive abilities but the highest productivity level among students was almost as the lowest productivity of the operators. Wagner-Hartl et al. (2018) examined cognitive functions in young and old (expert) white-collars during extended working time, finding slower reaction time and more error in older workers.

4 Discussion

Building on the three main theories “ Use-it-or-lose-it” ( Hultsch et al., 1999 ; Salthouse, 2006 , 2016 ), “ the Brain or Cognitive reserve hypothesis” ( Mortimer and Graves, 1993 ; Stern, 2002 ; Fratiglioni and Wang, 2007 ) and, “Schooler’s theory of environmental influences on cognitive functioning” ( Schooler, 1984 , 1990 ) which describe how different kinds of job and different work environments can influence the cognitive aging in a normative way, the question we asked was: “What happens to this normative functioning when factors intrinsic to the work itself, or factors caused by the work, intrude in workers’ lives and impair their cognitive functioning? “. We were sure that shift work, based on the very extensive literature on the subject, caused sleep disturbances, thus dysregulation of circadian rhythm and many other alterations ( Kantermann et al., 2012 ) that targeted cognitive functioning ( Ma et al., 2020 ). Just as we were certain that stress, including stress in the workplace, increased burnout risk and resulted in cognitive impairment ( Lupien et al., 2009 ) and that sedentary behaviours, on the other hand, had effects on cognitive functioning ( Gafni et al., 2021 ), but there were still doubts about their actual impact ( Magnon et al., 2018 ). But we also had the doubt that these were not the only factors that could influence cognitive functioning, for this reason we decided to draft a systematic review to classify the effects of different work-related factors on cognitive functions.

This review had to include studies that quantitatively assessed workers’ cognitive functions according to different work-related factors, aiming to identify potential variations in cognitive functioning between workers exposed and not exposed to these factors, as well as differences among workers with varying levels of exposure (e.g., the same workers exposed to both night shift and day shift).

At the end of the review process, we classified the included studies according to the strict criteria into five categories: “Shift Work,” “Occupational Stress,” Sedentary Work,” “Prolonged Working Hours,” and “Expertise. Therefore, two new categories (Extended Working Hours and Competence) were introduced in addition to the three assumed during the formulation of the research question.

The category with the largest number of included studies, as expected, was “shift work” (39 studies out of 64): since it is well known the neurocognitive impairment it can cause ( Vlasak et al., 2022 ), particularly when the shift is performed at night ( Leso et al., 2021 ), and therefore many studies dealt with it as a better quantification of the possible damage is essential. The other most studied category is “occupational stress” (12 studies), followed by “sedentary work” (7 studies) and “prolonged working hours” (3 studies), while the last 3 studies are focused on the effect of “expertise” on cognitive functions.

The included studies were conducted in developed countries across all continents (North America, South America, Europe, and Asia), while studies conducted in developing countries are still lacking.

In the following paragraphs we will discuss the results for each category one by one in separate sections.

4.1 Principal outcomes

4.1.1 shift work and cognitive functions.

Many studies focused their attention on the negative effects of shift work on circadian rhythm ( Schettini et al., 2023 ) and consequently on sleep and cognitive functions. Particularly, most studies highlighted detrimental effects: such among healthcare workers, medical professionals, as anaesthesiologists had a decline in reaction time, problem-solving, attention, and mental flexibility after night shift ( Saricaoğlu et al., 2005 ; Griffiths et al., 2006 ; Williams et al., 2017 ; Abdelhamid et al., 2020 ; Adams and Venter, 2020 ; Prasad et al., 2021 ), nurses had affected reaction time, motor ability, attention, cognitive flexibility, working memory, and decision making after work shift ( Chang et al., 2011 , 2013a , b ; Niu et al., 2013 ; James et al., 2021 ; Sun et al., 2021 ; An et al., 2022 ; Esmaily et al., 2022 ; Peng et al., 2022 ), other categories of physicians(intensivists, emergency doctors, gynaecologists, junior doctors, hospital doctors, medical house officers, postgraduate year one physicians) had a deficit in attention, executive function, memory, motor function, and reaction time after night shift ( Deary and Tait, 1987 ; Orton and Gruzelier, 1989 ; Lingenfelser et al., 1994 ; Anderson et al., 2012 ; Özdemir et al., 2013 ; Veddeng et al., 2014 ; Maltese et al., 2016 ; Persico et al., 2018 ; Benítez-Provedo et al., 2022 ). Other workers, such as security staff ( Vajravelu et al., 2016 ; Athar et al., 2020 ), control room operators ( Kazemi et al., 2016 , 2018 ; Haidarimoghadam et al., 2017 ), business process outsourcing employees ( Shwetha and Sudhakar, 2012 , 2014 ), power plant workers ( Smith et al., 1995 ), petrochemical workers ( Soares and de Almondes, 2017 ), firefighters ( Stout et al., 2021 ), police employees ( Taylor et al., 2019 ), miners ( Zhao et al., 2021 ), and generic groups of workers ( Rouch et al., 2005 ; Titova et al., 2016 ) had an impairment in one or more cognitive functions. The only study reporting a slight difference result from the others was the one involving automotive workers ( Petru et al., 2005 ) which suggest no immediate negative effects in cognitive and psychomotor performance of night shift workers when compared with other workers, but this effect is observed only when the night shift was voluntary. It is therefore clear from all these studies that shift work, particularly night work with the aggravation of altered circadian rhythms and sleep patterns, causes damage to cognitive functions, particularly those related to alertness, sustained attention and working memory, corroborating previous studies on sleep deprivation ( Nilsson et al., 2005 ; Joo et al., 2012 ). These results are in line with what we expected; the immense literature attesting to the damage to cognitive functioning caused by sleep problems is corroborated by studies involving those types of work that cause dysregulation. Long-term damage may be mitigated in jobs such as physician, which are less monotonous and repetitive than an assembly line in a factory, as asserted by “ the Brain or Cognitive reserve hypothesis” ( Mortimer and Graves, 1993 ; Stern, 2002 ; Fratiglioni and Wang, 2007 ), but the short-term effects remain equally hazardous to health.

