U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • v.9(Suppl 1); 2008 Jul

Logo of emborep

The socio-cultural evolution of our species

Jürgen Klüver

1 University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany.E-mail: [email protected]

Many biologists and social scientists have noted that with the development of human culture, the biological evolution of Homo sapiens was usurped by socio-cultural evolution. The construction of artificial environments and social structures created new criteria for selection, and biological fitness was replaced by ‘cultural fitness', which is often different for different cultures and is generally not measured by the number of offspring. Moreover, the mechanism of socio-cultural evolution is different from the model of biological evolution that was proposed by Charles Darwin (1809–1882), and refined by many others. In essence, socio-cultural evolution is ‘Lamarckian' in nature—it is an example of acquired inheritance, as described by the French naturalist Jean-Baptiste Lamarck (1744–1829)—because humans are able to pass on cultural achievements to the next generation.

Yet, the idea that cultural fitness has replaced biological fitness does not fully take into account the thousands of years of human biological evolution that occurred long before socio-cultural evolution, in its strictest sense, took its course. Modern Homo sapiens first appeared about 200,000 years ago; however, socio-cultural evolution only began about 10,000 years ago, when early hunter–gatherer societies began to change their simple forms of segmentary social differentiation during the so-called Neolithic revolution, which was mainly caused by the invention of agriculture and cattle breeding. In mathematical terms, one could say that human biological evolution created an attractor: a stable state impervious to change. Various mathematical models of biological evolution, namely the genetic algorithm ( Holland, 1975 ), show that the generation of such an attractor is the usual result of evolutionary processes ( Klüver, 2000 ). Nevertheless, socio-cultural evolution did not end biological evolution; in fact, for most of the time that Homo sapiens has existed, socio-cultural evolution has been so slow that it could not have affected biological evolution. Here, I attempt to explain why modern humans existed long before socio-cultural evolution really began.

What does socio-cultural evolution mean? There have been many attempts to define this ambiguous concept ( Trigger, 1998 ), which have interpreted the term ‘evolution' in a literal sense and assumed that socio-cultural evolution is determined by the same mechanisms as its biological counterpart. It is true that the evolution of human societies and cultures shares some similarities with biological evolution, but in many respects these two are not the same. Therefore, at the outset, it is necessary to give a precise definition of evolution in the field of human societies ( Klüver, 2002 ).

Socio-cultural evolution, as the name implies, has two dimensions: social and cultural. Some of the great social theorists of the last century defined ‘culture' in terms of the generally accepted knowledge of a certain society or social group ( Habermas, 1981 ; Giddens, 1984 ). Under this definition, ‘knowledge' is not limited to natural and social phenomena, but includes, for example, religion, worldviews and moral values. Similarly, ‘accepted' does not imply that such knowledge is true according to scientific standards—for example, the Judaeo-Christian belief that God created the world—but only that it is accepted within one culture as ‘true'. The definition of ‘social' naturally refers to social structures. ‘Social' can be defined as the set of rules that govern all social interactions in a certain society. The separation of power into legislative, judicative and executive arms of government in modern democracies is such a rule, as is the rule to drive on the right-hand side of the road in most countries. In mathematical terms, we can then define a society ( S ) using the equation S = ( St , C ), where C refers to culture and St refers to social structure.

In essence, socio-cultural evolution is ‘Lamarckian' in nature […] because humans are able to pass on cultural achievements to the next generation

Culture and social structure are, of course, abstracts that cannot be quantified and must instead be translated into empirical categories—namely, observable actions by, and interactions of, social actors. In a meta-theoretical sense, this transforms the concepts of culture and social structure into an action theory because only individual actors can be the units of an empirical social science. The main concepts here are social roles and their occupants.

Consider, for example, the social role of a medical doctor. A doctor is characterized by his or her knowledge of disease diagnosis, how to choose appropriate therapies and how to tell the patient to follow the therapy. However, the role of the doctor is also defined by specific rules—the Hippocratic Oath, for example—and by specific laws about how to treat patients, or how to adhere to health insurance or national regulations. Similarly, the role of a university professor is defined by specific scientific knowledge and specific rules of interaction with respect to, for example, teaching, publishing and dealing with university administration. We can therefore define a social role ( r ) as r = ( k , ru ), where k is the role-specific knowledge and ru represents the role-specific rules of social interaction ( Berger & Luckmann, 1966 ).

An individual in a society is a social actor when he or she occupies a specific social role, which is not necessarily a professional role. There are other social roles such as being a parent or being a member of a political party, and it is relatively easy to define the social rules and role-specific knowledge of these positions. Therefore, we can define a society as a web of social roles, the occupants of which interact according to the rules and to the knowledge that define these roles. A society is then produced and reproduced through the role-specific interactions of the role occupants. In many cases, the social structure and culture of a society merely reproduce—that is, they do not change notably. Yet, sometimes roles and interactions change markedly, and the social structure and culture change accordingly. Such times are called periods of reform or—in the extreme—revolutions.

…for most of the time that Homo sapiens has existed, socio-cultural evolution has been so slow that it could not have affected biological evolution

Now that we have defined what we mean by a society—based on culture and social structure—we can define socio-cultural evolution as the creation and change of social roles through new knowledge that changes and creates social rules. Socio-cultural evolution, then, alters and enlarges a society in the two dimensions of social structure and culture. The driving force is new ideas in the cultural dimension and the ensuing changes to the social structure that create new social rules of interaction. Social roles in a societal system therefore “become the equivalent of genes in a genetic system” ( Read, 2005 ); however, this is only a formal equivalence, as the evolutionary mechanisms in these cases operate differently.

When we speak of social roles, we must make an important distinction. On the one hand, some social roles—those of artisans, craftspeople, artists, technicians, scientists or entrepreneurs, for example—are defined by ‘creative tasks', which expand the culture of society. Cultural evolution is therefore only possible if the occupants of creative roles enjoy a certain degree of freedom. On the other hand, there are roles—those of priests, politicians or teachers, for example—that serve to maintain social traditions, culture and social structures. We can call these ‘maintenance roles' in contrast to the ‘creative roles'. These are essential for the integration of a society because traditional norms and values allow a society to maintain its societal identity.

The crucial factor for the evolutionary potential of a society, then, is the relationship between creative roles and maintenance roles. If the maintenance roles have a strong influence on the creative roles, the occupants of creative roles cannot fulfil their creativity and the development of culture stagnates; a society gets caught in a cultural evolutionary attractor. The relationship between these two classes of roles is the decisive parameter for the evolutionary power of a society, which can be called an evolutionary parameter (EP) and determines the evolutionary fate of a society. The ultimately unsuccessful attempts of the Catholic Church to silence proponents of the heliocentric model of the planetary system—most notably Giordano Bruno (1548–1600) and Galileo Galilei (1564–1642)—is an example of an unfavourable EP. A society must have a certain degree of heterogeneity with respect to the existence of different roles and the social ‘distance' between the two kinds of roles. If a society is too homogeneous, socio-cultural evolution will stop sooner or later.

Looking at historical examples can validate this general hypothesis about the logic of socio-cultural evolution. Starting in the fourteenth century, the European nations entered a period characterized by reforms, revolutions and scientific progress—known respectively as the Renaissance, the Reformation and the Enlightenment—and eventually evolved into modern Western societies. The technological and social competitors of Europe during the Middle Ages—notably feudal China and the Islamic societies—did not change in the same way because they did not have the EP values of European societies, despite the fact that they were culturally and scientifically more advanced than feudal Europe. The main reason for this was that the occupants of creative roles in Europe enjoyed a larger degree of freedom than those in rival societies ( Klüver, 2002 ; Needham, 1970 ). In particular, the large trading cities of the Hanse, the Flemish cities and the cities of Northern Italy were centres of cultural growth with a certain political autonomy. This environment gave the occupants of creative social roles the benefit of greater freedom from the feudal political powers and the Catholic Church. This political and social structure had no parallels in the other great cultures.

On the basis of this hypothesis, our research group constructed mathematical models of socio-cultural evolution, the so-called socio-cultural algorithm (SCA) and the expanded socio-cultural cognitive algorithm (SCCA). These are multi-agent systems that consist of artificial actors. Each actor is represented by a combination of different neural nets, and the social relations between the actors are modelled by a cellular automaton and a Boolean net ( Klüver, 2002 ; Klüver et al, 2003 ). Each actor is able to occupy a certain social role, can learn from others and can generate new ideas—of course, in an idealized and simplified manner. The sum of all the ideas that these actors generate is the level of the respective culture. According to the general evolutionary hypothesis, the actors, if they occupy a creative role, develop new ideas in proportion to the influence of the occupants of maintenance roles. We ran the models with different EP values and different numbers of actors ranging from 100 to more than 1,000,000. One important result was that the number of actors had no significant impact on the results—the evolutionary logic operated in small or large artificial societies.

Social roles in a societal system therefore “become the equivalent of genes in a genetic system”…

One typical result that we observed was a so-called Toynbee development, named after the British historian Arnold Toynbee (1889–1975) who showed that this is the fate of all known cultures ( Toynbee, 1934–39 ; Fig 1 ). This artificial culture grows quickly but eventually slows down and stagnates. Most EP values led to this development in our simulations, which shows, at least in part, the significance of EP values and provides an explanation for the historical processes.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is embor200835-f1.jpg

A Toynbee development.

Only a few evolutionarily favourable EP values were able to generate a different image ( Fig 2 ). In these cases, the artificial culture did not stop, but was able to continue to advance its cultural growth for as long as it existed. This might be the fate of Western culture, as its growth, particularly in science and technology, shows no detectable limits at present. Again, the reason for this is the decisive role of the EP and the relatively large degree of freedom that the occupants of creative roles enjoy in the West. In addition, we assume that the EP values themselves changed during European cultural development because the current values are even more favourable than those during the Medieval Ages. In other words, the EP values start a process of socio-cultural evolution and are themselves changed by this process—an evolution of evolution.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is embor200835-f2.jpg

A Western development.

The general hypothesis about socio-cultural evolution, the historical data and our simulations can apparently explain human history as an evolutionary process. In particular, they can explain the special path of European and, subsequently, Western culture. They might also answer the question raised at the beginning of this article: why did it take such a long time before socio-cultural evolution started at the beginning of the Neolithic revolution?

Early hunter–gatherer societies, or segmentary differentiated tribal societies as they are called in sociology, are homogeneous. There is little differentiation of social roles, which are mostly based on gender and age. The creative potential of these early humans could not unfold; small degrees of labour division did not allow for special roles and a common worldview of animistic religions further hindered individual thinking. It took a long time for these societies to become sufficiently heterogeneous to generate the creative achievements of the Neolithic revolution, which, in turn, changed the social structure of societies. The segmentary differentiated societies became stratified into social hierarchies and allowed a significant division of labour. Yet it took a long time to achieve this stage of socio-cultural evolution—and many tribal societies did not reach it at all—because only small processes of differentiation took place and creative individuals could only slowly create new ideas in their respective society. The long period of time between the biological emergence of Homo sapiens and the Neolithic revolution was necessary to allow these slow processes to generate a sufficiently heterogeneous society that could move to the next step in the evolutionary process. In other words, the Neolithic revolution could only take place when some societies were sufficiently differentiated to allow for individual creative processes. Moreover, it can be assumed that the initial EP values of the tribal societies did not significantly change with the slow growth of human culture.