4.1.2 Sedentary work and cognitive functions

Having sedentary behaviour more than 75% of the time was associated with cognitive impairment in middle age ( Gafni et al., 2021 ), however the included studies have different outcomes. Some studies found no correlation between sedentary/standing behaviours and decrease in cognitive functions ( Russell et al., 2016 ; Baker et al., 2018c ; Bojsen-Møller et al., 2019 ); other studies tried to use movement interventions or treadmill walking to counteract sedentary behaviours but, one of these ( Baker et al., 2018b ) found no advantage on cognitive functions and the other two of these ( John et al., 2009 ; Ohlinger et al., 2011 ) even found decreased performance in some tasks and no change in others; only one of the studies ( Baker et al., 2018a ) found a deterioration in sustained attention and reaction time, confirming results of previous studies. The overall results are not conclusive, partly due to the profound heterogeneity in the methods of the studies, which makes them difficult to compare even with the existing literature. Also for this category, as for the previous one, the results of the systematic review confirm the premises, the picture is still not entirely clear regarding the effect sedentary work can have on cognitive health. Sedentary work, combined with a incorrect diet and lack of physical activity, can certainly cause various health problems, such as cardiovascular disorders ( Elagizi et al., 2020 ), which can result in cognitive impairment ( Picano et al., 2014 ), but its effect per se is still not completely clear; more in-depth studies with different designs capable of isolating the sedentary factor and studying it individually, without the influence of other factors causing bias, will be necessary.

4.1.3 Occupational stress and cognitive functions

Stress exposure had a high detrimental effect on cognitive performance ( Chen et al., 2022 ) and accelerated cognitive decline ( Aggarwal et al., 2014 ). Health care workers are generally under great pressure that puts them at high risk of burnout, which has a critical effect on executive functioning ( Cano-López et al., 2023 ), especially when they are on the front-line, such as during COVID-19 pandemic ( Farahat et al., 2022 ). Among hospital workers, memory can be impaired by job stress as well ( Vuori et al., 2014 ). However, not only health care workers are affected by cognitive impairment as a result of stress, even police officers ( Gutshall et al., 2017 ), jockeys ( Landolt et al., 2017 ), and farmworkers ( Nguyen et al., 2012 ). Other studies have found lower cognitive performance related to job stress in young ( Gafarov et al., 2021 ), middle-aged ( Elovainio et al., 2009 ; de Souza-Talarico et al., 2020 ) workers, and after retirement ( Zhuo et al., 2021 ) explaining that each age group may be affected, although in different ways. In this case, the included studies have very consistent results, all indicating a relationship between stress, burnout and poor cognitive functioning. This category is very consistent with the initial hypothesis we posited: the negative effects of stress can be very insidious and affect any area of health, especially cognitive functioning, which deteriorates with lack of concentration, a sense of hopelessness and then often results or overlaps with much more serious psychiatric disorders such as depression or anxiety disorders ( Golonka et al., 2019 ).

4.1.4 Prolonged working hours and cognitive functions

Prolonged working hours category can be considered in the same way as shift work, but since it does not fit completely into the category, it deserved a separate section because while shift work has precise rules and defined hours, prolonged working hours consists precisely of working long hours without this control, without these rules, to the extent that work pervades almost the totality of a person’s daily routine. This resemblance suggests similar effects on cognitive functions: working after a certain time thereby extending one’s working hours ( Ansiau et al., 2008 ), increasing overtime commitments ( Proctor et al., 1996 ), and work more than 55 h per week compared with to 40 h per week ( Virtanen et al., 2009 ), leads to a poorer cognitive performance in executive functions and attention, and could even have negative effect on cognitive performance in middle age. As expected, all these studies point in the same direction, which is that increased work hours lead to a deterioration of cognitive function that can also be expressed in middle age. In our initial hypothesis, we had not considered this category, partly because, as mentioned, it could be assimilated with shift work, but the three studies included deserved a separate category as they expressed different concepts and looked at cognitive impairment that is not caused by circadian dysregulation leading to sleep problems, but rather by an immersion in work that causes workers to pervade their daily lives.

4.1.5 Expertise and cognitive functions

The studies in this category matched our inclusion criteria, but deserve special mention because they deal with how experience can modify certain cognitive functions both positively and negatively. The three included studies regarding expertise found: a better cognitive functioning on the domain of intelligence in experts compared to non-experts bank managers ( Colonia-Willner, 1998 ), no difference related to experience but only to good mastering of different abilities in timber harvester operators ( Ovaskainen and Heikkilä, 2007 ), and slower reaction time in older but more experienced white-collar workers than in younger workers ( Wagner-Hartl et al., 2018 ). This category is very heterogeneous and it cannot be said whether the experience has a positive or negative effect in general, in the intellectual part of the job certainly the effect can be described as positive but in the physical part the same does not apply, so we can say that the effects vary especially depending on the type of work, so a unified result cannot be obtained.

4.2 Limitations

This systematic review has some limitations. First, the inclusion of keywords such as “work,” however indispensable, massively enlarged the initial pool (on the order of one million papers), forcing us to eliminate less relevant keywords so as not to make it impossible to complete the selection work; conversely, we may have missed some papers to be included. Notwithstanding this, we believe that the initial pool of 61,781 papers constituted a solid starting point for reviewing all the literature regarding the topic. Another limitation concerned the choice of PICOS criteria; we were forced to narrow the field by excluding all work concerning the assessment of cognitive functions in workers exposed to chemicals/or neurotoxic substances, or physical hazard. This choice was made to focus our effort only on work-related risk factors modifiable through workplace health and wellbeing promotion strategies, otherwise the work would have been too dispersed; unfortunately, several interesting papers that could provide information on the effects of these types of jobs on cognitive functions were excluded. The last limitation concerns the included studies, some of which have small sample size but, in some cases these sample size was justified by power analyses, in other cases this lack of reliability is pointed out in AXIS Tool ( Supplementary Table S2 ).

5 Conclusion

In conclusion, our systematic review showed that shift work and occupational stress have a consistent detrimental effect on cognitive functions, which is justified by the large number of studied included that confirm the literature on the topic. Shift work leads to chronic sleep deficiency, which in the long-term causes problems with attention and working memory, especially in the category of healthcare workers (e.g., nurses) who are exposed to shifts at any age and for long periods of time. Work-related stress increases the risk of burnout, which is related to poor cognitive functioning, and this risk cuts across different jobs.

The category of prolonged working hours also shows a strong negative effect on cognitive functions, but only three studies were included, that have very large sample sizes, not sufficient to give this result the same consistency as the two in the previous categories, although the negative effects are very clear; in fact, it can have immediate effects on cognitive functions, but the worst-case scenario is when the effects manifest themselves with early cognitive impairment in middle age.

So, we found evidence that relates to our research question posed as the goal of this paper the work-related factors that most and most negatively affect cognitive function are: shift work (especially night shift), occupational stress, and, most likely although further evidence is needed, prolonged working hours. These findings suggest that workplace health and wellbeing promotion should consider, for example, reducing or rescheduling night shifts to lessen circadian disruption and inadequate sleep quality ( Garde et al., 2020 ) and/or reducing occupational stress by creating less demanding and more resourceful work environments ( Bakker and Demerouti, 2014 ). Another idea, supported by literature, especially during prolonged working hours, but also in the other two harmful categories, is to take micro-breaks as possible cognitive recovery mechanisms in the workplace; specifically, these are not cognitive activities such as reading short magazine articles, doing personal projects or surfing the Internet, which exacerbate the effects of the workload, but rather moments of relaxation such as short naps, walks around the office, stretching, listening to music and daydreaming, or micro-social break activities, which refer to relatively short online or offline interactions with colleagues and other people outside work while at work ( Kim et al., 2017 ).

All these suggestions emphasise the importance of protecting the health of during the working period in order to lead them to a safer working age that can lead to a reduced risk of early cognitive decline in post-retirement life.

In contrast, results related to sedentary work and expertise have been inconclusive and extremely miscellaneous, thus more studies are needed; but on this point it is very important to emphasise, especially for studies on sedentary work, the lack of a common methodological design. These studies would benefit greatly from a group-based design, in which conditions (e.g., sedentary work habits) are isolated and compared in terms of between-subject comparisons ( Schumann et al., 2022 ).

Finally, in order to get a total picture on the impact of work-related factors on cognitive functioning, two future prospective could be: in future systematic reviews include categories excluded a priori from this research (e.g., workers exposed to chemicals/or neurotoxic substances, or physical hazard), and conduct studies with larger samples ( Bonzini et al., 2023 ) and more consistent methodologies.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/ Supplementary material , further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

PB: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. CT: Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing. AF: Writing – review & editing. TB: Writing – review & editing. AC: Writing – review & editing. CC: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. LF: Writing – review & editing. FM: Writing – review & editing. ML: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. MB: Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This was supported, after peer review, by the Italian National Institute for Insurance against Accidents at Work (INAIL) with the BRIC 2019 project (“PROAGEING – Promuovere la produttività e il benessere dei lavoratori che invecchiano: studio prospettico di work ability, età cognitiva e biologica in un mondo del lavoro in cambiamento”).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1351625/full#supplementary-material

Abdelhamid, B. M., Omar, H., Hassan, M. M., Embaby, S. A., Rady, A., and Mohamed Aly, H. (2020). Effects of partial sleep deprivation following night shift on cognitive functions of Egyptian anesthesiologists; prospective observational study. Egypt. J. Anaesth. 36, 61–68. doi: 10.1080/11101849.2020.1768630

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Adams, T. P., and Venter, S. (2020). All night long: An assessment of the cognitive effects of night shift work in anaesthesiology trainees. South. Afr. J. Anaesth. Analg. 26, 287–292. doi: 10.36303/SAJAA.2020.26.6.2361

Aggarwal, N. T., Wilson, R. S., Beck, T. L., Rajan, K. B., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Evans, D. A., et al. (2014). Perceived stress and change in cognitive function among adults 65 years and older. Psychosom. Med. 76, 80–85. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

An, R., Li, C., Ai, S., Wu, Y., Luo, X., Li, X., et al. (2022). Effect of shift work on fatigue, reaction time and accuracy of nurses in the Department of Neurology: a cross-sectional observational study. J. Nurs. Manag. 30, 2074–2083. doi: 10.1111/jonm.13665

Andel, R., Crowe, M., Hahn, E. A., Mortimer, J. A., Pedersen, N. L., Fratiglioni, L., et al. (2012). Work-related stress may increase the risk of vascular dementia. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60, 60–67. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03777.x

Anderson, C., Sullivan, J. P., Flynn-Evans, E. E., Cade, B. E., Czeisler, C. A., and Lockley, S. W. (2012). Deterioration of neurobehavioral performance in resident physicians during repeated exposure to extended duration work shifts. Sleep 35, 1137–1146. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2004

Ansiau, D., Wild, P., Niezborala, M., Rouch, I., and Marquié, J. C. (2008). Effects of working conditions and sleep of the previous day on cognitive performance. Appl. Ergon. 39, 99–106. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2007.01.004

Athar, M. E., Atef-Vahid, M.-K., and Ashouri, A. (2020). The influence of shift work on the quality of sleep and executive functions. J. Circadian Rhythms 18, 1–8. doi: 10.5334/jcr.194

Baker, R., Coenen, P., Howie, E., Lee, J., Williamson, A., and Straker, L. (2018a). A detailed description of the short-term musculoskeletal and cognitive effects of prolonged standing for office computer work. Ergonomics 61, 877–890. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2017.1420825

Baker, R., Coenen, P., Howie, E., Lee, J., Williamson, A., and Straker, L. (2018b). Musculoskeletal and cognitive effects of a movement intervention during prolonged standing for office work. Hum. Factors 60, 947–961. doi: 10.1177/0018720818783945

Baker, R., Coenen, P., Howie, E., Williamson, A., and Straker, L. (2018c). The short term musculoskeletal and cognitive effects of prolonged sitting during office computer work. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15:15. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15081678

Bakker, A. B., and Demerouti, E. (2014). “Job demands–resources theory” in Wellbeing: A complete reference guide . eds. P. Y. Chen and C. L. Cooper (New York: John Wiley & Sons), 1–28.

Google Scholar

Belin, A., Dixon, K., Davis, A., Graveling, R., Dupont, C., Crawford, J., et al. (2016). The ageing workforce: implications for occupational safety and health: a research review . Luxembourg: Publication Office of the European Union.

Benítez-Provedo, C., Talavera, B., García-Azorín, D., and Marcos-Dolado, A. (2022). The cognitive impact of guard shifts in physicians: a before-after study. Neurol. Sci. 43, 1701–1708. doi: 10.1007/s10072-021-05501-x

Bojsen-Møller, E., Boraxbekk, C.-J., Ekblom, Ö., Blom, V., and Ekblom, M. M. (2019). Relationships between physical activity, sedentary behaviour and cognitive functions in office workers. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16:4721. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16234721

Bonzini, M., Comotti, A., Fattori, A., Serra, D., Laurino, M., Mastorci, F., et al. (2023). Promoting health and productivity among ageing workers: a longitudinal study on work ability, biological and cognitive age in modern workplaces (PROAGEING study). BMC Public Health 23:1115. doi: 10.1186/s12889-023-16010-1

Brownlow, J. A., Miller, K. E., and Gehrman, P. R. (2020). Insomnia and cognitive performance. Sleep Med. Clin. 15, 71–76. doi: 10.1016/j.jsmc.2019.10.002