The decisive question is, of course, whether this model of socio-cultural evolution can help us to make some educated guesses about the possible future of mankind. Will the process of globalization lead to a world culture that is characterized by the Western way? In theoretical sociology, we call this the hypothesis of universal modernization, which implies that only Western societies are truly modern ones and that the process of modernization will change all societies until they become modern in the Western sense, albeit with local variants. This classical hypothesis dates back to the Enlightenment, and was formulated in its most influential form by the social theorists Karl Marx (1818–1883) and Max Weber (1864–1920). Of course, the universal modernization hypothesis was, and still is, much discussed and criticized, in particular for being Eurocentric. One of the most famous critiques was made by the American political scientist Samuel Huntington in his bestseller The Clash of Civilizations ( Huntington, 1996 ). Although I cannot discuss this and other criticisms of the modernization hypothesis for reasons of space, I can provide empirical data to validate the hypothesis, and make a methodical proposal based on the model of socio-cultural evolution and the SCCA program.

…the social future of mankind is probably a global society based on the traditions of Western societies with local adaptations

The European, and eventually Western, process of modernization is characterized by certain economic, political, educational and gender-based criteria that are indicators of modern development. If we apply these criteria to the developmental processes in different countries, we can detect astonishing parallels to Western history ( Oesterdiekhoff, 2003 ). The economical relevance of the agrarian sector is decreasing in developing countries, even in Africa, whereas industry is gaining in importance. The same trend is valid for urbanization processes: in all developing countries, the rural population is decreasing as large cities emerge, just as happened in Europe in the eighteenth century. In most places, birth rates are also steadily declining—a trend that has been observed in Western countries since the nineteenth century. The mean marriage age of women is rising, which is certainly one cause of the decline in the birth rate and an important indicator of an increasing degree of female autonomy. The average number of democratic or semi-democratic societies is increasing—in which ‘democratic' means adopting the Western model of a parliamentary democracy. The levels of literacy and the number of participants in higher education are increasing in most countries, and many rapidly developing countries are investing massively in science and technology—not only large nations such as China and India, but also various South American countries. All of the trends that are now visible in developing countries were seen previously in Europe and North America as they progressed towards modern Western culture.

Although there are certainly other factors at work, this selection shows that many countries that are on their way to modernization follow the path of Western societies. Even politically regressive processes, for example the rise of Islamic theocracies, are expected—indeed, European countries experienced regressive fascist movements or periods of stagnation. Modernization as a form of socio-cultural evolution is not a linear process. As a preliminary summary, it seems that Marx, Weber and the other adherents of the universal modernization theory are right. At least, the data are more compatible with the universalistic theory of modernization than with its rivals.

Furthermore, our SCCA model provides support for this theory. The theoretical foundation of the model is the assumption that socio-cultural evolution depends on an increasing degree of role autonomy in important social domains. In particular, this assumption can explain the fact that the process of modernization emerged in Europe before it became the core of Western culture. If these theoretical and mathematical assumptions are correct, the validity of the universalistic theory of modernization—the question of the final socio-cultural character that will result from globalization processes—can be analysed in a twofold manner.

Empirical data from developing countries indicate that there is a growing trend in favour of role autonomy—again referring to gender roles and the rise of higher education. Overall, women are becoming more autonomous, and education is emancipating itself from religious and political influences in developing countries. Again, women's rights and the introduction of universal education marked important points in the history and development of Western countries. Such data can then be inserted into simulations, such as our SCA or SCCA, to predict roughly the probable development of these countries. Clearly, even such micro-sociologically based simulation programs can only give predictions about probable developments, but this is still better than a ‘best guess' or wishful thinking.

In any case, the future of our species depends on more factors than can be covered in this article. Yet, the social future of mankind is probably a global society based on the traditions of Western societies with local adaptations. Neither China nor India will become a mirror of the USA, but similarly neither Germany nor France is such a mirror. In the end, I believe, Marx and Weber will be proved right.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is embor200835-i1.jpg

  • Berger P, Luckmann T (1966) The Social Construction of Reality . New York, NY, USA: Doubleday [ Google Scholar ]
  • Giddens A (1984) The Constitution of Society. Outlines of the Theory of Structuration . Cambridge, UK: Polity Press [ Google Scholar ]
  • Habermas J (1981) Theorie des Kommunikativen Handelns , Vol. 2 . Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Suhrkamp [ Google Scholar ]
  • Holland JR (1975) Adaptation in Natural and Artificial Systems . Ann Arbor, MI, USA: University of Michigan Press [ Google Scholar ]
  • Huntington SP (1996) The Clash of Civilizations . New York, NY, USA: Simon & Schuster [ Google Scholar ]
  • Klüver J (2000) The Dynamics and Evolution of Social Systems. New Foundations of a Mathematical Sociology . Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer [ Google Scholar ]
  • Klüver J (2002) An Essay Concerning Sociocultural Evolution. Theoretical Principles and Mathematical Models . Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer [ Google Scholar ]
  • Klüver J, Malecki R, Schmidt J, Stoica C (2003) Sociocultural evolution and cognitive ontogenesis. A sociocultural cognitive algorithm . Comput Math Organ Theor 9 : 255–273 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Needham J (1970) Clerks and Craftsmen in China and the West . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oesterdiekhoff GW (2003) Entwicklung der Weltgesellschaft . Hamburg, Germany: Lit [ Google Scholar ]
  • Read D (2005) Change in the form of evolution. Transitions from primate to hominid forms of social organization . J Math Sociol 29 : 91–114 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Toynbee A (1934) A Study of History ( 12 Vols). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press [ Google Scholar ]
  • Trigger BG (1998) Sociocultural Evolution. New Perspectives on the Past . Oxford, UK: Blackwell [ Google Scholar ]

Human Biology: An Evolutionary and Biocultural Perspective

  • This person is not on ResearchGate, or hasn't claimed this research yet.

Barry Bogin at Loughborough University

  • Loughborough University

Dennis O'Rourke at University of Kansas

  • University of Kansas

Abstract and Figures

Model of poverty risks showing the multiple pathways through which poverty can affect health, growth, and school achievement. From Crooks (1995).

Discover the world's research

  • 25+ million members
  • 160+ million publication pages
  • 2.3+ billion citations

Anna Tornberg

  • Silvia Lucrecia Dahinten

Bibiana Cadena

  • Douglas H. Ubelaker

Kaisa Vehkalahti

  • Juma Khalifa Juma Aldarmaki

Rafael Tomás-Cardoso

  • Andres Frisancho
  • R. Brooke Thomas

Thomas L Leatherman

  • Michael A. Little
  • Sewall Wright

Naoyuki Takahata

  • F.B. Livingstone

Tatjana Buklijas

  • Alan Beedle

Mark Hanson

  • H.S. Padrón
  • Recruit researchers
  • Join for free
  • Login Email Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google Welcome back! Please log in. Email · Hint Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google No account? Sign up

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

LOOKING BACK THROUGH BIO-CULTURAL AND SOCIAL EVOLUTION

Profile image of Aaron Paul Santos

Related Papers

Human culture changes over time and varies across space. Two main approaches to study cultural evolution have developed in the last fifty years: human behavioural ecology and a suite of perspectives centred on the role of cultural transmission. The latter are often confusingly referred to with the name of the phenomenon they are trying to explain, ‘cultural evolution’. We argue that this is unhelpful and is generating confusion, including the claim that human behavioural ecology disregards cultural evolution. The aim of behavioural ecology is to explain human behaviours, and the vast majority of them are at least to some extent cultural. In addition, culture forms part of the ecology that determines the costs and benefits associated with adopting a behaviour. Thus, human behavioural ecologists have studied cultural evolution from the very beginning, even though they have not focussed on social learning. We explore three examples in detail: kinship systems, religious institutions, an...

human biocultural and social evolution essay

Matteo Rivera

Human Ecology

William Durham

Dwight Read

Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences

Kevin Laland

Stephan Lewandowsky

Journal of Human Evolution

Vernon Reynolds

Sorry, this document isn't available for viewing at this time.

In the meantime, you can download the document by clicking the 'download' button above., related papers.

Romain Duda

Martin Soukup

Matti Kamppinen

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

Oren Kolodny

Liane Gabora

Evolutionary Biology-new York

michael bradie

Behavioral Ecology

Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning

Carlos Tomaz

Colin Irwin

Mohamed Hassouna

Evolution and Human Behavior

Journal of Theoretical Biology

Laureano Castro

Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B

Eugene Ruyle

Robin Allott

Philosophical Transactions of …

Julien Passinhas

Emanuele Serrelli

American Journal of Physical Anthropology

Leslie Lieberman

Journal of Bioeconomics

Christian Cordes

Marcus Feldman

Current Anthropology

Politics and the Life Sciences

Frank Salter

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

Niche Construction Theory and Human Biocultural Evolution

  • First Online: 04 June 2019

Cite this chapter

human biocultural and social evolution essay

  • Felix Riede 2  

76k Accesses

22 Citations

4 Altmetric

Biologists and anthropologists have extensively documented how many animals—human and non-human—modify their immediate surroundings, some subtly, others extensively. This trend is carried to its extreme in Homo sapiens to the point where many of us today live in the almost entirely constructed niches of the built urban environment. Proponents of niche construction theory (NCT) argue that classical evolutionary theory does not account satisfactorily for organisms’ active niche modification that impacts selective parameters for themselves and/or also other organisms. Complementing evolutionary theory focused on genetic change alone as well as gene-culture co-evolutionary models, NCT aims at integrating ecology, anthropology and evolution by a greater awareness to ecological inheritances. The key to the niche construction approach therefore is the inclusion of organism-induced environmental modification bequeathed from the modifying generation to its offspring. Critically then, it is either selection that leads to changes over time in a given population, or individuals induce changes in their environment in order to offset or channel further selection. While there is intense debate about the merits of NCT within the biosciences, archaeology is in a situation to make an important contribution here: it allows for a greater role of organismal agency in the evolutionary process, and many objects as well as features in the archaeological record speak directly about lasting modifications of the environment. In this chapter, I aim to provide a richly but by no means exhaustively referenced review of the early emergence and development of NCT as well as the controversies surrounding it. I illustrate the specific application of NCT in anthropology and archaeology by focusing on selected behaviours of premodern and modern humans and their co-evolutionary impacts such as the use of fire, changing human-animal and human-plant relations and landscape modifications. I also highlight more subtle forms of niche construction that may have left traces in the cognitive make-up of past and indeed present populations and link NCT with the notion of the Anthropocene. NCT remains a contested body of theory. In concluding, I point to potential archaeological research avenues that can make significant contributions to this emerging field.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

human biocultural and social evolution essay

Evolutionary Ecology and Ethnobiology

human biocultural and social evolution essay

Domestication as a model system for niche construction theory

Niche construction theory as an explanatory framework for human phenomena.