Bufano, P., Albertini, N., Chiarelli, S., Barone, V., Valleggi, M., Parrini, G., et al. (2023). Weakened sustained attention and increased cognitive effort after Total sleep deprivation: a virtual reality ecological study. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact. 39, 3546–3555. doi: 10.1080/10447318.2022.2099047

Cano-López, I., Pérez, M. I., and Puig-Pérez, S. (2023). Burnout is related to executive dysfunction in primary healthcare professionals working in rural areas. Rev. Neurol. 76, 91–99. doi: 10.33588/rn.7603.2022283

Chang, Y.-S., Chen, H.-L., Hsu, C.-Y., Su, S.-F., Liu, C.-K., and Hsu, C. (2013a). Nurses working on fast rotating shifts overestimate cognitive function and the capacity of maintaining wakefulness during the daytime after a rotating shift. Sleep Med. 14, 605–613. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2013.03.011

Chang, Y. S., Wu, Y. H., Hsu, C. Y., Tang, S. H., Yang, L. L., and Su, S. F. (2011). Impairment of perceptual and motor abilities at the end of a night shift is greater in nurses working fast rotating shifts. Sleep Med. 12, 866–869. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2011.03.018

Chang, Y.-S., Wu, Y.-H., Su, S.-F., Hsu, C.-Y., and Chen, H.-L. (2013b). Factors related with the ability to maintain wakefulness in the daytime after fast and forward rotating shifts. Kaohsiung J. Med. Sci. 29, 611–616. doi: 10.1016/j.kjms.2013.01.012

Chen, R., Williams, D. R., Nishimi, K., Slopen, N., Kubzansky, L. D., and Weuve, J. (2022). A life course approach to understanding stress exposures and cognitive function among middle-aged and older adults. Soc. Sci. Med. 314, 314:115448. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.115448

Cohen, R. A., Marsiske, M. M., and Smith, G. E. (2019). Neuropsychology of aging. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 167, 149–180. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-804766-8.00010-8

Colonia-Willner, R. (1998). Practical intelligence at work: relationship between aging and cognitive efficiency among managers in a bank environment. Psychol. Aging 13, 45–57. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.13.1.45

de Souza-Talarico, J. N., Suemoto, C. K., Santos, I. S., Griep, R. H., Yamaguti, S. T. F., Lotufo, P. A., et al. (2020). Work-related stress and cognitive performance among middle-aged adults: the Brazilian longitudinal study of adult health (ELSA-Brasil). Stress. Health 36, 19–30. doi: 10.1002/smi.2906

Deary, I. J., and Tait, R. (1987). Effects of sleep disruption on cognitive performance and mood in medical house officers. Br. Med. J. (Clin. Res. Ed.) 295, 1513–1516. doi: 10.1136/bmj.295.6612.1513

Downes, M. J., Brennan, M. L., Williams, H. C., and Dean, R. S. (2016). Development of a critical appraisal tool to assess the quality of cross-sectional studies (AXIS). BMJ Open 6:e011458. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-011458

Elagizi, A., Kachur, S., Carbone, S., Lavie, C. J., and Blair, S. N. (2020). A review of obesity, physical activity, and cardiovascular disease. Curr. Obes. Rep. 9, 571–581. doi: 10.1007/s13679-020-00403-z

Elovainio, M., Feme, J. E., Singh-Manoux, A., Gimeno, D., De Vogli, R., Shipley, M. J., et al. (2009). Cumulative exposure to high-strain and active jobs as predictors of cognitive function: the Whitehall II study. Occup. Environ. Med. 66, 32–37. doi: 10.1136/oem.2008.039305

Eskildsen, A., Andersen, L. P., Pedersen, A. D., Vandborg, S. K., and Andersen, J. H. (2015). Work-related stress is associated with impaired neuropsychological test performance: a clinical cross-sectional study. Stress 18, 198–207. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2015.1004629

Esmaily, A., Jambarsang, S., Mohammadian, F., and Mehrparvar, A. H. (2022). Effect of shift work on working memory, attention and response time in nurses. Int. J. Occup. Saf. Ergon. 28, 1085–1090. doi: 10.1080/10803548.2020.1863656

Falck, R. S., Davis, J. C., and Liu-Ambrose, T. (2017). What is the association between sedentary behaviour and cognitive function? A systematic review. Br. J. Sports Med. 51, 800–811. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2015-095551

Farahat, S. A., Amin, O. R., Hamdy, H. S., and Fouad, M. M. (2022). The impact of work-related stress on the cognition domain of executive functioning of health care workers during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 95, 1079–1090. doi: 10.1007/s00420-021-01814-8

Fisher, G. G., Chaffee, D. S., Tetrick, L. E., Davalos, D. B., and Potter, G. G. (2017). Cognitive functioning, aging, and work: a review and recommendations for research and practice. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 22, 314–336. doi: 10.1037/ocp0000086

Fratiglioni, L., and Wang, H.-X. (2007). Brain reserve hypothesis in dementia. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 12, 11–22. doi: 10.3233/jad-2007-12103

Gafarov, V. V., Sukhanov, A. V., Gromova, E. A., Panov, D. O., Denisova, D. V., Gagulin, I. V., et al. (2021). Workplace stress and cognitive functions (a population based study of adults aged 25-44 years). Nevrol. Neiropsikhiatr. Psikhosomatika 13, 30–36. doi: 10.14412/2074-2711-2021-4-30-36

Gafni, T., Shuval, K., Weinstein, G., Barlow, C. E., Gabriel, K. P., Willis, B. L., et al. (2021). Sitting time, physical activity, and cognitive impairment in midlife and older adults. J. Aging Phys. Act. 30, 355–363. doi: 10.1123/japa.2020-0473

Garde, A. H., Begtrup, L., Bjorvatn, B., Bonde, J. P., Hansen, J., Hansen, Å. M., et al. (2020). How to schedule night shift work in order to reduce health and safety risks. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 46, 557–569. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.3920

Golonka, K., Mojsa-Kaja, J., Blukacz, M., Gawłowska, M., and Marek, T. (2019). Occupational burnout and its overlapping effect with depression and anxiety. Int. J. Occup. Med. Environ. Health 32, 229–244. doi: 10.13075/ijomeh.1896.01323

Griffiths, J. D., McCutcheon, C., Silbert, B. S., and Maruff, P. (2006). A prospective observational study of the effect of night duty on the cognitive function of anaesthetic registrars. Anaesth. Intensive Care 34, 621–628. doi: 10.1177/0310057x0603400512

Gutshall, C. L., Hampton, D. P. Jr., Sebetan, I. M., Stein, P. C., and Broxtermann, T. J. (2017). The effects of occupational stress on cognitive performance in police officers. Police Pract. Res. 18, 463–477. doi: 10.1080/15614263.2017.1288120

Haidarimoghadam, R., Kazemi, R., Motamedzadeh, M., Golmohamadi, R., Soltanian, A., and Zoghipaydar, M. R. (2017). The effects of consecutive night shifts and shift length on cognitive performance and sleepiness: a field study. Int. J. Occup. Saf. Ergon. 23, 251–258. doi: 10.1080/10803548.2016.1244422

Hultsch, D. F., Hammer, M., and Small, B. J. (1993). Age differences in cognitive performance in later life: relationships to self-reported health and activity life style. J. Gerontol. 48, P1–P11. doi: 10.1093/geronj/48.1.p1

Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J., and Dixon, R. A. (1999). Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychol. Aging 14, 245–263. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.14.2.245

Ihle, A., Oris, M., Fagot, D., Baeriswyl, M., Guichard, E., and Kliegel, M. (2015). The Association of Leisure Activities in middle adulthood with cognitive performance in old age: the moderating role of educational level. Gerontology 61, 543–550. doi: 10.1159/000381311

James, L., Elkins-Brown, N., Wilson, M., James, S. M., Dotson, E., Edwards, C. D., et al. (2021). The effects of three consecutive 12-hour shifts on cognition, sleepiness, and domains of nursing performance in day and night shift nurses: a quasi-experimental study. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 123:104041. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2021.104041

John, D., Bassett, D., Thompson, D., Fairbrother, J., and Baldwin, D. (2009). Effect of using a treadmill workstation on performance of simulated office work tasks. J. Phys. Act. Health 6, 617–624. doi: 10.1123/jpah.6.5.617

Joo, E. Y., Yoon, C. W., Koo, D. L., Kim, D., and Hong, S. B. (2012). Adverse effects of 24 hours of sleep deprivation on cognition and stress hormones. J. Clin. Neurol. 8, 146–150. doi: 10.3988/jcn.2012.8.2.146

Kantermann, T., Wehrens, S. M. T., Ulhôa, M. A., Moreno, C., and Skene, D. J. (2012). Noisy and individual, but doable: shift-work research in humans. Prog. Brain Res. 199, 399–411. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-59427-3.00022-8

Kazemi, R., Haidarimoghadam, R., Motamedzadeh, M., Golmohamadi, R., Soltanian, A., and Zoghipaydar, M. R. (2016). Effects of shift work on cognitive performance, sleep quality, and sleepiness among petrochemical control room operators. J. Circadian Rhythms 14, 1–8. doi: 10.5334/jcr.134

Kazemi, R., Motamedzade, M., Golmohammadi, R., Mokarami, H., Hemmatjo, R., and Heidarimoghadam, R. (2018). Field study of effects of night shifts on cognitive performance, salivary melatonin, and sleep. Saf. Health Work 9, 203–209. doi: 10.1016/j.shaw.2017.07.007

Kim, S., Park, Y., and Niu, Q. (2017). Micro-break activities at work to recover from daily work demands. J. Organ. Behav. 38, 28–44. doi: 10.1002/job.2109

Landolt, K., Maruff, P., Horan, B., Kingsley, M., Kinsella, G., O’Halloran, P. D., et al. (2017). Chronic work stress and decreased vagal tone impairs decision making and reaction time in jockeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology 84, 151–158. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.07.238

Leso, V., Fontana, L., Caturano, A., Vetrani, I., Fedele, M., and Iavicoli, I. (2021). Impact of shift work and long working hours on worker cognitive functions: current evidence and future research needs. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:6540. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18126540

Lingenfelser, T., Kaschel, R., Weber, A., Zaiser-Kaschel, H., Jakober, B., and Küper, J. (1994). Young hospital doctors after night duty: their task-specific cognitive status and emotional condition. Med. Educ. 28, 566–572. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.1994.tb02737.x

Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., and Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 434–445. doi: 10.1038/nrn2639

Ma, Y., Liang, L., Zheng, F., Shi, L., Zhong, B., and Xie, W. (2020). Association between sleep duration and cognitive decline. JAMA Netw. Open 3:e2013573. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.13573

Magnon, V., Vallet, G. T., and Auxiette, C. (2018). Sedentary behavior at work and cognitive functioning: a systematic review. Front. Public Health 6:239. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2018.00239

Maltese, F., Adda, M., Bablon, A., Hraeich, S., Guervilly, C., Lehingue, S., et al. (2016). Night shift decreases cognitive performance of ICU physicians. Intensive Care Med. 42, 393–400. doi: 10.1007/s00134-015-4115-4

Marconcin, P., Zymbal, V., Gouveia, É. R., Jones, B., Marques, A., Marconcin, P., et al. (2021). “Sedentary behaviour: definition, determinants, impacts on health, and current recommendations” in Sedentary behaviour—a contemporary view . eds. A. Marques and É. R. Gouveia (London: IntechOpen).

McEwen, B. S., and Sapolsky, R. M. (1995). Stress and cognitive function. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 5, 205–216. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(95)80028-x

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G.PRISMA Group (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

Mortimer, J. A., and Graves, A. B. (1993). Education and other socioeconomic determinants of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 43, S39–S44,

Nguyen, H. T., Quandt, S. A., Grzywacz, J. G., Chen, H., Galván, L., Kitner-Triolo, M. H., et al. (2012). Stress and cognitive function in Latino farmworkers. Am. J. Ind. Med. 55, 707–713. doi: 10.1002/ajim.22035

Nilsson, J. P., Söderström, M., Karlsson, A. U., Lekander, M., Akerstedt, T., Lindroth, N. E., et al. (2005). Less effective executive functioning after one night’s sleep deprivation. J. Sleep Res. 14, 1–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2005.00442.x

Niu, S.-F., Chu, H., Chen, C.-H., Chung, M.-H., Chang, Y.-S., Liao, Y.-M., et al. (2013). A comparison of the effects of fixed-and rotating-shift schedules on nursing staff attention levels: a randomized trial. Biol. Res. Nurs. 15, 443–450. doi: 10.1177/1099800412445907

Ohlinger, C. M., Horn, T. S., Berg, W. P., and Cox, R. H. (2011). The effect of active workstation use on measures of cognition, attention, and motor skill. J. Phys. Act. Health 8, 119–125. doi: 10.1123/jpah.8.1.119