Aiello, L. C., & Wheeler, P. (1995). The expensive-tissue hypothesis: The brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Current Anthropology, 36 (2), 199–221. https://doi.org/10.2307/2744104

Article   Google Scholar  

Allaby, R. G., Stevens, C., Lucas, L., Maeda, O., & Fuller, D. Q. (2017). Geographic mosaics and changing rates of cereal domestication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372 (1735), 20160429. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0429

Bakker, E. S., Gill, J. L., Johnson, C. N., Vera, F. W. M., Sandom, C. J., Asner, G. P., et al. (2016). Combining paleo-data and modern exclosure experiments to assess the impact of megafauna extinctions on woody vegetation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113 (4), 847–855. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1502545112

Barton, C. M., Clark, G. A., & Cohen, A. E. (1994). Art as information: Explaining upper palaeolithic art in Western Europe. World Archaeology, 26 (2), 185–207.

Bellwood, P. (2005). First farmers: The origins of agricultural societies . Oxford: Blackwell.

Google Scholar  

Berkes, F., Colding, J., & Folke, C. (2000). Rediscovery of traditional ecological knowledge as adaptive management. Ecological Applications, 10 (5), 1251–1262. https://doi.org/10.2307/2641280

Binford, L. R. (1962). Archaeology as anthropology. American Antiquity, 28 (2), 217–225. https://doi.org/10.2307/278380

Bird, D. W., Bird, R. B., Codding, B. F., & Taylor, N. (2016). A landscape architecture of fire: Cultural emergence and ecological pyrodiversity in Australia’s Western Desert. Current Anthropology, 57 (S13), S65–S79. https://doi.org/10.1086/685763

Bird, D. W., Bird, R. B., & Parker, C. H. (2005). Aboriginal burning regimes and hunting strategies in Australia’s Western Desert. Human Ecology, 33 (4), 443–464. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10745-005-5155-0

Bird, R. B., Bird, D. W., Codding, B. F., Parker, C. H., & Jones, J. H. (2008). The “fire stick farming” hypothesis: Australian aboriginal foraging strategies, biodiversity, and anthropogenic fire mosaics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105 (39), 14796–14801. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0804757105

Bleed, P. (2006). Living in the human niche. Evolutionary Anthropology, 15 (1), 8–10. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20084

Boggs, C. (2016). Human niche construction and the anthropocene. In R. Emmett & T. Lekan (Eds.), Whose anthropocene? Revisiting Dipesh Chakrabarty’s ‘four theses’ (RCC perspectives: Transformations in environment and society) (Vol. 2, pp. 27–31). München: Rachel Carson Centre.

Bond, W. J., & Keeley, J. E. (2005). Fire as a global ‘herbivore’: The ecology and evolution of flammable ecosystems. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 20 (7), 387–394.

Boogert, N. J., Paterson, D. M., & Laland, K. N. (2006). The implications of niche construction and ecosystem engineering for conservation biology. Bioscience, 56 (7), 570–578.

Bos, J. A. A., & Janssen, C. R. (1996). Local impact of palaeolithic man on the environment during the end of the last glacial in the Netherlands. Journal of Archaeological Science, 23 (5), 731–739. https://doi.org/10.1006/jasc.1996.0069

Braje, T. J., & Erlandson, J. M. (2013). Human acceleration of animal and plant extinctions: A late pleistocene, holocene, and anthropocene continuum. Anthropocene, 4 , 14–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ancene.2013.08.003

Brewer, J., Gelfand, M., Jackson, J. C., MacDonald, I. F., Peregrine, P. N., Richerson, P. J., et al. (2017). Grand challenges for the study of cultural evolution. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 1 , 0070. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41559-017-0070

Brewer, J., & Riede, F. (2018). Cultural heritage and climate adaptation. A cultural evolutionary perspective for the Anthropocene. World Archaeology , 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/00438243.2018.1527246

Brewer, S., Jackson, S. T., & Williams, J. W. (2012). Paleoecoinformatics: Applying geohistorical data to ecological questions. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 27 (2), 104–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2011.09.009

Brown, A. G., Basell, L. S., & Farbstein, R. (2017). Eels, beavers, and horses: Human niche construction in the European late upper palaeolithic. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society, 83 , 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1017/ppr.2017.6

Bulbulia, J. (2008). Meme infection or religious niche construction? An adaptationist alternative to the cultural maladaptationist hypothesis. Method & Theory in the Study of Religion, 20 (1), 67–107. https://doi.org/10.1163/157006808x260241

Butzer, K. W. (1982). Archaeology as human ecology. Method and theory for a contextual approach . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Book   Google Scholar  

Carey, J. (2016). Core concept: Are we in the “Anthropocene”? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113 (15), 3908–3909.

Carroll, J., Clasen, M., Jonsson, E., Kratschmer, A. R., McKerracher, L., Riede, F., et al. (2017). Biocultural theory: The current state of knowledge. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 11 (1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1037/ebs0000058

Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1973a). Cultural versus biological inheritance. Phenotypic transmission from parents to children (a theory of the effect of parental phenotypes on children’s phenotypes). American Journal of Human Genetics, 25 , 618–637.

Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1973b). Models for cultural inheritance. I. Group mean and within variation. Theoretical Population Biology, 4 (1), 42–55.

Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach . Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Chamberlain, A. T. (2006). Demography in archaeology (Cambridge manuals in archaeology). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Chamberlain, C. P., Waldbauer, J. R., Fox-Dobbs, K., Newsome, S. D., Koch, P. L., Smith, D. R., et al. (2005). Pleistocene to recent dietary shifts in California condors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102 (46), 16707–16711. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0508529102

Childe, V. G. (1936). Man Makes Himself . London: Watts.

Codding, B. F., Bird, R. B., Kauhanen, P. G., & Bird, D. W. (2014). Conservation or co-evolution? Intermediate levels of aboriginal burning and hunting have positive effects on kangaroo populations in Western Australia. Human Ecology, 42 (5), 659–669. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10745-014-9682-4

Coles, B. J. (2006). Beavers in Britain’s past (WARP occasional paper 19) . Oxford: Oxbow.

Collard, M., Tarle, L., Sandgathe, D., & Allan, A. (2016). Faunal evidence for a difference in clothing use between Neanderthals and early modern humans in Europe. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 44 (Part B), 235–246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaa.2016.07.010

Corenblit, D., Gurnell, A. M., Steiger, J., & Tabacchi, E. (2008). Reciprocal adjustments between landforms and living organisms: Extended geomorphic evolutionary insights. Catena, 73 (3), 261–273. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.catena.2007.11.002

Corenblit, D., Steiger, J., Gurnell, A. M., & Tabacchi, E. (2007). Darwinian origin of landforms. Earth Surface Processes and Landforms, 32 (13), 2070–2073. https://doi.org/10.1002/esp.1536

Coward, F., Shennan, S. J., Colledge, S., Conolly, J., & Collard, M. (2008). The spread of neolithic plant economies from the near east to Northwest Europe: A phylogenetic analysis. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35 (1), 42–56.

Crutzen, P. J., & Stoermer, E. F. (2000). The Anthropocene. Global Change Newsletter, 41 , 17–18.

Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2011). Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366 (1567), 1149–1157. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0319

d’Alpoim Guedes, J. A., Crabtree, S. A., Bocinsky, R. K., & Kohler, T. A. (2016). Twenty-first century approaches to ancient problems: Climate and society. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113 (51), 14483–14491. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1616188113

Daniau, A.-L., d’Errico, F., & Sánchez-Goñi, M. F. (2010). Testing the hypothesis of fire use for ecosystem management by Neanderthal and upper palaeolithic modern human populations. PLoS One, 5 (2), e9157. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0009157

Dawkins, R. (1982). The extended phenotype. The long reach of the gene . Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Dincauze, D. F. (1993). The oops factor: Unintended consequences in biocultural evolution. In R. W. Jamieson, S. Abonyi, & N. A. Mirau (Eds.), Culture and environment: A fragile coexistence (pp. 3–10). Calgary: The University of Calgary Archaeological Association.

Dincauze, D. F. (2000). Environmental archaeology: Principles and practice . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ellis, E. C. (2015). Ecology in an anthropogenic biosphere. Ecological Monographs, 85 (3), 287–331. https://doi.org/10.1890/14-2274.1

Ellis, E. C., Richerson, P. J., Mesoudi, A., Svenning, J.-C., Odling-Smee, J., & Burnside, W. R. (2016). Evolving the human niche. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113 (31), E4436–E4436.

Erwin, D. H. (2008). Macroevolution of ecosystem engineering, niche construction and diversity. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 23 (6), 304–310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2008.01.013

Feldman, M. W., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1975). Models for cultural inheritance. A general linear model. Annals of Human Biology, 2 , 215–226.

Feldman, M. W., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1976). Cultural and biological evolutionary processes, selection for a trait under complex transmission. Theoretical Population Biology, 9 (2), 238–259.

Feldman, M. W., Odling-Smee, J. F., & Laland, K. N. (2017). Why Gupta et al.’s critique of niche construction theory is off target. Journal of Genetics, 96 (3), 505–508. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12041-017-0797-4

Fiedel, S., & Haynes, G. (2004). A premature burial: Comments on Grayson and Meltzer’s “requiem for overkill”. Journal of Archaeological Science, 31 (1), 121–131. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jas.2003.06.004

Finney, S. C., & Edwards, L. E. (2016). The “Anthropocene” epoch: Scientific decision or political statement? GSA Today, 26 (3), 4–10. https://doi.org/10.1130/GSATG270A.1

Fischer, A., Olsen, J., Richards, M., Heinemeier, J., Sveinbjörnsdóttir, Á. E., & Bennike, P. (2007). Coast-inland mobility and diet in the Danish Mesolithic and Neolithic: Evidence from stable isotope values of humans and dogs. Journal of Archaeological Science, 34 (12), 2125–2150.

Flynn, E. G., Laland, K. N., Kendal, R. L., & Kendal, J. R. (2013). Developmental niche construction. Developmental Science, 16 (2), 296–313. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12030

Foley, S. F., Gronenborn, D., Andreae, M. O., Kadereit, J. W., Esper, J., Scholz, D., et al. (2013). The Palaeoanthropocene – The beginnings of anthropogenic environmental change. Anthropocene, 3 , 83–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ancene.2013.11.002

Fox, T., Pope, M., & Ellis, E. C. (2017). Engineering the Anthropocene: Scalable social networks and resilience building in human evolutionary timescales. The Anthropocene Review, 4 (3), 199–215. https://doi.org/10.1177/2053019617742415

Fracchia, J., & Lewontin, R. C. (2005). The price of metaphor. History and Theory, 44 (1), 14–29.

Franklin, J., Potts, A. J., Fisher, E. C., Cowling, R. M., & Marean, C. W. (2015). Paleodistribution modeling in archaeology and paleoanthropology. Quaternary Science Reviews, 110 , 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quascirev.2014.12.015

Freckleton, R. P., Harvey, P. H., & Pagel, M. (2002). Phylogenetic analysis and comparative data: A test and review of evidence. American Naturalist, 160 (6), 712–726. https://doi.org/10.1086/343873

Fuentes, A. (2009). A new synthesis: Resituating approaches to the evolution of human behaviour. Anthropology Today, 25 (3), 12–17.