Orton, D. I., and Gruzelier, J. H. (1989). Adverse changes in mood and cognitive performance of house officers after night duty. Br. Med. J. 298, 21–23. doi: 10.1136/bmj.298.6665.21

Ovaskainen, H., and Heikkilä, M. (2007). Visuospatial cognitive abilities in cut-to-length single-grip timber harvester work. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 37, 771–780. doi: 10.1016/j.ergon.2007.06.004

Özdemir, P. G., Selvi, Y., Özkol, H., Aydin, A., Tülüce, Y., Boysan, M., et al. (2013). The influence of shift work on cognitive functions and oxidative stress. Psychiatry Res. 210, 1219–1225. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.09.022

Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

Park, J. H., Moon, J. H., Kim, H. J., Kong, M. H., and Oh, Y. H. (2020). Sedentary lifestyle: overview of updated evidence of potential health risks. Korean J. Fam. Med. 41, 365–373. doi: 10.4082/kjfm.20.0165

Peng, J., Lu, H., Zhang, J., Shao, Y., Wang, L., and Lv, J. (2022). Need for cognition moderates the impairment of decision making caused by nightshift work in nurses. Sci. Rep. 12:1756. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-05843-2

Persico, N., Maltese, F., Ferrigno, C., Bablon, A., Marmillot, C., Papazian, L., et al. (2018). Influence of shift duration on cognitive performance of emergency physicians: a prospective cross-sectional study. Ann. Emerg. Med. 72, 171–180. doi: 10.1016/j.annemergmed.2017.10.005

Petru, R., Wittmann, M., Nowak, D., Birkholz, B., and Angerer, P. (2005). Effects of working permanent night shifts and two shifts on cognitive and psychomotor performance. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 78, 109–116. doi: 10.1007/s00420-004-0585-3

Picano, E., Bruno, R. M., Ferrari, G. F., and Bonuccelli, U. (2014). Cognitive impairment and cardiovascular disease: so near, so far. Int. J. Cardiol. 175, 21–29. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2014.05.004

Prasad, R. V., Gupta, S. L., and Swaminathan, S. (2021). Evaluation of cognitive and psychomotor functional changes in anesthesiology residents after 12 hours of continuous work in operation theater: an observational study. Anesth. Essays Res. 15, 362–367. doi: 10.4103/aer.aer_153_21

Proctor, S. P., White, R. F., Robins, T. G., Echeverria, D., and Rocskay, A. Z. (1996). Effect of overtime work on cognitive function in automotive workers. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 22, 124–132. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.120

Rinsky-Halivni, L., Hovav, B., Christiani, D. C., and Brammli-Greenberg, S. (2022). Aging workforce with reduced work capacity: from organizational challenges to successful accommodations sustaining productivity and well-being. Soc. Sci. Med. 312:115369. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.115369

Rouch, I., Wild, P., Ansiau, D., and Marquie, J.-C. (2005). Shiftwork experience, age and cognitive performance. Ergonomics 48, 1282–1293. doi: 10.1080/00140130500241670

Russell, B. A., Summers, M. J., Tranent, P. J., Palmer, M. A., Cooley, P. D., and Pedersen, S. J. (2016). A randomised control trial of the cognitive effects of working in a seated as opposed to a standing position in office workers. Ergonomics 59, 737–744. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2015.1094579

Salazar-Xirinachs, J. M. (2012). Promoting longer life and ensuring work ability . ILO – International Labour Organization. Available at: https://www.ilo.org/employment/about/executive-director-office/statements-speeches-old/WCMS_190915/lang--en/index.htm

Salthouse, T. A. (2006). Mental exercise and mental aging: evaluating the validity of the “use it or lose it” hypothesis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 1, 68–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00005.x

Salthouse, T. A. (2016). Theoretical perspectives on cognitive aging . London: Psychology Press.

Saricaoğlu, F., Akinci, S. B., Gözaçan, A., Güner, B., Rezaki, M., and Aypar, U. (2005). The effect of day and night shift working on the attention and anxiety levels of anesthesia residents. Turk. Psikiyatri. Derg. 16, 106–112.

Schettini, M. A. S., Passos, R. F. D. N., and Koike, B. D. V. (2023). Shift work and metabolic syndrome updates: a systematic review. Sleep Sci. 16, 237–247. doi: 10.1055/s-0043-1770798

Schooler, C. (1984). Psychological effects of complex environments during the life span: a review and theory. Intelligence 8, 259–281. doi: 10.1016/0160-2896(84)90011-4

Schooler, C. (1990). “Twenty one—psychosocial factors and effective cognitive functioning in adulthood” in Handbook of the psychology of aging . eds. J. E. Birren and K. W. Schaie. Third ed (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 347–358.

Schooler, C., and Mulatu, M. S. (2001). The reciprocal effects of leisure time activities and intellectual functioning in older people: a longitudinal analysis. Psychol. Aging 16, 466–482. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.16.3.466

Schooler, C., Mulatu, M. S., and Oates, G. (1999). The continuing effects of substantively complex work on the intellectual functioning of older workers. Psychol. Aging 14, 483–506. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.14.3.483

Schumann, F., Steinborn, M. B., Kürten, J., Cao, L., Händel, B. F., and Huestegge, L. (2022). Restoration of attention by rest in a multitasking world: theory, methodology, and empirical evidence. Front. Psychol. 13:867978. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.867978

Shwetha, B., and Sudhakar, H. (2012). Influence of shift work on cognitive performance in male business process outsourcing employees. Indian J. Occup. Environ. Med. 16, 114–118. doi: 10.4103/0019-5278.111751

Shwetha, B. L., and Sudhakar, H. H. (2014). Learning, memory & executive function in female BPO employees exposed to regular shifts. Natl. J. Physiol. Pharm. Pharmacol. 4, 20–24. doi: 10.5455/njppp.2014.4.140620133

Smith, L., Spelten, E., and Folkard, S. (1995). Shiftwork effects in nuclear power workers: a field study using portable computers. Work Stress 9, 235–244. doi: 10.1080/02678379508256559

Soares, C. S., and de Almondes, K. M. (2017). Sleep quality and visuospatial performance in rotating shifts workers from a petrochemical company. Biol. Rhythm. Res. 48, 403–415. doi: 10.1080/09291016.2016.1263025

Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 8, 448–460. doi: 10.1017/S1355617702813248

Stern, Y., Alexander, G. E., Prohovnik, I., Stricks, L., Link, B., Lennon, M. C., et al. (1995). Relationship between lifetime occupation and parietal flow: implications for a reserve against Alzheimer’s disease pathology. Neurology 45, 55–60. doi: 10.1212/wnl.45.1.55