Fuentes, A. (2016). The extended evolutionary synthesis, ethnography, and the human niche: Toward an integrated anthropology. Current Anthropology, 57 (S13), S13–S26. https://doi.org/10.1086/685684

Futuyma, D. J. (2017). Evolutionary biology today and the call for an extended synthesis. Interface Focus, 7 (5), 20160145. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsfs.2016.0145

Gärdenfors, P., & Högberg, A. (2015). Evolutionary mechanisms of teaching. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38 (e31), 41. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1400048X

Germonpré, M., Lázničková-Galetová, M., & Sablin, M. V. (2012). Palaeolithic dog skulls at the Gravettian Předmostí site, the Czech Republic. Journal of Archaeological Science, 39 (1), 184–202.

Germonpré, M., Sablin, M. V., Stevens, R. E., Hedges, R. E. M., Hofreiter, M., Stiller, M., et al. (2009). Fossil dogs and wolves from Palaeolithic sites in Belgium, the Ukraine and Russia: Osteometry, ancient DNA and stable isotopes. Journal of Archaeological Science, 36 (2), 473–490. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jas.2008.09.033

Gilligan, I. (2010). The prehistoric development of clothing: Archaeological implications of a thermal model. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 17 (1), 15–80.

Glikson, A. (2013). Fire and human evolution: The deep-time blueprints of the Anthropocene. Anthropocene, 3 , 89–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ancene.2014.02.002

Gowlett, J. A. J. (2016). The discovery of fire by humans: A long and convoluted process. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371 (1696), 20150164. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0164

Gowlett, J. A. J., & Wrangham, R. W. (2013). Earliest fire in Africa: Towards the convergence of archaeological evidence and the cooking hypothesis. Azania: Archaeological Research in Africa, 48 (1), 5–30. https://doi.org/10.1080/0067270X.2012.756754

Gray, R. D., Bryant, D., & Greenhill, S. J. (2010). On the shape and fabric of human history. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 365 (1559), 3923–3933. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0162

Grayson, D. K., & Meltzer, D. J. (2003). A requiem for North American overkill. Journal of Archaeological Science, 30 (5), 585–593.

Grayson, D. K., & Meltzer, D. J. (2004). North American overkill continued? Journal of Archaeological Science, 31 (1), 133–136.

Gremillion, K. J., Barton, L., & Piperno, D. R. (2014). Particularism and the retreat from theory in the archaeology of agricultural origins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111 (17), 6171–6177.

Grote, K. (Ed.). (1994). Die Abris im südlichen Leinebergland bei Göttingen. Archäologische Befunde zum Leben unter Felsschutzdächern in urgeschichtlicher Zeit . Oldenburg: Isensee Verlag.

Guagnin, M., Perri, A. R., & Petraglia, M. D. (2018). Pre-Neolithic evidence for dog-assisted hunting strategies in Arabia. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 49 , 225–236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaa.2017.10.003

Guisan, A., Thuiller, W., & Zimmermann, N. E. (2017). Habitat suitability and distribution models: With applications in R (Ecology, biodiversity and conservation). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Gupta, M., Prasad, N. G., Dey, S., Joshi, A., & Vidya, T. N. C. (2017a). Feldman et al. do protest too much, we think. Journal of Genetics, 96 (3), 509–511. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12041-017-0796-5

Gupta, M., Prasad, N. G., Dey, S., Joshi, A., & Vidya, T. N. C. (2017b). Niche construction in evolutionary theory: The construction of an academic niche? Journal of Genetics, 96 (3), 491–504. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12041-017-0787-6

Hill, R., Baird, A., & Buchanan, D. (1999). Aborigines and fire in the wet tropics of Queensland, Australia: Ecosystem management across cultures. Society & Natural Resources, 12 (3), 205–223. https://doi.org/10.1080/089419299279704

Hosfield, R. T. (2016). Walking in a winter wonderland? Strategies for early and middle pleistocene survival in midlatitude Europe. Current Anthropology, 57 (5), 653–682. https://doi.org/10.1086/688579

Inglis, J. T. (Ed.). (1993). Traditional ecological knowledge. Concepts and cases . Ottawa: Internatioanl Program on Traditional Ecological Knowledge/IDRC.

Innes, J. B., Blackford, J. J., & Rowley-Conwy, P. A. (2013). Late Mesolithic and early Neolithic forest disturbance: A high resolution palaeoecological test of human impact hypotheses. Quaternary Science Reviews, 77 , 80–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quascirev.2013.07.012

Iriki, A., & Sakura, O. (2008). The neuroscience of primate intellectual evolution: Natural selection and passive and intentional niche construction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363 (1500), 2229–2241. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.2274

Iriki, A., & Taoka, M. (2012). Triadic (ecological, neural, cognitive) niche construction: A scenario of human brain evolution extrapolating tool use and language from the control of reaching actions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367 (1585), 10–23.

Jessen, M. D. (2012). Material culture and the construction of religious niches. In R. Berge, M. E. Jasinski, & K. Sognnes (Eds.), N-TAG TEN. Proceedings of the 10th Nordic TAG conference at Stiklestad, Norway 2009 (BAR (international series) 2399) (pp. 79–85). Oxford: Archaeopress.

Johannsen, N. N. (2007). Animal machines and Funnel Beaker economies: Exploring the use of draught cattle technology. In F. Both (Ed.), Experimentelle Archäologie in Europa (pp. 57–75). Oldenburg: Isensee Verlag.

Johannsen, N. N. (2010). Technological conceptualization: Cognition on the shoulders of history. In L. Malafouris & C. Renfrew (Eds.), The cognitive life of things (pp. 59–69). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

Johnson, C. N., Bradshaw, C. J. A., Cooper, A., Gillespie, R., & Brook, B. W. (2013). Rapid megafaunal extinction following human arrival throughout the New World. Quaternary International, 308–309 (Supplement C), 273–277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2013.06.022

Jones, J. R., & Mulville, J. (2016). Isotopic and zooarchaeological approaches towards understanding aquatic resource use in human economies and animal management in the prehistoric Scottish North Atlantic Islands. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports, 6 , 665–677. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jasrep.2015.08.019

Jones, J. R., & Mulville, J. A. (2018). Norse animal husbandry in liminal environments: Stable isotope evidence from the Scottish North Atlantic Islands. Environmental Archaeology , 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1080/14614103.2018.1439678

Jöris, O., & Terberger, T. (2001). Zur Rekonstruktion eines Zeltes mit trapezförmigem Grundriß der Magdalénien-Station Gönnersdorf/ Mittelrhein - eine “Quadratur des Kreises”? Archäologisches Korrespondenzblatt, 31 , 163–172.

Kass, J. M., Vilela, B., Aiello-Lammens, M. E., Muscarella, R., Merow, C., & Anderson, R. P. (2018). Wallace: A flexible platform for reproducible modeling of species niches and distributions built for community expansion. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 9 (4), 1151–1156. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12945

Kennett, D. J., Anderson, A., & Winterhalder, B. (2006). The ideal free distribution, food production, and the colonization of Oceania. In D. J. Kennett & B. Winterhalder (Eds.), Behavioral ecology and the transition to agriculture (pp. 265–288). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Kerr, B. (2007). Niche construction and cognitive evolution. Biological Theory, 2 (3), 250–262. https://doi.org/10.1162/biot.2007.2.3.250

Kerr, B., & Feldman, M. W. (2003). Carving the cognitive niche: Optimal learning strategies in homogeneous and heterogeneous environments. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 220 (2), 169–188. https://doi.org/10.1006/jtbi.2003.3146

Kirch, P. V. (1997). The Lapita peoples. Ancestors of the oceanic world . Oxford: Blackwell.

Kittler, R., Kayser, M., & Stoneking, M. (2003). Molecular evolution of Pediculus humanus and the origin of clothing. Current Biology, 13 , 1414–1417.

Kline, M. A. (2015). How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38 (e31), 1–71. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14000090

Kline, M. A., Boyd, R., & Henrich, J. (2013). Teaching and the life history of cultural transmission in Fijian villages. Human Nature, 24 (4), 351–374. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9180-1

Kluiving, S. J. (2015). How geoarchaeology and landscape archaeology contribute to niche construction theory (NCT). Water History, 7 (4), 557–571. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12685-015-0144-8

Koster, J. M. (2008). Hunting with dogs in Nicaragua: An optimal foraging approach. Current Anthropology, 49 (5), 935–944. https://doi.org/10.1086/592021

Laland, K. N., Boogert, N., & Evans, C. (2014). Niche construction, innovation and complexity. Environmental Innovation and Societal Transitions, 11 , 71–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eist.2013.08.003

Laland, K. N., & Boogert, N. J. (2010). Niche construction, co-evolution and biodiversity. Ecological Economics, 69 (4), 731–736.

Laland, K. N., & Brown, G. R. (2006). Niche construction, human behavior, and the adaptive-lag hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 15 , 95–104. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20093

Laland, K. N., & Brown, G. R. (2011). Sense & nonsense. Evolutionary perspectives on human behaviour . Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Laland, K. N., & O’Brien, M. J. (2010). Niche construction theory and archaeology. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 17 (4), 303–322. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-010-9096-6

Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, F. J., & Gilbert, S. F. (2008). EvoDevo and niche construction: Building bridges. Journal of Experimental Zoology Part B-Molecular and Developmental Evolution, 310B (7), 549–566. https://doi.org/10.1002/jez.b.21232

Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J. F., & Feldman, M. W. (1999). Evolutionary consequences of niche construction and their implications for ecology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96 , 10242–10247.

Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J. F., & Feldman, M. W. (2000). Niche construction earns its keep. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23 (1), 164–175.

Laland, K. N., & Sterelny, K. (2006). Perspective: Seven reasons (not) to neglect niche construction. Evolution, 60 (9), 1751–1762.

Laland, K. N., Uller, T., Feldman, M. W., Sterelny, K., Müller, G. B., Moczek, A., et al. (2015). The extended evolutionary synthesis: Its structure, assumptions and predictions. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 282 (1813), 20151019. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.1019

Langley, M. C. (2018). Magdalenian children: Projectile points, portable art and playthings. Oxford Journal of Archaeology, 37 (1), 3–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/ojoa.12128

Larson, G., Karlsson, E. K., Perri, A., Webster, M. T., Ho, S. Y. W., Peters, J., et al. (2012). Rethinking dog domestication by integrating genetics, archeology, and biogeography. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109 (23), 8878–8883. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1203005109

Lewis, S. L., & Maslin, M. A. (2015). Defining the Anthropocene. [perspectives]. Nature, 519 (7542), 171–180. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature14258

Lewontin, R. C. (1970). The units of selection. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 1 , 1–14.

Lewontin, R. C. (1978). Adaptation. Scientific American, 239 , 156–169.

Lewontin, R. C. (1983). Gene, organism and environment. In D. S. Bendall (Ed.), Evolution from molecules to men (pp. 273–286). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Lima-Ribeiro, M. S., & Felizola Diniz-Filho, J. A. (2013). American megafaunal extinctions and human arrival: Improved evaluation using a meta-analytical approach. Quaternary International, 299 (Supplement C), 38–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2013.03.007

Lupo, K. D. (2017). When and where do dogs improve hunting productivity? The empirical record and some implications for early Upper Paleolithic prey acquisition. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 47 (Supplement C), 139–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaa.2017.05.003

Mace, R., & Pagel, M. D. (1994). The comparative method in anthropology. Current Anthropology, 35 (4), 549–564.

Malhi, Y. (2017). The concept of the Anthropocene. Annual Review of Environment and Resources, 42 (1), 77–104. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-environ-102016-060854

Malhi, Y., Doughty, C. E., Galetti, M., Smith, F. A., Svenning, J.-C., & Terborgh, J. W. (2016). Megafauna and ecosystem function from the Pleistocene to the Anthropocene. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113 (4), 838–846. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1502540113

Maring, R., & Riede, F. (2019). Possible wild boar management during the Ertebølle period. A carbon and nitrogen isotope analysis of Mesolithic wild boar from Fannerup F, Denmark. Environmental Archaeology, 24 (1), 15–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/14614103.2018.1516328

Marwick, B. (2017). Computational reproducibility in archaeological research: Basic principles and a case study of their implementation. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 24 (2), 424–450. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-015-9272-9

Marwick, B., d’Alpoim Guedes, J., Barton, C. M., Bates, L. A., Baxter, M., Bevan, A., et al. (2017). Open Science in archaeology. The SAA Archaeological Record, 17 (4), 8–14.

Matthews, B., De Meester, L., Jones, C. G., Ibelings, B. W., Bouma, T. J., Nuutinen, V., et al. (2014). Under niche construction: An operational bridge between ecology, evolution, and ecosystem science. Ecological Monographs, 84 (2), 245–263. https://doi.org/10.1890/13-0953.1

Mellars, P. A. (1976). Fire ecology, animal populations and man: A study of some ecological relationships in prehistory. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society, 42 , 15–42.

Mesoudi, A., Blanchet, S., Charmantier, A., Danchin, É., Fogarty, L., Jablonka, E., et al. (2013). Is non-genetic inheritance just a proximate mechanism? A corroboration of the extended evolutionary synthesis. Biological Theory, 7 (3), 189–195. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-013-0091-5

Mithen, S. J. (1991). Ecological interpretations of Palaeolithic art. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society, 57 (1), 103–114.

Mohlenhoff, K. A., & Codding, B. F. (2017). When does it pay to invest in a patch? The evolution of intentional niche construction. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 26 (5), 218–227. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21534

Moore, J. (2000). Forest fire and human interaction in the early Holocene woodlands of Britain. Palaeogeography, Palaeoclimatology, Palaeoecology, 164 , 125–137. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-0182(00)00180-2

Morgan, L. H. (1868). The American beaver and his works . Philadelphia, PA: J.B. Lippincott.

Müller, G. B. (2017). Why an extended evolutionary synthesis is necessary. Interface Focus, 7 (5), 20170015. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsfs.2017.0015

Musil, R. (2000). Evidence for the domestication of wolves in central European Magdalenian sites. In S. J. Crockford (Ed.), Dogs through time: An archaeological perspective (BAR (international series) 889) (pp. 21–28). Oxford: Archaeopress.

Naiman, R. J., Melillo, J. M., & Hobbie, J. E. (1986). Ecosystem alteration of boreal forest streams by beaver ( Castor Canadensis ). Ecology, 67 (5), 1254–1269.

Napierala, H., & Uerpmann, H.-P. (2012). A ‘new’ palaeolithic dog from Central Europe. International Journal of Osteoarchaeology, 22 (2), 127–137. https://doi.org/10.1002/oa.1182

Nolet, B. A., & Rosell, F. (1998). Comeback of the beaver Castor fiber : An overview of old and new conservation problems. Biological Conservation, 83 (2), 165–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3207(97)00066-9

Normand, S., Høye, T. T., Forbes, B. C., Bowden, J. J., Davies, A. L., Odgaard, B. V., et al. (2017). Legacies of historical human activities in Arctic woody plant dynamics. Annual Review of Environment and Resources, 42 (1), 541–567. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-environ-110615-085454

Nowell, A. (2015). Learning to see and seeing to learn: Children, communities of practice and Pleistocene visual cultures. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 25 (04), 889–899. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774315000360

O’Brien, M. J., & Bentley, R. A. (2015). The role of food storage in human niche construction: An example from Neolithic Europe. Environmental Archaeology, 20 (4), 364–378. https://doi.org/10.1179/1749631414Y.0000000053

O’Brien, M. J., & Holland, T. D. (1992). The role of adaptation in archaeological explanation. American Antiquity, 57 (1), 36–59.

O’Brien, M. J., & Holland, T. D. (1995). Behavioural archaeology and the extended phenotype. In J. M. Skibo, W. H. Walker, & A. E. Nielsen (Eds.), Expanding archaeology (pp. 143–161). Salt Lake City, UT: University of Utah Press.

O’Brien, M. J., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Genes, culture, and agriculture: An example of human niche construction. Current Anthropology, 53 (4), 434–470. https://doi.org/10.1086/666585

O’Brien, M. J., Lyman, R. L., Collard, M., Holden, C. J., Gray, R. D., & Shennan, S. J. (2008). Transmission, phylogenetics, and the evolution of cultural diversity. In M. J. O’Brien (Ed.), Cultural transmission and archaeology: Issues and case studies (pp. 39–58). Washington, DC: Society for American Archaeology Press.

O’Connor, T. P. (1998). Environmental archaeology: A matter of definition. Environmental Archaeology, 2 , 1–6.

Odgaard, U. (2007). On the trail of the Caribou hunters: Archaeological surveys in Western Greenland. In R. Johnson & V. Cummings (Eds.), Prehistoric journeys (pp. 21–32). Oxford: Oxbow.

Odling-Smee, J. F. (1988). Niche constructing phenotypes. In H. C. Plotkin (Ed.), The Role of behavior in evolution . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Odling-Smee, J. F. (1995). Niche construction, genetic evolution and cultural change. Behavioural Processes, 35 (1–3), 195–205. https://doi.org/10.1016/0376-6357(95)00055-0

Odling-Smee, J. F. (2007). Niche inheritance: A possible basis for classifying multiple inheritance systems in evolution. Biological Theory, 2 (3), 276–289. https://doi.org/10.1162/biot.2007.2.3.276

Odling-Smee, J. F., Erwin, D. H., Palkovacs, E. P., Feldman, M. W., & Laland, K. N. (2013). Niche construction theory: A practical guide for ecologists. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 88 (1), 3–28. https://doi.org/10.1086/669266

Odling-Smee, J. F., Laland, K. N., & Feldman, M. W. (2003). Niche construction. The neglected process in evolution . Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Olsson, F., Gaillard, M.-J., Lemdahl, G., Greisman, A., Lanos, P., Marguerie, D., et al. (2010). A continuous record of fire covering the last 10,500 calendar years from southern Sweden -- the role of climate and human activities. Palaeogeography, Palaeoclimatology, Palaeoecology, 291 (1–2), 128–141.

Oswalt, W. H. (1976). An anthropological analysis of food-getting technology . New York: Wiley.

Ovodov, N. D., Crockford, S. J., Kuzmin, Y. V., Higham, T. F. G., Hodgins, G. W. L., & van der Plicht, J. (2011). A 33,000-year-old incipient dog from the Altai Mountains of Siberia: Evidence of the earliest domestication disrupted by the last glacial maximum. PLoS One, 6 (7), e22821. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022821

Pang, J.-F., Kluetsch, C., Zou, X.-J., Zhang, A.-B., Luo, L.-Y., Angleby, H., et al. (2009). mtDNA data indicate a single origin for dogs south of Yangtze River, less than 16,300 years ago, from numerous wolves. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 26 (12), 2849–2864. https://doi.org/10.1093/molbev/msp195

Parker, C. H., Keefe, E. R., Herzog, N. M., O’Connell, J. F., & Hawkes, K. (2016). The pyrophilic primate hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 25 (2), 54–63. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21475

Pasda, C. (2004). Hotel Grønland: Human use of caves and rock shelters in West Greenland (BAR (IS) 1309). Oxford: Archaeopress.

Pionnier-Capitan, M., Bemilli, C., Bodu, P., Cèlérier, G., FerriÈ, J.-G., Fosse, P., et al. (2011). New evidence for Upper Palaeolithic small domestic dogs in South-Western Europe. Journal of Archaeological Science, 38 (9), 2123–2140.

Potts, R. (1994). Variables versus models of early Pleistocene hominid land use. Journal of Human Evolution, 27 (1–3), 7–24. https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1994.1033

Puttock, A., Graham, H. A., Cunliffe, A. M., Elliott, M., & Brazier, R. E. (2017). Eurasian beaver activity increases water storage, attenuates flow and mitigates diffuse pollution from intensively-managed grasslands. Science of the Total Environment, 576 (Supplement C), 430–443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2016.10.122

Pyne, S. J. (1991). Burning bush: A fire history of Australia . New York: Henry Holt.

Riede, F. (2005a). Darwin vs. Bourdieu. Celebrity deathmatch or postprocessual myth? Prolegomenon for the reconciliation of agentive-interpretative and ecological-evolutionary archaeology. In H. Cobb, S. Price, F. Coward, & L. Grimshaw (Eds.), Investigating prehistoric hunter-gatherer identities: Case studies from Palaeolithic and Mesolithic Europe (BAR (IS)) (Vol. 1411, pp. 45–64). Oxford: Oxbow.

Riede, F. (2005b). To boldly go where no (Hu-)man has gone before. Some thoughts on the Pioneer colonisations of pristine landscapes. Archaeological Review from Cambridge, 20 (1), 20–38.

Riede, F. (2007). ‘Stretched thin, like butter on too much bread…’: Some thoughts about journeying in the unfamiliar landscapes of late Palaeolithic Southern Scandinavia. In R. Johnson & V. Cummings (Eds.), Prehistoric journeys (pp. 8–20). Oxford: Oxbow.

Riede, F. (2009a). Climate and demography in early prehistory: Using calibrated 14 C dates as population proxies. Human Biology, 81 (2–3), 309–337. https://doi.org/10.3378/027.081.0311

Riede, F. (2009b). Climate change, demography and social relations: An alternative view of the Late Palaeolithic pioneer colonization of Southern Scandinavia. In S. McCartan, P. C. Woodman, R. J. Schulting, & G. Warren (Eds.), Mesolithic horizons. Papers presented at the seventh international conference on the Mesolithic in Europe, Belfast 2005 (Vol. 1, pp. 3–10). Oxford: Oxbow.

Riede, F. (2010). Niche construction theory and human prehistory. Using artefact phylogenies and comparative methods to study past human ecosystem engineering. In J. L. Escacena Carrasco, D. García Rivero, & F. J. García Fernández (Eds.), Clasificación y Arqueología: Enfoques y métodos taxonómicos a la luz de la evolución darwiniana (pp. 175–204). Seville: University of Seville Press.