Stout, J. W., Beidel, D. C., Brush, D., and Bowers, C. (2021). Sleep disturbance and cognitive functioning among firefighters. J. Health Psychol. 26, 2248–2259. doi: 10.1177/1359105320909861

Sun, J., Stewart, P., Chiew, A., Becker, T., Siu, W., Varndell, W., et al. (2021). Association between shift work and cognitive performance on the trail making test in emergency department health officers. Emerg. Med. Australas. 33, 711–717. doi: 10.1111/1742-6723.13753

Taylor, Y., Merat, N., and Jamson, S. (2019). The effects of fatigue on cognitive performance in police officers and staff during a forward rotating shift pattern. Saf. Health Work 10, 67–74. doi: 10.1016/j.shaw.2018.08.003

Titova, O. E., Lindberg, E., Elmståhl, S., Lind, L., Schiöth, H. B., and Benedict, C. (2016). Association between shift work history and performance on the trail making test in middle-aged and elderly humans: the EpiHealth study. Neurobiol. Aging 45, 23–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.05.007

Vajravelu, H. R., Krishnan, P., Ayyavoo, S., and Narayanan, K. (2016). Evaluation of cognition using neurophysiological and neuropsychological tests in rotating night shift workers: a pilot study. Natl. J. Physiol. Pharm. Pharmacol. 6, 146–149. doi: 10.5455/njppp.2016.6.01122015100

Van Der Linden, D., Keijsers, G. P. J., Eling, P., and Van Schaijk, R. (2005). Work stress and attentional difficulties: An initial study on burnout and cognitive failures. Work Stress 19, 23–36. doi: 10.1080/02678370500065275

Veddeng, A., Husby, T., Engelsen, I. B., Kent, A., and Flaatten, H. (2014). Impact of night shifts on laparoscopic skills and cognitive function among gynecologists. Acta Obstet. Gynecol. Scand. 93, 1255–1261. doi: 10.1111/aogs.12496

Vendramin, P., Valenduc, G., Molinié, A.-F., Volkoff, S., Ajzen, M., and Leonard, E. (2012). Sustainable work and the ageing workforce . Eur. Secur. Available at: https://www.eurofound.europa.eu/publications/report/2012/working-conditions-social-policies/sustainable-work-and-the-ageing-workforce

Virtanen, M., Singh-Manoux, A., Ferrie, J. E., Gimeno, D., Marmot, M. G., Elovainio, M., et al. (2009). Long working hours and cognitive function: the Whitehall II study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 169, 596–605. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn382

Vives, A., Gray, N., González, F., and Molina, A. (2018). Gender and ageing at work in Chile: employment, working conditions, work-life balance and health of men and women in an ageing workforce. Ann. Work Expo. Health 62, 475–489. doi: 10.1093/annweh/wxy021

Vlasak, T., Dujlovic, T., and Barth, A. (2022). Neurocognitive impairment in night and shift workers: a meta-analysis of observational studies. Occup. Environ. Med. 79, 365–372. doi: 10.1136/oemed-2021-107847

Vuori, M., Akila, R., Kalakoski, V., Pentti, J., Kivimäki, M., Vahtera, J., et al. (2014). Association between exposure to work stressors and cognitive performance. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 56, 354–360. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0000000000000129

Wagner-Hartl, V., Grossi, N. R., and Kallus, K. W. (2018). Impact of age and hearing impairment on work performance during long working hours. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15:98. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15010098

Wang, Z., Chen, W.-H., Li, S.-X., He, Z.-M., Zhu, W.-L., Ji, Y.-B., et al. (2021). Gut microbiota modulates the inflammatory response and cognitive impairment induced by sleep deprivation. Mol. Psychiatry 26, 6277–6292. doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01113-1

Williams, G. W., Shankar, B., Klier, E. M., Chuang, A. Z., El Marjiya-Villarreal, S., Nwokolo, O. O., et al. (2017). Sensorimotor and executive function slowing in anesthesiology residents after overnight shifts. J. Clin. Anesth. 40, 110–116. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinane.2017.04.002

Zhao, X.-C., Han, K.-Y., Gao, Y.-Y., Li, N., Wang, L., Yu, L.-L., et al. (2021). Effects of shift work on sleep and cognitive function among male miners. Psychiatry Res. 297:113716. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2021.113716

Zhuo, L.-B., Pei, J.-J., Yan, Z., Yao, W., Hao, C.-F., and Wang, H.-X. (2021). Working life job strain status and cognitive aging in Europe: a 12-year follow-up study: job strain and cognitive aging. J. Affect. Disord. 295, 1177–1183. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.08.114

Keywords: cognitive functions, aging workforce, occupational stress, shift work, sedentary work, cognitive impairment

Citation: Bufano P, Di Tecco C, Fattori A, Barnini T, Comotti A, Ciocan C, Ferrari L, Mastorci F, Laurino M and Bonzini M (2024) The effects of work on cognitive functions: a systematic review. Front. Psychol . 15:1351625. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1351625

Received: 07 December 2023; Accepted: 22 April 2024; Published: 09 May 2024.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2024 Bufano, Di Tecco, Fattori, Barnini, Comotti, Ciocan, Ferrari, Mastorci, Laurino and Bonzini. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marco Laurino, [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

IMAGES

  1. 15 Literature Review Examples (2024)

    a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

  2. A Review of Literature of Anxiety Amongst Adolescence

    a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

  3. (PDF) Social Anxiety Disorder and Social Skills: A Critical Review of

    a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

  4. Literature Review

    a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

  5. A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational

    a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

  6. Test anxiety causes university students to underperform in their

    a review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

VIDEO

  1. ASMR Therapist

  2. Literature Review

  3. How do we manage anxiety in school children?

  4. Talk About It Tuesday: Anxiety Disorders

  5. Episode 16 Learning by Doing: Assessments as Your Ally

  6. Understanding and Navigating the Invigilator App and Moodle Proctoring

COMMENTS

  1. A review of the literature on anxiety for educational assessments

    Details. This review is part of a wider collection of material focused on managing test anxiety. It pulls together statistics and academic literature relating to the causes, symptoms and effects ...

  2. PDF A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

    The literature refers to debilitating performance anxiety, whereby excessive anxiety about a performance disables the individual from demonstrating their maximum skills and abilities. In performance-based assessments, the role of the audience is an important contributory factor to anxiety (Putwain, 2008a).

  3. A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments

    Psychology. Cogent Psychology. 2023. Abstract Test anxiety is a distinct fear of exams that is highly upsetting and can seriously impair academic performance and generate negative behavioural, physiological, and emotional responses.…. Expand. 1. Highly Influenced. PDF. 3 Excerpts.