Riede, F. (2011). Adaptation and niche construction in human prehistory: A case study from the southern Scandinavian Late Glacial. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366 (1566), 793–808. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0266

Riede, F. (2012). Theory for the A-theoretical: Niche construction theory and its implications for environmental archaeology. In R. Berge, M. E. Jasinski, & K. Sognnes (Eds.), N-TAG TEN. Proceedings of the 10th Nordic TAG conference at Stiklestad, Norway 2009 (BAR (international series) 2399) (pp. 87–98). Oxford: Archaeopress.

Riede, F. (2014). Success and failure during the Late Glacial pioneer human re-colonisation of southern Scandinavia. In F. Riede & M. Tallavaara (Eds.), Lateglacial and postglacial pioneers in northern Europe (British archaeological reports (international series) 2599) (Vol. 2599, pp. 33–52). Oxford: Archaeopress.

Riede, F., Johannsen, N. N., Högberg, A., Nowell, A., & Lombard, M. (2018). The role of play objects and object play in human cognitive evolution and innovation. Evolutionary Anthropology, 27 (1), 46–59. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21555

Riede, F., & Pedersen, J. B. (2018). Late glacial human dispersals in northern Europe and disequilibrium dynamics. Human Ecology, 46 (5), 621–632. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10745-017-9964-8

Riel-Salvatore, J. (2010). A niche construction perspective on the Middle–Upper Paleolithic transition in Italy. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 17 (4), 323–355. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-010-9093-9

Riel-Salvatore, J., & Negrino, F. (2018). Proto-Aurignacian lithic technology, mobility, and human niche construction: A case study from Riparo Bombrini, Italy. In E. Robinson & F. Sellet (Eds.), Lithic technological organization and paleoenvironmental change: Global and diachronic perspectives (pp. 163–187). Cham: Springer.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Rindos, D. (1984). The origins of agriculture: An evolutionary perspective . London: Academic.

Rockman, M. (2009). Landscape learning in relation to evolutionary theory. In A. M. Prentiss, I. Kuijt, & J. C. Chatters (Eds.), Macroevolution in human prehistory (pp. 51–71). New York: Springer.

Rockman, M., & Steele, J. (Eds.). (2003). Colonization of unfamiliar landscapes: The archaeology of adaptation . London: Routledge.

Rowley-Conwy, P., & Layton, R. (2011). Foraging and farming as niche construction: Stable and unstable adaptations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366 (1566), 849–862. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0307

Ruddiman, W. F. (2013). The Anthropocene. Annual Review of Earth and Planetary Sciences, 41 (1), 45–68. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-earth-050212-123944

Sandom, C., Dalby, L., Fløjgaard, C., Kissling, W. D., Lenoir, J., Sandel, B., et al. (2013). Mammal predator and prey species richness are strongly linked at macroscales. Ecology, 94 (5), 1112–1122. https://doi.org/10.1890/12-1342.1

Sandom, C., Faurby, S., Sandel, B., & Svenning, J.-C. (2014). Global late quaternary megafauna extinctions linked to humans, not climate change. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281 (1787). https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.3254

Savolainen, P., Leitner, T., Wilton, A. N., Matisoo-Smith, E., & Lundeberg, J. (2004). A detailed picture of the origin of the Australian dingo, obtained from the study of mitochondrial DNA. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101 (33), 12387–12390.

Savolainen, P., Zhang, Y. P., Luo, J., Lundeberg, J., & Leitner, T. (2002). Genetic evidence for an East Asian origin of domestic dogs. Science, 298 (5598), 1610–1613.

Schwilk, D. W. (2003). Flammability is a niche construction trait: Canopy architecture affects fire intensity. American Naturalist, 162 (6), 725–733. https://doi.org/10.1086/379351

Scott-Phillips, T. C., Laland, K. N., Shuker, D. M., Dickins, T. E., & West, S. A. (2014). The niche construction perspective: A critical appraisal. Evolution, 68 (5), 1231–1243. https://doi.org/10.1111/evo.12332

Shennan, S. J. (2004). An evolutionary perspective on agency in archaeology. In A. Gardner (Ed.), Agency uncovered: Archaeological perspectives on social agency, power, and being human (pp. 19–32). London: UCL Press.

Shennan, S. J. (2006). From cultural history to cultural evolution: An archaeological perspective on social information transmission. In J. C. K. Wells, S. Strickland, & K. N. Laland (Eds.), Social information transmission and human biology (pp. 173–190). London: CRC Press.

Shimelmitz, R., Kuhn, S. L., Jelinek, A. J., Ronen, A., Clark, A. E., & Weinstein-Evron, M. (2014). ‘Fire at will’: The emergence of habitual fire use 350,000 years ago. Journal of Human Evolution, 77 , 196–203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2014.07.005

Smith, B. D. (2007). Niche construction and the behavioral context of plant and animal domestication. Evolutionary Anthropology, 16 (5), 188–199. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20135

Smith, B. D., & Zeder, M. A. (2013). The onset of the Anthropocene. Anthropocene, 4 , 8–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ancene.2013.05.001

Smith, E. A. (2013). Agency and adaptation: New directions in evolutionary anthropology. Annual Review of Anthropology, 42 (1), 103–120. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-anthro-092412-155447

Sterelny, K. (2007). Social intelligence, human intelligence and niche construction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362 (1480), 719–730. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.2006

Sterelny, K. (2011). From hominins to humans: How sapiens became behaviourally modern. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366 (1566), 809–822. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0301

Sterelny, K., & Watkins, T. (2015). Neolithization in Southwest Asia in a context of niche construction theory. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 25 (3), 673–691. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774314000675

Stiner, M. C., & Kuhn, S. L. (2016). Are we missing the “sweet spot” between optimality theory and niche construction theory in archaeology? Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 44 (Part B), 177–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaa.2016.07.006

Street, M. (2002). Ein Wiedersehen mit dem Hund von Bonn-Oberkassel. Bonner Zoologische Beiträge, 50 (3), 269–290.

Sugiyama, M. S., & Sugiyama, L. (2009). A frugal (re)past: Use of Oral tradition to buffer foraging risk. Studies in the Literary Imagination, 42 (2), 15–41.

Svenning, J.-C., Fløjgaard, C., Marske, K. A., Nógues-Bravo, D., & Normand, S. (2011). Applications of species distribution modeling to paleobiology. Quaternary Science Reviews, 30 (21–22), 2930–2947. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quascirev.2011.06.012

Swanson, H. A. (2016). Anthropocene as political geology: Current debates over how to tell time. Science as Culture, 25 (1), 157–163. https://doi.org/10.1080/09505431.2015.1074465

Szabó, P. (2010). Why history matters in ecology: An interdisciplinary perspective. Environmental Conservation, 37 (4), 380–387. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0376892910000718

Terrell, J. E., Hart, J. P., Barut, S., Cellinese, N., Curet, A., Denham, T., et al. (2003). Domesticated landscapes: The subsistence ecology of plant and animal domestication. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 10 (4), 323–368. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JARM.0000005510.54214.57

Thalmann, O., Shapiro, B., Cui, P., Schuenemann, V. J., Sawyer, S. K., Greenfield, D. L., et al. (2013). Complete mitochondrial genomes of ancient canids suggest a European origin of domestic dogs. Science, 342 (6160), 871–874. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1243650

Tolksdorf, J. F., Turner, F., Veil, S., Bittmann, F., & Breest, K. (2017). Beaver ( Castor fiber ) activity in an archaeological Context: A Mid-Holocene Beaver Burrow Feature and a Late-Holocene Ecofact at the Late Palaeolithic Grabow Site, Northern Germany. Journal of Wetland Archaeology, 17 (1), 36–50. https://doi.org/10.1080/14732971.2017.1371432

Turner, J. S. (2000). The extended organism: The physiology of animal-built structures . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Uller, T., & Helanterä, H. (2017). Niche construction and conceptual change in evolutionary biology. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science , axx050, https://doi.org/10.1093/bjps/axx050 .

VanPool, T. L., & VanPool, C. S. (2003). Agency and evolution: The role of intended and unintended consequences of action. In T. L. VanPool & C. S. VanPool (Eds.), Essential tensions in archaeological method and theory (Foundation of archaeological inquiry) (pp. 89–114). Salt Lake City, UT: University of Utah Press.

Verdú, M., Pausas, J. G., Segarra-Moragues, J. G., & Ojeda, F. (2007). Burning phylogenies: Fire, molecular evolutionary rates, and diversification. Evolution, 61 (9), 2195–2204.

Vila, C., Savolainen, P., Maldonado, J. E., Amorim, I. R., Rice, J. E., Honeycutt, R. L., et al. (1997). Multiple and ancient origins of the domestic dog. Science, 276 (5319), 1687–1689. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.276.5319.1687

Walker, M., Gibbard, P., & Lowe, J. (2015). Comment on “when did the Anthropocene begin? A mid-twentieth century boundary is stratigraphically optimal” by Jan Zalasiewicz et al. (2015), Quaternary International, 383, 196–203. Quaternary International, 383 , 204–207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2015.04.007

Waters, C. N., Zalasiewicz, J., Summerhayes, C., Barnosky, A. D., Poirier, C., Gałuszka, A., et al. (2016). The Anthropocene is functionally and stratigraphically distinct from the Holocene. Science, 351 (6269), aad2622. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aad2622

Watkins, T. (2017). From Pleistocene to Holocene: The prehistory of Southwest Asia in evolutionary context. History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences, 39 (3), 22. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40656-017-0152-3

White, L. A. (1959). The evolution of culture . New York: McGraw-Hill.

Whitford, B. R. (2018). Characterizing the cultural evolutionary process from eco-cultural niche models: Niche construction during the Neolithic of the Struma River Valley (c. 6200–4900 BC). Archaeological and Anthropological Sciences . https://doi.org/10.1007/s12520-018-0667-x .

Wiessner, P. W. (2014). Embers of society: Firelight talk among the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111 (39), 14027–14035. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1404212111

Williams, G. C. (1992). Gaia, nature worship and biocentric fallacies. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 67 (4), 479–486.

Wrangham, R. W. (2009). Catching fire: How cooking made us human . New York: Basic Books.

Wright, J. P., Jones, C. G., & Flecker, A. S. (2002). An ecosystem engineer, the beaver, increases species richness at the landscape scale. Oecologia, 132 (1), 96–101. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-002-0929-1

Zalasiewicz, J., Waters, C. N., Summerhayes, C. P., Wolfe, A. P., Barnosky, A. D., Cearreta, A., et al. (2017). The working group on the Anthropocene: Summary of evidence and interim recommendations. Anthropocene, 19 (Supplement C), 55–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ancene.2017.09.001

Zalasiewicz, J., Waters, C. N., Williams, M., Barnosky, A. D., Cearreta, A., Crutzen, P., et al. (2015). When did the Anthropocene begin? A mid-twentieth century boundary level is stratigraphically optimal. Quaternary International, 383 , 196–203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2014.11.045

Zeanah, D. W. (2017). Foraging models, niche construction, and the eastern agricultural complex. American Antiquity, 82 (1), 3–24. https://doi.org/10.1017/aaq.2016.30

Zeder, M. A. (2006). Central questions in the domestication of plants and animals. Evolutionary Anthropology, 15 , 105–117. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20101

Zeder, M. A. (2014). Alternative to faith-based science. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111 (28), E2827. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1408209111

Zeder, M. A. (2016). Domestication as a model system for niche construction theory. Evolutionary Ecology, 30 (2), 325–348. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10682-015-9801-8

Zeder, M. A. (2017). Domestication as a model system for the extended evolutionary synthesis. Interface Focus, 7 (5), 20160133. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsfs.2016.0133

Zeder, M. A., Emshwiller, E., Smith, B. D., & Bradley, D. G. (2006). Documenting domestication: The intersection of genetics and archaeology. Trends in Genetics, 22 (3), 139–155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tig.2006.01.007

Download references

Data Sharing Statement

Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no data sets were generated or analysed during the current study.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Archaeology and Heritage Studies/Centre for Environmental Humanities, BIOCHANGE Center for Biodiversity Dynamics in a Changing World, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark

Felix Riede

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Felix Riede .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

Department of Anthropology, University of Montana, Missoula, MT, USA

Anna Marie Prentiss

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Riede, F. (2019). Niche Construction Theory and Human Biocultural Evolution. In: Prentiss, A. (eds) Handbook of Evolutionary Research in Archaeology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-11117-5_17

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-11117-5_17

Published : 04 June 2019

Publisher Name : Springer, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-030-11116-8

Online ISBN : 978-3-030-11117-5

eBook Packages : Social Sciences Social Sciences (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

human biocultural and social evolution essay

1st Edition

Human Nature and Biocultural Evolution

Description.

First published in 1984, Human Nature and Biocultural Evolution aims to delineate a theory of human nature, viewed as an interrelated set of genetically programmed behavioral predispositions, and a theory of biocultural evolution. The author’s approach is based on the hypothesis that innate predispositions and cultural-environmental factors cooperate to determine human behavior and socio-cultural forms.

Professor Lopreato begins by tracing the development of evolutionary biology up to sociobiology. It is his argument that the social and biological disciplines have, for over a century, been moving towards a synthesis, and that Homo sapiens is neither just another animal, nor so unique a being that culture has become divorced from its genetic underpinnings. The argument is supported with evidence from evolutionary biology and social science, with a critical discussion of basic issues of behavioral science and with an analysis of certain famous theories in social science (e.g. theories of suicide, anomie, capitalism), which prove to be richer and more complete when viewed from a biocultural perspective.

The theory of human nature is arrived at through a rich analysis of ethnographic, psychological, and sociological arguments and data, as well as facts and theories from comparative zoology. In the process, the author treats critically numerous theoretical problems associated with topics such as exploitation, class consciousness, structured inequality, reciprocal behavior, territorial aggression, religious ritual, socialization, ethnicity, and prejudice. The author concludes with an examination of behavioral predispositions that are hypothesized to be at the base of cultural variation.

Table of Contents

1. Introduction  2. Behavioral Predispositions, Cultural Universals, and Cultural Variants  3. The Interplay of Biology and Culture  4. Predispositions of Self-Enhancement  5. Predispositions of Sociality, I  6. Sociality, II: Ascetic Altruism  7. A Model of Sociocultural Variation and Selective Retention  8. Behavioral Predispositions and Religious Behavior  9. Evolutionary Foundations of Family and Ethnicity   

Joseph Lopreato was former chairperson of the Department of Sociology, University of Texas at Austin, USA. He published widely in areas such as sociological theory, political sociology, social development, and ethnic studies.

Critics' Reviews

Reviews of the first publication: ‘…admirably lucid and runs smoothly from sociology to biology and back again, showing it can be done. Professor Lopreato’s grasp of evolutionary biology is truly remarkable. This book has a potentially unifying role to play.’ — Professor E. O. Wilson , Harvard University ‘…useful for teaching purposes with regard to sociobiology and society, along with an excellent reference section.’ — Frank K. Salter , Griffith University ‘…it might well represent a major conceptual bridge between important aspects of social science and the expanding scientific synthesis.’ — Professor D. D. Thiessen , University of Texas at Austin

About VitalSource eBooks

VitalSource is a leading provider of eBooks.

  • Access your materials anywhere, at anytime.
  • Customer preferences like text size, font type, page color and more.
  • Take annotations in line as you read.

Multiple eBook Copies

This eBook is already in your shopping cart. If you would like to replace it with a different purchasing option please remove the current eBook option from your cart.

Book Preview

human biocultural and social evolution essay

The country you have selected will result in the following:

  • Product pricing will be adjusted to match the corresponding currency.
  • The title Perception will be removed from your cart because it is not available in this region.

Session Timeout

For security purposes, your session is about to expire due to inactivity.

Your session will end in 2:00

Introductory essay

Written by the educator who created What Makes Us Human?, a brief look at the key facts, tough questions and big ideas in his field. Begin this TED Study with a fascinating read that gives context and clarity to the material.

As a biological anthropologist, I never liked drawing sharp distinctions between human and non-human. Such boundaries make little evolutionary sense, as they ignore or grossly underestimate what we humans have in common with our ancestors and other primates. What's more, it's impossible to make sharp distinctions between human and non-human in the paleoanthropological record. Even with a time machine, we couldn't go back to identify one generation of humans and say that the previous generation contained none: one's biological parents, by definition, must be in the same species as their offspring. This notion of continuity is inherent to most evolutionary perspectives and it's reflected in the similarities (homologies) shared among very different species. As a result, I've always been more interested in what makes us similar to, not different from, non-humans.

Evolutionary research has clearly revealed that we share great biological continuity with others in the animal kingdom. Yet humans are truly unique in ways that have not only shaped our own evolution, but have altered the entire planet. Despite great continuity and similarity with our fellow primates, our biocultural evolution has produced significant, profound discontinuities in how we interact with each other and in our environment, where no precedent exists in other animals. Although we share similar underlying evolved traits with other species, we also display uses of those traits that are so novel and extraordinary that they often make us forget about our commonalities. Preparing a twig to fish for termites may seem comparable to preparing a stone to produce a sharp flake—but landing on the moon and being able to return to tell the story is truly out of this non-human world.

Humans are the sole hominin species in existence today. Thus, it's easier than it would have been in the ancient past to distinguish ourselves from our closest living relatives in the animal kingdom. Primatologists such as Jane Goodall and Frans de Waal, however, continue to clarify why the lines dividing human from non-human aren't as distinct as we might think. Goodall's classic observations of chimpanzee behaviors like tool use, warfare and even cannibalism demolished once-cherished views of what separates us from other primates. de Waal has done exceptional work illustrating some continuity in reciprocity and fairness, and in empathy and compassion, with other species. With evolution, it seems, we are always standing on the shoulders of others, our common ancestors.

Primatology—the study of living primates—is only one of several approaches that biological anthropologists use to understand what makes us human. Two others, paleoanthropology (which studies human origins through the fossil record) and molecular anthropology (which studies human origins through genetic analysis), also yield some surprising insights about our hominin relatives. For example, Zeresenay Alemsegad's painstaking field work and analysis of Selam, a 3.3 million-year old fossil of a 3-year-old australopithecine infant from Ethiopia, exemplifies how paleoanthropologists can blur boundaries between living humans and apes.

Selam, if alive today, would not be confused with a three-year-old human—but neither would we mistake her for a living ape. Selam's chimpanzee-like hyoid bone suggests a more ape-like form of vocal communication, rather than human language capability. Overall, she would look chimp-like in many respects—until she walked past you on two feet. In addition, based on Selam's brain development, Alemseged theorizes that Selam and her contemporaries experienced a human-like extended childhood with a complex social organization.

Fast-forward to the time when Neanderthals lived, about 130,000 – 30,000 years ago, and most paleoanthropologists would agree that language capacity among the Neanderthals was far more human-like than ape-like; in the Neanderthal fossil record, hyoids and other possible evidence of language can be found. Moreover, paleogeneticist Svante Pääbo's groundbreaking research in molecular anthropology strongly suggests that Neanderthals interbred with modern humans. Paabo's work informs our genetic understanding of relationships to ancient hominins in ways that one could hardly imagine not long ago—by extracting and comparing DNA from fossils comprised largely of rock in the shape of bones and teeth—and emphasizes the great biological continuity we see, not only within our own species, but with other hominins sometimes classified as different species.

Though genetics has made truly astounding and vital contributions toward biological anthropology by this work, it's important to acknowledge the equally pivotal role paleoanthropology continues to play in its tandem effort to flesh out humanity's roots. Paleoanthropologists like Alemsegad draw on every available source of information to both physically reconstruct hominin bodies and, perhaps more importantly, develop our understanding of how they may have lived, communicated, sustained themselves, and interacted with their environment and with each other. The work of Pääbo and others in his field offers powerful affirmations of paleoanthropological studies that have long investigated the contributions of Neanderthals and other hominins to the lineage of modern humans. Importantly, without paleoanthropology, the continued discovery and recovery of fossil specimens to later undergo genetic analysis would be greatly diminished.

Molecular anthropology and paleoanthropology, though often at odds with each other in the past regarding modern human evolution, now seem to be working together to chip away at theories that portray Neanderthals as inferior offshoots of humanity. Molecular anthropologists and paleoanthropologists also concur that that human evolution did not occur in ladder-like form, with one species leading to the next. Instead, the fossil evidence clearly reveals an evolutionary bush, with numerous hominin species existing at the same time and interacting through migration, some leading to modern humans and others going extinct.

Molecular anthropologist Spencer Wells uses DNA analysis to understand how our biological diversity correlates with ancient migration patterns from Africa into other continents. The study of our genetic evolution reveals that as humans migrated from Africa to all continents of the globe, they developed biological and cultural adaptations that allowed for survival in a variety of new environments. One example is skin color. Biological anthropologist Nina Jablonski uses satellite data to investigate the evolution of skin color, an aspect of human biological variation carrying tremendous social consequences. Jablonski underscores the importance of trying to understand skin color as a single trait affected by natural selection with its own evolutionary history and pressures, not as a tool to grouping humans into artificial races.

For Pääbo, Wells, Jablonski and others, technology affords the chance to investigate our origins in exciting new ways, adding pieces into the human puzzle at a record pace. At the same time, our technologies may well be changing who we are as a species and propelling us into an era of "neo-evolution."

Increasingly over time, human adaptations have been less related to predators, resources, or natural disasters, and more related to environmental and social pressures produced by other humans. Indeed, biological anthropologists have no choice but to consider the cultural components related to human evolutionary changes over time. Hominins have been constructing their own niches for a very long time, and when we make significant changes (such as agricultural subsistence), we must adapt to those changes. Classic examples of this include increases in sickle-cell anemia in new malarial environments, and greater lactose tolerance in regions with a long history of dairy farming.