  4. A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational

    Aims: The aims of this thesis were firstly, to explore how teachers support student test-anxiety based on the literature available prior to the covid-19 pandemic. Secondly, to explore teacher and educational psychologists' experiences of supporting test-anxiety in the context of a global pandemic. Phase 1: A systematic literature review was ...

  5. An international systematic literature review of test anxiety

    Across different countries, age ranges, and assessment regimes, a high level of test anxiety has been identified as posing a risk to student academic achievement and wellbeing. The present study provides a rigorous systematic review of the international evidence on test anxiety interventions in the period 2011-2018.

  6. Test anxiety in primary school children: A 20-year systematic review

    1. Introduction. Test anxiety has been a topic of research in the education and psychology literature for over 70 years (Sarason & Mandler, 1952).Test anxiety can impact a student's performance and wellbeing as well as impact their family and school staff who show concern for the student (Lavy, 2020; Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development [OECD], 2015).

  7. Supporting Student Success: The Role of Test Anxiety, Emotional

    The test anxiety construct occupies a prominent position in the educational and psychology literature, given the widespread use of high-stakes testing to evaluate student learning and educational quality (Putwain, 2008; Segool et al., 2014; Symes & Putwain, 2020; Zeinder, 1998).Although theoretical frameworks differ in their conceptualization of the construct, there is general agreement that ...

  8. A review of the literature on anxiety for educational assessments

    This review is part of a wider collection of material focused on managing test anxiety. It pulls together statistics and academic literature relating to the causes, symptoms and effects of assessment-related anxiety, known as test anxiety in the literature. It is an academic piece of writing that considers what is currently known about test ...

  9. A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational

    Overall conclusions. Appendix A - A timeline of key policy changes and campaigns supporting the transformation in mental health in recent years. Appendix B - The ten most common types of concerns discussed during Childline counselling sessions, report published 2018. Appendix C - Further details on contributors to test anxiety.

  10. A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational

    A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments Howard, Emma , Office of Qualifications and Examinations Regulation (OFQUAL), corp creator. (2020) A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments.

  11. A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational

    A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments Understanding trends in mental health issues in young people Alongside an increase in reported poor mental health, the use of mental health services by children and young people has also increased in recent years, as indicated by statistics from the NHS and Childline.

  12. 'I'm going to feel stressed, but now I know how to handle it': reducing

    Increasing concerns reported in the media about the impact of test anxiety have led to a recent OFQUAL review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments (OFQUAL, 2020) and a briefing paper by the British Psychological Society (BPS, 2020). OFQUAL have also produced a series of blogs on the topic and support materials around ...

  13. Systematic review of student anxiety and performance during objective

    Anxiety was assessed via multiple scales. Six out of the eight studies assessing the relationship OSCE-related anxiety and OSCE performance found no association between the two measures. Contrary to literature concerning test anxiety in higher education, female gender was not associated with OSCE-related anxiety.

  14. (PDF) A systematic review of research on teaching anxiety

    [email protected]. Abstract. Teaching anxiety is a significant affective state that may profoundly affect both the. learning and teaching processes. However, there is no conclusive ...

  15. PDF A SYSTEMATIC REVIEW OF RESEARCH ON TEACHING ANXIETY

    sources and effects of teaching anxiety in an educational context. The study adopted a research design utilizing a systematic review methodology. This systematic review aims to present an overview of the current literature on teaching anxiety and explore the variables related to teaching anxiety in terms of its causes and effects.

  16. A Systematic Review of Research on Teaching Anxiety

    Teaching anxiety is a significant affective state that may profoundly affect both the learning and teaching processes. However, there is no conclusive evidence regarding the sources and effects of teaching anxiety in an educational context. The study adopted a research design utilizing a systematic review methodology. This systematic review aims to present an overview of the current literature ...

  17. Education for Mental Health Toolkit

    Educational Psychologist. 2002;37:91-105; Marin MF, Lord C, Andrews J, Juster RP, Sindi S, Arsenault-Lapierre G, Fiocco AJ, Lupien SJ. Chronic stress, cognitive functioning and mental health. Neurobiology of learning and memory. 2011 Nov 1;96(4):583-95. Howard E, A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments.

  18. Systematic review of student anxiety and performance during ...

    Contrary to literature concerning test anxiety in higher education, female gender was not associated with OSCE-related anxiety. Conclusion: OSCE-related anxiety appears to have minimal to no influence on student performance. Future studies should utilize standardized anxiety assessments and should seek to understand anxiety's effects on student ...

  19. 'I'm going to feel stressed, but now I know how to handle it': reducing

    to a recent OFQUAL review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments (OFQUAL, 2020) and a briefing paper by the British Psychological Society (BPS, 2020). OFQUAL have also produced a series of blogs on the topic and support materials around 'coping with exam pressure'. The role of educational psychologists Educational ...

  20. Assessing and Treating Child Anxiety in Schools

    Abstract. Anxiety disorders in youth are common and, if left untreated, can lead to a variety of negative sequelae. Randomized clinical trials have demonstrated that cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) is an efficacious treatment for anxiety disorders in youth with preliminary evidence showing that CBT can be successfully transported into schools.

  21. A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational

    A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments. Emma Howard. 1 January 2020. Office of Qualifications and Examinations Regulation.

  22. Student Anxiety and Practical Strategies for the School Setting: A

    This thesis aims to. answer these questions through a review of literature regarding the most common types of. anxiety seen in school-aged children; the comorbid diagnoses that often accompany anxiety; the. behaviors and indicators frequently observed in students with anxiety; and the possible causes of.

  23. A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational ...

    RESEARCH AND ANALYSIS A review of the literature concerning anxiety for educational assessments Author Emma Howard, Ofqual Acknowledgements. I would like to thank colleagues at Ofqual and David Putwain (Professor of Education and Early Childhood Studies, School of Education, Liverpool John Moores University) who reviewed and provided helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.

  24. The effects of work on cognitive functions: a systematic review

    The aim of this paper was to systematically review the scientific literature related to the effects of work on cognitive functions to assess which work-related factors most adversely affect them. ... we were forced to narrow the field by excluding all work concerning the assessment of cognitive functions in workers exposed to chemicals/or ...

  25. Home

    The SAT Suite of Assessments is an integrated system of tests including the SAT, PSAT/NMSQT, PSAT 10, and PSAT 8/9. Content Alignment See how content evolves throughout the tests in the SAT Suite.