Today we can, in some ways, evolve ourselves. We can enact biological change through genetic engineering, which operates at an astonishing pace in comparison to natural selection. Medical ethicist Harvey Fineberg calls this "neo-evolution". Fineberg goes beyond asking who we are as a species, to ask who we want to become and what genes we want our offspring to inherit. Depending on one's point of view, the future he envisions is both tantalizing and frightening: to some, it shows the promise of science to eradicate genetic abnormalities, while for others it raises the specter of eugenics. It's also worth remembering that while we may have the potential to influence certain genetic predispositions, changes in genotypes do not guarantee the desired results. Environmental and social pressures like pollution, nutrition or discrimination can trigger "epigenetic" changes which can turn genes on or off, or make them less or more active. This is important to factor in as we consider possible medical benefits from efforts in self-directed evolution. We must also ask: In an era of human-engineered, rapid-rate neo-evolution, who decides what the new human blueprints should be?

Technology figures in our evolutionary future in other ways as well. According to anthropologist Amber Case, many of our modern technologies are changing us into cyborgs: our smart phones, tablets and other tools are "exogenous components" that afford us astonishing and unsettling capabilities. They allow us to travel instantly through time and space and to create second, "digital selves" that represent our "analog selves" and interact with others in virtual environments. This has psychological implications for our analog selves that worry Case: a loss of mental reflection, the "ambient intimacy" of knowing that we can connect to anyone we want to at any time, and the "panic architecture" of managing endless information across multiple devices in virtual and real-world environments.

Despite her concerns, Case believes that our technological future is essentially positive. She suggests that at a fundamental level, much of this technology is focused on the basic concerns all humans share: who am I, where and how do I fit in, what do others think of me, who can I trust, who should I fear? Indeed, I would argue that we've evolved to be obsessed with what other humans are thinking—to be mind-readers in a sense—in a way that most would agree is uniquely human. For even though a baboon can assess those baboons it fears and those it can dominate, it cannot say something to a second baboon about a third baboon in order to trick that baboon into telling a fourth baboon to gang up on a fifth baboon. I think Facebook is a brilliant example of tapping into our evolved human psychology. We can have friends we've never met and let them know who we think we are—while we hope they like us and we try to assess what they're actually thinking and if they can be trusted. It's as if technology has provided an online supply of an addictive drug for a social mind evolved to crave that specific stimulant!

Yet our heightened concern for fairness in reciprocal relationships, in combination with our elevated sense of empathy and compassion, have led to something far greater than online chats: humanism itself. As Jane Goodall notes, chimps and baboons cannot rally together to save themselves from extinction; instead, they must rely on what she references as the "indomitable human spirit" to lessen harm done to the planet and all the living things that share it. As Goodall and other TED speakers in this course ask: will we use our highly evolved capabilities to secure a better future for ourselves and other species?

I hope those reading this essay, watching the TED Talks, and further exploring evolutionary perspectives on what makes us human, will view the continuities and discontinuities of our species as cause for celebration and less discrimination. Our social dependency and our prosocial need to identify ourselves, our friends, and our foes make us human. As a species, we clearly have major relationship problems, ranging from personal to global scales. Yet whenever we expand our levels of compassion and understanding, whenever we increase our feelings of empathy across cultural and even species boundaries, we benefit individually and as a species.

Get started

The search for humanity's roots

Zeresenay Alemseged

The search for humanity's roots, relevant talks.

We are all cyborgs now

We are all cyborgs now

Moral behavior in animals

Frans de Waal

Moral behavior in animals.

Are we ready for neo-evolution?

Harvey Fineberg

Are we ready for neo-evolution.

What separates us from chimpanzees?

Jane Goodall

What separates us from chimpanzees.

Skin color is an illusion

Nina Jablonski

Skin color is an illusion.

A family tree for humanity

Spencer Wells

A family tree for humanity.

DNA clues to our inner neanderthal

Svante Pääbo

Dna clues to our inner neanderthal.

IMAGES

  1. Human Biocultural and Social Evolution

    human biocultural and social evolution essay

  2. Lesson 3 Looking Back at Human Biocultural and Social Evolution

    human biocultural and social evolution essay

  3. Social Changes Evolution Free Essay Example

    human biocultural and social evolution essay

  4. Chapter-3-Human-Biocultural-and-Social-Evolution

    human biocultural and social evolution essay

  5. SOLUTION: Human biocultural and social evolution

    human biocultural and social evolution essay

  6. 2 Looking Back at the Human Biocultural and Social Evolution

    human biocultural and social evolution essay

VIDEO

  1. UCSP Week 4 Biocultural and Social Evolution

  2. Human Biocultural & Social Evolution Unlocking of terms

  3. UCSP Human Biocultural and Social Evolution Early Humans to the Rise of Civilization

  4. UCSPOL Mod 3: Lesson 1, Human Biocultural and Social Evolution

  5. What is Australopithecus Afarensis?

  6. IGCSE Biology 21

COMMENTS

  1. Human Evolution: Biological, Cultural, and Social

    Three processes—biological, cultural, and social evolution—are united by the overarching analytical framework of Darwinian thought. The chapter first surveys biological evolution in a theory that now balances genetic evolution of individual organisms from generation to generation with epigenetic development during the life course of each ...

  2. UCSP-Lesson 3: Looking back at Human Biocultural and Social Evolution

    Biological and cultural evolution: from Homo habilis (or earlier) to Homo sapiens sapiens in the fossil record lesson looking back at human biocultural and

  3. Cultural evolutionary theory: How culture evolves and why it matters

    Here, we review the core concepts in cultural evolutionary theory as they pertain to the extension of biology through culture, focusing on cultural evolutionary applications in population genetics, ecology, and demography. For each of these disciplines, we review the theoretical literature and highlight relevant empirical studies.

  4. The socio-cultural evolution of our species

    Many biologists and social scientists have noted that with the development of human culture, the biological evolution of Homo sapiens was usurped by socio-cultural evolution. The construction of artificial environments and social structures created new criteria for selection, and biological fitness was replaced by 'cultural fitness', which is often different for different cultures and is ...

  5. Human socio-cultural evolution in light of evolutionary transitions

    The recently proliferating domain of cultural evolution is a major key for understanding the unique human evolution. It is sometimes argued that the role of cultural evolution in humans currently bypasses genetic evolution and weakens genetic adaptive potential [ 12 ].

  6. PDF Biological, Cultural, and Social Evolution

    Biological, Cultural, and Social EvolutionBi. lution Unpublished essay, 2021PrologueSince the analysis of Darwin, the term "evolution" has mostly referred to the biological evolution of animal and plant species through natural selection and gen. tic change, as emphasized by E. O. Wilson. But recent research is now showing that two further ...

  7. Human Evolution: Biological, Cultural, and Social

    Three processes—biological, cultural, and social evolution—are united by the overarching analytical framework of Darwinian thought. The chapter first surveys biological evolution in a theory ...

  8. Human Biology: An Evolutionary and Biocultural Perspective

    The major points of this chapter are the following: (1) Human biology is a well-defined discipline. (2) Human biology is founded on an evolutionary perspective. (3) The recognition of different ...

  9. Human Nature and Biocultural Evolution

    Definition Human nature refers to psychological aspects and behavioral traits that are common to most people. Biocultural evolution is the process by which biological and cultural aspects interact to shape human nature throughout human evolutionary history.

  10. LOOKING BACK THROUGH BIO-CULTURAL AND SOCIAL EVOLUTION

    Human culture changes over time and varies across space. Two main approaches to study cultural evolution have developed in the last fifty years: human behavioural ecology and a suite of perspectives centred on the role of cultural transmission. The latter are often confusingly referred to with the name of the phenomenon they are trying to ...

  11. PDF Looking Back at the Human Bio-cultural and Social Evolution

    Write an editorial essay that assesses the important role of museums in educating the public about the human species' bio-cultural and social evolution. See the guidelines in making an editorial essay.

  12. Human life course biology: A centennial perspective of scholarship on

    the discovery that there exists a constant interplay between evolution with physical growth and development; the recognition of novel features of human growth and development, and several ways these may be organized into a continuum of ontogenetic events; evidence that human life course biology establishes the foundation for the capacity for human culture and biocultural reproduction; the ...

  13. Niche Construction Theory and Human Biocultural Evolution

    Abstract Biologists and anthropologists have extensively documented how many animals—human and non-human—modify their immediate surroundings, some subtly, others extensively. This trend is carried to its extreme in Homo sapiens to the point where many of us today live in the almost entirely constructed niches of the built urban environment. Proponents of niche construction theory (NCT ...

  14. Evolution: Biological, Social, Cultural

    The life cycle of a human being involves fixed stages (infancy, childhood, adolescence, adulthood, old age) that represent an unfolding of innate potentialities and that culminate in inevitable death. The evolution of a new biological species, by contrast, is a unique historical development involving movement in directions shaped primarily by environmental pressures, without any inevitable ...

  15. Human Nature and Biocultural Evolution

    First published in 1984, Human Nature and Biocultural Evolution aims to delineate a theory of human nature, viewed as an interrelated set of genetically programmed behavioral predispositions, and a theory of biocultural evolution. The author's approach is based on the hypothesis that innate predispositions and cultural-environmental factors cooperate to determine human behavior and socio ...

  16. Module 3: Human Biocultural and Social Evolution

    This document provides an overview of the module objectives and topics to be covered in a course on human biocultural and social evolution. The module aims to help students understand themselves as members of society with traditions developed over 2000 years. It will explore the biological and cultural evolution of early humans, examine archaeological evidence of cultural processes, and ...

  17. UCSP: Human Bio-cultural and Social Evolution

    Human Bio-Cultural and Social Evolution Presented By: Mark Kristopher Ruiz STEM 11-7 What is Biocultural Evolution? Biocultural Evolution The mutual, interactive evolution of human biology and culture; the concept that biology makes culture possible and that developing culture

  18. Introductory essay

    Introductory essay. Written by the educator who created What Makes Us Human?, a brief look at the key facts, tough questions and big ideas in his field. Begin this TED Study with a fascinating read that gives context and clarity to the material. As a biological anthropologist, I never liked drawing sharp distinctions between human and non-human.

  19. Humans are biocultural, science should be too

    Humans are born into a world of social and physical ecologies, patterns, institutions, and ideologies that become inextricably entangled with our biology, even before we leave the womb. Basic perceptions such as smell and color, for example, are mutually shaped by physiology and cultural experience. The biocultural approach—a paradigm in ...

  20. A critical perspective on the concept of biocultural diversity and its

    Biocultural diversity, defined as the total variety exhibited by the world's natural and cultural systems, denotes three concepts: diversity of life includes human cultures and languages; links exist between biodiversity and cultural diversity; and these links have developed over time through mutual adaptation and possibly co-evolution.

  21. UCSP L03 Human Biocultural Social Evolution

    This video lecture was created for the SHS Core Subject Understanding Culture, Society and Politics.

  22. Looking Back at Human Biocultural and Social Evolution

    The document traces the biological and cultural evolution of humans from early hominids to modern Homo sapiens. It discusses key species in human biological evolution such as Australopithecus, Homo habilis, Homo erectus, and Homo sapiens. It also examines major developments in cultural evolution like the use of stone tools, control of fire, and the Neolithic Revolution involving the beginnings ...

  23. SHS UCSP: Lesson 3 Looking back at Human biocultural and social

    SHS UCSP: Lesson 3 Looking back at Human biocultural and social evolution part 1 Eman Stories 10.3K subscribers Subscribed 215 12K views 3 years ago #ucsp